How to go to your page This eBook contains three volumes. Each volume has its own page numbering scheme, consisting of ...
161 downloads
3133 Views
3MB Size
Report
This content was uploaded by our users and we assume good faith they have the permission to share this book. If you own the copyright to this book and it is wrongfully on our website, we offer a simple DMCA procedure to remove your content from our site. Start by pressing the button below!
Report copyright / DMCA form
How to go to your page This eBook contains three volumes. Each volume has its own page numbering scheme, consisting of a volume number and a page number, separated by a colon. For example, to go to page 5 of Volume 1, type 1:5 in the "page #" box at the top of the screen and click "Go." To go to page 5 of Volume 2, type 2:5… and so forth.
DEMENTIA
Dementia Volume 1: History and Incidence Volume 2: Science and Biology Volume 3: Treatments and Developments
DEMENTIA Volume 1: History and Incidence Patrick McNamara, Editor
Brain, Behavior, and Evolution Patrick McNamara, Series Editor
Copyright 2011 ABC-CLIO, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior permission in writing from the publisher. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Dementia / Patrick McNamara, editor. p. cm.—(Brain, behavior, and evolution) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-0-313-38434-9 (hard copy : alk. paper)—ISBN 978-0-313-38435-6 (ebook) 1. Dementia. 2. Alzheimer ’s disease. I. McNamara, Patrick, 1956– II. Series: Brain, behavior, and evolution [DNLM: 1. Dementia. WM 220] RC521.D4524 2011 616.8’3—dc22 2010041082 ISBN 978-0-313-38434-9 EISBN 978-0-313-38435-6 15
14
13
12
11
1
2
3
4
5
This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook. Visit www.abc-clio.com for details. Praeger An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC ABC-CLIO, LLC 130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911 Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911 This book is printed on acid-free paper Manufactured in the United States of America
Contents
Series Foreword Preface: Hopeful Trends in Meeting the Challenge of the Dementias Patrick McNamara
vii ix
Volume 1: History and Incidence Chapter 1. Epidemiology of the Dementias Chengxuan Qiu and Laura Fratiglioni Chapter 2. Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia Wilm Quentin, Steffi G. Riedel-Heller, Melanie Luppa, Hanna Leicht, and Hans-Helmut König Chapter 3. A Stroke of Bad Luck: CADASIL and Friedrich Nietzsche’s “Dementia” or Madness Paul M. Butler Chapter 4. Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia Laura E. Middleton Chapter 5. Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle: Implications for Healthy Brain Aging and Dementia Prevention Michael J. Valenzuela Chapter 6. Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia Art Walaszek
1
35
57 75
99
123
vi
Contents
Chapter 7. Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological and Functional Assessment: Contributions to Early Detection of Dementia Mônica Sanches Yassuda, Mariana Kneese Flaks, and Fernanda Speggiorin Pereira Chapter 8. Does Poor Sleep Quality in Late Life Compromise Cognition and Accelerate Progression of the Degenerative Dementias? Peter Engel Chapter 9. Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy: A Tool for Understanding Brain Chemical Changes in Dementias Jacquelynn N. Copeland and H. Randall Griffith
151
177
201
About the Contributors
229
About the Series Editor
235
Index
237
Series Foreword
Beginning in the 1990s, behavioral scientists—that is, people who study mind, brain, and behavior—began to take the theory of evolution seriously. They began to borrow techniques developed by the evolutionary biologists and apply them to problems in mind, brain, and behavior. Now, of course, virtually all behavioral scientists up to that time had claimed to endorse evolutionary theory, but few used it to study the problems they were interested in. All that changed in the 1990s. Since that pivotal decade, breakthroughs in the behavioral and brain sciences have been constant, rapid, and unremitting. The purpose of the Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series of titles published by ABC-CLIO is to bring these new breakthroughs in the behavioral sciences to the attention of the general public. In the past decade, some of these scientific breakthroughs have come to inform the clinical and biomedical disciplines. That means that people suffering from all kinds of diseases and disorders, particularly brain and behavioral disorders, will benefit from these new therapies. That is exciting news indeed, and the general public needs to learn about these breakthrough findings and treatments. A whole new field called evolutionary medicine has begun to transform the way medicine is practiced and has led to new treatments and new approaches to diseases, like the dementias, sleep disorders, psychiatric diseases, and developmental disorders that seemed intractable to previous efforts. The series of books in the Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series seeks both to contribute to this new evolutionary approach to brain and behavior and to bring the insights emerging from the new evolutionary approaches to psychology, medicine, and anthropology to the general public. The Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series was inspired by and brought to fruition with the help of Debora Carvalko at ABC-CLIO. The series editor,
viii
Series Foreword
Dr. Patrick McNamara, is the director of the Evolutionary Neurobehavior Laboratory in the Department of Neurology at Boston University School of Medicine. He has devoted most of his scientific work to development of an evolutionary approach to problems of sleep medicine and to neurodegenerative diseases. Titles in the series will focus on applied and clinical implications of evolutionary approaches to the whole range of brain and behavioral disorders. Contributions are solicited from leading figures in the fields of interest to the series. Each volume will cover the basics, define the terms, and analyze the full range of issues and findings relevant to the clinical disorder or topic that is the focus of the volume. Each volume will demonstrate how the application of evolutionary modes of analysis leads to new insights on causes of disorder and functional breakdowns in brain and behavior relationships. Each volume, furthermore, will be aimed at both popular and professional audiences and will be written in a style appropriate for the general reader, the local and university libraries, and graduate and undergraduate students. The publications that become part of this series will therefore bring the gold discovered by scientists using evolutionary methods to understand brain and behavior to the attention of the general public, and ultimately, it is hoped, to those families and individuals currently suffering from those most intractable of disorders— the brain and behavioral disorders.
Preface: Hopeful Trends in Meeting the Challenge of the Dementias Patrick McNamara
It is estimated that 24.3 million people around the world have dementia and that, with an estimated 4.6 million new cases every year, we can expect about 43 million people and their families to face the challenge of dementia by 2020. There are several forms of dementia, with the most common being Alzheimer ’s disease (40% of cases), vascular dementia with or without Alzheimer features (25%), and dementia with Lewy bodies (25%), the latter being related to the increasingly important form of dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. The annual healthcare costs for Alzheimer ’s disease alone is estimated at about $155 billion in the United States. A substantial portion of these costs is due to behavioral and neuropsychiatric disturbances associated with the dementing process— yet these neuropsychiatric and behavioral problems have only recently become the focus of study and treatment in the biomedical communities. The successes of neuropsychiatric approaches to the dementias is measured in reduced suffering for patients and their families and reduced healthcare costs for the system as a whole. The authors of the chapters in these three volumes, devoted to emerging trends in dementia studies, have virtually all emphasized identification, study, and treatment of behavioral and neuropsychiatric problems of patients and their families. The reason they have done so is the dawning realization in both the biomedical and caregiving communities that targeting behavioral and neuropsychiatric problems of dementia leads to some pretty effective scientific studies of mechanisms and very effective and low-cost treatment programs that act to alleviate both patients’ suffering and caregivers’ burdens.
x
Preface
Although the standard, it has long been established that dementia most commonly occurs in older people, and that primary symptoms are memory impairment (both short- and long-term), deficits in executive functions, and impairments of abstract thinking and judgment. It has now become crystal clear that some of the best and earliest predictors of dementia risk are mood and personality changes, which all too often are misdiagnosed as depression or some other common mood disorder. Family members may express concern to a primary care physician, but these concerns too often get ignored or shunted aside as a standard mood disorder. It is vitally important to take reports of significant behavioral changes seriously as identification of cognitive components of a dementing process—may be a later-occurring symptom than the behavioral changes. Although the three-step diagnostic process (single question about memory, MMSE, neuropsychological testing) has high positive predictive value, it only detects 18% of future dementia cases. It is the behavioral and neuropsychiatric disturbances, along with incipient cognitive changes, that may yield better detection rates for dementia. Tremendous progress has been made in identification of biomarkers for dementia. The use of functional imaging, proteomic, genetic, biochemical and electrophysiological markers, including sleep polysomnographic techniques, has meant that our ability to detect dementia early on has vastly improved. In addition, the new appreciation of the importance of behavioral and psychiatric problems in dementia as well as validated assessment tools to measure these behavioral problems suggests that it is time to deploy all these new techniques to identify those at risk for dementia so as to prevent or to slow onset of the disorder in these individuals. What is needed are large-scale, multisite, comparative studies that can evaluate optimal use and validity of these various techniques for detecting and selecting asymptomatic people at risk for dementia. The recent Leon Thal Symposium 2009 in Las Vegas, Nevada, explored algorithms, biomarkers, and assessment tools for identifying asymptomatic individuals at elevated risk for dementia. The consensus recommendations of symposium participants included: 1. Establishment of a National Database for Longitudinal Studies as a shared research core resource; 2. Launch of a large collaborative study that will compare multiple screening approaches and biomarkers to determine the best method for identifying asymptomatic people at risk;
Preface
xi
3. Initiation of a Global Database that extends the concept of the National Database for Longitudinal Studies for longitudinal studies beyond the United States; and 4. Development of an educational campaign that will promote healthy brain aging. (Khachaturian et al. 2010) These are all laudable recommendations. But behavioral and neuropsychiatric assessment tools must be included in these large multisite studies of at-risk individuals. A perusal of the essays in these volumes (volume 1 focuses on epidemiologic, descriptive, historical, and diagnostic innovations in dementia; volume 2 focuses on biobehavioral mechanisms of dementia; and volume 3 focuses on emerging treatment strategies including treatments for behavioral problems of dementia) leaves one with a sense of hope and confidence that the daunting challenges of the dementias, both for patients and for families, are finally being effectively addressed. REFERENCE Khachaturian, Z. S., D. Barnes, R. Einstein, et al. 2010. Developing a national strategy to prevent dementia: Leon Thal Symposium 2009. Alzheimer’s and Dementia 6 (2): 89–97.
Chapter 1
Epidemiology of the Dementias Chengxuan Qiu and Laura Fratiglioni
Dementia is defined as a clinical syndrome characterized by progressive deterioration in multiple cognitive domains which is severe enough to interfere with daily functioning. Epidemiology deals with the distribution, determinants, and prevention of a disease in the population. Since the 1980s, numerous community-based prospective studies of aging and health have been implemented in the world; many of which have focused on dementia and its main subtypes of Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) and vascular dementia (VaD). These studies have significantly contributed to the understanding of epidemiology of the dementias, including occurrence, determinants, and prevention. In this chapter, we review the literature of epidemiological research in the dementias by focusing on the most recent studies. OCCURRENCE The occurrence of a disease can be measured as the proportion of people affected by the disease in a defined population at a specific time point (prevalence), or as the number of new cases that occur during a specific time period in a population at risk for developing that disease (incidence). The prevalence reflects the public health burden of the disease, whereas the incidence indicates the risk of developing that disease. The prevalence is determined by both incidence and duration of the disease, and in certain circumstances the prevalence may be estimated as incidence × average disease duration.
2
Dementia
Prevalence In a consensus report in 2005, it was estimated that more than 25 million people in the world were affected by dementia, most suffering from AD, with around 5 million new cases occurring every year (Ferri et al. 2005). As the population ages, the number of patients with dementia is anticipated to double every 20 years. In Europe, the number of dementia cases has reached more than 6 million in 2010; this number is projected to be 14 million in 2050 (Mura et al. 2010). In the United States, there were 4.5 million AD patients in the year 2000; the number is projected to reach 13.2 million by 2050 (Hebert et al. 2003). In the Asia Pacific region, the number of dementia cases will increase from 13.7 million in 2005 to 64.6 million by 2050 (Access Economics 2006). The global prevalence of dementia was estimated to be 3.9 percent in people aged 60+ years, with the regional prevalence being 1.6 percent in Africa, 4.0 percent in China and the western Pacific region, 4.6 percent in Latin America, 5.4 percent in Western Europe, and 6.4 percent in North America (Ferri et al. 2005). Figure 1.1 shows the age-specific prevalence of dementia across different regions. The prevalence of dementia is very low in persons under 60; after age 65, the rate doubles almost every five years until very old ages; nearly half of the oldest old (i.e., 90 years and older) become demented (von Strauss et al. 1999; Corrada et al. 2008). Thus, the overall prevalence and burden of the dementias depend largely on age structure of the population. The prevalence of dementia appears to vary by regions across the world, but this may be due to variation in age structure of the populations, diagnostic accuracy, and disease duration or survival. In Europe, the pooling data suggest that the age-standardized prevalence in people aged 65 years or older is 6.4 percent for dementia and 4.4 percent for AD (Lobo et al. 2000). A systematic review of studies from developing countries reported that the overall prevalence in people aged 65 years or over was 5.3 percent for dementia and 3.4 percent for AD (Kalaria et al. 2008). The 10/66 Dementia Research Group found that the prevalence of dementia (DSM-IV criteria) in people aged 65+ years in seven developing nations varied widely from less than 0.5 percent to more than 6 percent (Llibre Rodriguez et al. 2008). The prevalence of dementia in India and Sub-Saharan Africa was about half of other regions (Ferri et al. 2005). The proportion of subtype dementias also differs across continents. In Europe and North America, AD and VaD account for up to 70 percent and 20–30 percent, respectively, of all dementia cases (Lobo et al. 2000), whereas earlier studies from Asia showed a relatively high proportion for VaD (Chiu and Zhang 2000; Ikeda et al. 2001). The difference may be due
Epidemiology of the Dementias
3
Figure 1.1
Age-specific prevalence rates of dementia (per 100 population) across the world. (Lobo et al. 2000; Brayne 2006; Dong et al. 2007; Lopes et al. 2007; Nitrini et al. 2009).
to variations in diagnostic criteria, ascertainment procedure of the cases, selective survival, and geographical distribution of vascular diseases such as stroke (Matthews and Brayne 2005). Indeed, the large-scale communitybased surveys and meta-analysis in Asian countries have yielded the proportion of major dementia subtypes (e.g., AD and VaD) largely comparable with those in Western countries (Zhang et al. 2005; Kalaria et al. 2008). In addition, population-based neuropathological studies reveal that dementia often occurs with concomitant AD pathologies and cerebrovascular lesions (Schneider et al. 2007). Incidence Over the past decades, many incidence studies of dementia have become available; the majority of which are conducted among developed
4
Dementia
nations (Fratiglioni et al. 2008; Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2009). Pooling data in Europe suggested that the overall incidence of dementia in people aged 65 years or older was 19.4 per 1,000 person-years (Fratiglioni, Launer, et al. 2000), and the incidence was 13.6 per 1,000 person-years in Brazil (Kalaria et al. 2008). Figure 1.2 shows the age-specific incidence of dementia across the world. The incidence rates of dementia increase steeply with advancing age. In Europe, approximately two per 1,000 person-years become demented among people aged 65–69 years, and the incidence increases to 70 to 80 per 1,000 person-years for people 90 years or over (Fratiglioni, Launer, et al. 2000). The age-specific pattern of incidence for AD is similar to that of all-cause dementia, but the age-specific pattern for VaD is less stable. It remains debatable regarding whether the incidence of dementia continues to increase even in the oldest-old or reaches a plateau at a certain age. The Cache County Study found that the incidence of dementia increased with age, peaked, and then started to decline at extreme old ages for both men and women (Miech et al. 2002). But some meta-analyses and large-scale studies in Europe provided no evidence for the potential decline in the incidence of dementia among the oldest old (Fratiglioni et al. 2008; Matthews and Brayne 2005). The apparent decline seen in some studies may be an artifact due to poor response rate and survival effect in the very old. Several studies in Europe observed a higher incidence rate of dementia and AD in women than in men, especially among the oldest-old (Fratiglioni, Launer, et al. 2000), whereas studies in North America found no gender difference (Kawas et al. 2000; Kukull et al. 2002). Mortality and Case-Fatality Dementia is one of the leading causes of death in older people. However, death certificates grossly underreport its cause (Jin et al. 2004), even when multiple underlying causes of death are taken into account (Ganguli and Rodriguez 1999). The community-based follow-up studies could provide reliable data on mortality. In the Swedish Kungsholmen Project of people aged 75 years or over, the mortality rate of dementia was 2.4 per 100 person-years; 70 percent of incident dementia cases died within five years following the diagnosis (Agüero-Torres et al. 1999). A follow-up study of nursing home residents with advanced dementia suggested that 55 percent of the patients died over eighteen months; pneumonia, febrile episodes, and eating problems are the most frequent complications that significantly contribute to the six-month mortality (Mitchell et al. 2009). Several community-based studies have shown that dementia increases the risk of death by two to five times (Agüero-Torres et al. 1999; Jagger
Epidemiology of the Dementias
5
Figure 1.2
Age-specific incidence rates of dementia (per 1,000 person-years) across the world. (Jorm and Jolley 1998; Fratiglioni, Launer, et al. 2000; Canadian Study of Health and Aging Working Group 2000; Kukull et al. 2002; Matthews and Brayne 2005).
et al. 2000; Wolfson et al. 2001), supporting the malignancy of dementia. The median survival time of patients with dementia ranges from two to five years after the diagnosis depending on demographic features and comorbidity (Helzner et al. 2008; Xie et al. 2008). After the age of 85, although dementia still shortens life expectancy, the extent is somehow less than in younger-old people (Tschanz et al. 2004). Older age, male gender, low education, morbidities, and functional disability contribute to a shorter survival in patients with dementia (Helmer et al. 2001; Xie et al. 2008). DETERMINANTS The dementias are multifactorial disorders that are determined by genetic and environmental factors as well as their interactions.
6
Dementia
Population-based prospective study is the major epidemiological approach to identifying influential factors for chronic multifactorial diseases such as dementia, in which the life-course approach should be taken into consideration (Whalley et al. 2006). Age is the most powerful determinant of dementia, and gene mutations contribute to a small proportion of all cases. Two groups of modifiable factors for late-life dementias have been established, that is, the “vascular risk factors” that have been strongly associated with an increased risk of dementia, and the “psychosocial factors” that may contribute to the delay of dementia onset. Evidence supporting these etiological profiles has been fully summarized elsewhere (Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2009; Qiu, Xu, and Fratiglioni 2010), which also provide a complete list of references when not otherwise specified herein.
Gene Mutations and Genetic Risk Factors Mutations in amyloid precursor protein, presenilin-1, and presenilin-2 genes can cause early-onset familial AD that accounts for no more than 5 percent of all cases (Blennow et al. 2006). The majority of AD cases are sporadic, with considerable heterogeneity in their risk profiles and neuropathological features. First-degree relatives of AD patients have a higher lifetime risk for developing AD than relatives of nondemented people or the general population (Green et al. 2002; Seshadri and Wolf 2007). It is likely that shared genetic and environmental factors contribute to the familial aggregation; twin studies can address this issue. The Swedish Twin Study estimated that the heritability of AD ranged from 0.58 to 0.74, with other variance being attributable to environmental factors (Gatz et al. 2006). However, familial aggregation of AD can only be partially explained by known genetic factors such as APOE ε4 allele, indicating that other susceptibility genes may be involved in AD (Huang et al. 2004; Hayden et al. 2009). The APOE ε4 allele is the only established genetic risk factor for both early and late-onset AD; it is a susceptibility gene, being neither necessary nor sufficient for the development of AD. The risk of AD increases with increasing number of the ε4 alleles in a dose-dependent manner (Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2004), but the risk effect decreases with increasing age. Overall, approximately 15 to 20 percent of AD cases are attributable to the APOE ε4 allele (Slooter et al. 1998; Qiu et al. 2004). Other candidate genes for AD, such as angiotensin-I converting enzyme gene, cholesterol 24-hydroxylase gene, and insulin degrading enzyme gene, remains to be clearly identified (Bertram et al. 2007).
Epidemiology of the Dementias
7
Vascular Risk Factors A number of vascular risk factors and disorders have been linked to dementia and AD as well, but some factors have a differential association with the risk of dementia depending on the age when the exposure is assessed. The age-dependent association is likely due to the pathophysiological and metabolic changes with age in vascular factors such as blood pressure, body mass index (BMI), and total serum cholesterol. High Blood Pressure An association of elevated blood pressure in midlife with an increased risk of dementia and AD later in life has been reported in several population-based studies (Qiu et al. 2005; Alonso et al. 2009); such an association was particularly evident for blood pressure levels of 160/95 mm Hg or higher, and for people who had high blood pressure but who were not treated with blood pressure–lowering drugs. It is plausible that longterm hypertension can be linked to the dementias by causing cerebral atherosclerosis and microvascular lesions (e.g., white-matter lesions, silent infarcts, and microbleeds). Furthermore, the postmortem and neuroimaging studies have directly linked midlife high blood pressure to the brain pathologies and imaging markers of AD such as neuritic plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, and more severe atrophy of the hippocampus (Korf et al. 2004; Launer et al. 2008). Follow-up studies of late-life blood pressure and risk of dementia yield mixed results, largely depending on the length of follow-up. The shortterm follow-up studies (e.g., less than 3 years) often found no association or even an inverse association between blood pressure and risk of dementia and AD (Qiu et al. 2005). Because dementia has a long latent period and blood pressure may start to decline a few years before the onset of the dementia syndrome due to ongoing brain aging and degenerative process, the inverse or lack of association has been interpreted as a consequence of the disease. However, studies of very old people (e.g., 75 years or older) with a longer follow-up period (e.g., more than 6 years) also revealed an increased risk of dementia associated with low blood pressure (Qiu, Winblad, et al. 2009), suggesting that among very old people low blood pressure may also contribute to the development of dementia, possibly by influencing cerebral blood perfusion. Use of antihypertensive drugs has been associated with a decreased incidence of dementia and AD in several observational studies (Qiu et al. 2005); recent studies further suggest that the beneficial effect is
8
Dementia
more evident for young-old people, for angiotensin receptor blockers, and for long-term treatment (Peila et al. 2006; Haag et al. 2009; Li et al. 2010). Neuropathological data also showed fewer neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in medicated hypertensives than nonhypertensive groups, suggesting a possible effect of antihypertensive therapy against AD pathologies (Hoffman et al. 2009). However, systematic review and meta-analysis of major randomized controlled clinical trials conducted in hypertensive individuals have only shown a marginal beneficial effect of antihypertensive therapy against the dementias (Peters, Beckett, et al. 2008; McGuinness, Todd, et al. 2009). Diabetes Mellitus Diabetes has often been associated with VaD, but the association with AD is also reported in systematic reviews (Biessels et al. 2006; Lu et al. 2009; Kopf and Frölich 2009). Pooled analysis of eight follow-up studies has shown that diabetes is associated with a nearly 50 percent increased risk of dementia independent of cardiovascular factors and comorbidities (Lu et al. 2009). Neuropathological data from the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study indicated that diabetes, especially diabetes in combination with APOE ε4 allele, was associated with a substantially increased risk of dementia and a heavier burden of Alzheimer pathologies (Peila et al. 2002), although other studies remain uncertain whether diabetes is associated with AD pathologies (Arvanitakis et al. 2006; Sonnen et al. 2009). Furthermore, long-term follow-up studies show that midlife diabetes is more strongly associated with an elevated risk of dementia (Alonso et al. 2009; Xu et al. 2009), suggesting that long duration and more severe diabetes play a crucial role in determining the disease risk. In addition, a higher HbA1c is associated with lower cognitive function in individuals with diabetes (CukiermanYaffe et al. 2009), whereas a history of severe hypoglycemic episodes is associated with a greater risk of dementia (Whitmer et al. 2009). Finally, pre-diabetes, impaired glucose regulation, and impaired insulin secretion have also been associated with dementia (Xu et al. 2007). The association of diabetes with dementia and AD is likely due to the convergent effects of multiple pathological processes that include cerebral macrovascular and microvascular injury, chronic hyperglycemia, insulin resistance, advanced glycation end products, oxidative stress, and inflammation (Craft 2009). Cerebrovascular Lesions and Cardiovascular Disease Systematic reviews of population-based studies reveal an approximately two- to four-fold increased risk of incident dementia associated
Epidemiology of the Dementias
9
with clinical stroke (post-stroke dementia) (Pendlebury and Rothwell 2009; Savva and Stephan 2010). It is conceivable that an association of clinical stroke with AD is rarely reported due to the fact that a history of stroke is part of the current criteria for excluding the diagnosis of AD. However, asymptomatic cerebrovascular lesions such as silent brain infarcts and white-matter lesions have been associated with an increased risk of dementia and AD (Vermeer et al. 2003; Troncoso et al. 2008), although the association with AD is likely to be due to the inclusion of mixed dementia cases. In addition, a case-control study found that spontaneous cerebral emboli were associated with an increased odds ratio of AD and VaD (Purandare et al. 2006). The Cardiovascular Health Study (CHS) found that cardiovascular disease was associated with an increased incidence of dementia, with the highest risk seen among people with peripheral arterial disease, suggesting that extensive peripheral atherosclerosis is a risk factor for dementia (Newman et al. 2005). Atrial fibrillation, heart failure, and severe atherosclerosis measured with ankle-to-brachial index are also associated with the increased risk of dementia and AD (Ott et al. 1997; Qiu et al. 2006; van Oijen et al. 2007; Laurin et al. 2007). Neuropathological data show that cerebrovascular lesions and AD pathologies often coexist in patients with dementia, suggesting that these lesions may be the results of coinciding processes converging to cause additive brain damage and promote clinical manifestation of the dementia syndrome. Body Mass Index A lifespan-dependent relationship between BMI and risk of the dementias has emerged in which a higher BMI in midlife is related to an elevated risk of dementia and AD later in life, whereas an accelerated decline in BMI during late life may anticipate the onset of dementia (Gustafson 2006). The CHS showed that obesity at midlife was related to a higher risk of latelife dementia, whereas BMI measured after age 65 years was inversely related to dementia risk (Fitzpatrick et al. 2009). The long-term follow-up studies found a gradual decline in BMI over the years preceding dementia onset (Stewart et al. 2005; Hassing et al. 2009), which is supported by several follow-up studies of older people that show an association of low BMI and decline in BMI with subsequent development of dementia and AD (Atti et al. 2008; Beydoun, Lhotsky, et al. 2008). A meta-analysis of cohort studies suggested an increased risk of dementia for being underweight (pooled relative risk [RR], 1.36; 95 percent confidence interval [CI], 1.07–1.73) and obesity (RR, 1.42; 95 percent CI, 0.93–2.18); the association of increased dementia risk with obesity was stronger in studies with a longer follow-up period (e.g., more than 10 years) and younger age at BMI
10
Dementia
measurement (Rosengren et al. 2005; Beydoun, Beydoun, and Wang 2008). Thus, while obesity in midlife is a risk factor for late-life dementia, late-life low BMI and weight loss can be interpreted as markers for the predrome of dementia. Overweight or obesity in midlife may increase dementia risk through its close association with hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes, and other vascular diseases. High Serum Cholesterol As with blood pressure and BMI, an age-dependent association with the risk of dementia is also suggested for serum cholesterol, such that high total cholesterol at midlife is more consistently associated with an increased risk of dementia diagnosed more than 20–30 years later, whereas no or an inverse association between total cholesterol and the risk of dementia is often reported in cohort studies of older people. Interestingly, such a pattern of association was confirmed with all-cause dementia and AD, but not with VaD, in a meta-analysis and a follow-up study of middle aged cohort (Anstey et al. 2008; Solomon et al. 2009). Long-term follow-up studies have shown that total cholesterol levels begin to decline more than a decade before the onset of dementia (Stewart et al. 2007). This implies that decreasing total cholesterol after middle age and a lower cholesterol level in late life may reflect ongoing disease processes and thus could be a marker for future development of dementia (Solomon et al. 2007). Cross-sectional studies suggest a lower likelihood of dementia associated with the use of statins, but this could be due to different prescribing patterns by physicians for people with and without dementia, such that dementia patients were less likely to be prescribed with lipid-lowering drugs than nondemented people (Rodriguez et al. 2002). Several follow-up studies show no beneficial effect of statin therapy or only a modestly decreased risk of dementia (Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2009). Neuropathological studies also show inconsistent results whether use of statins is associated with a reduced burden of Alzheimer pathological markers and infarcts in the brain (Li et al. 2007; Arvanitakis, Schneider, et al. 2008). A systematic review of randomized controlled trials concludes that statins given in late life to individuals at risk of vascular disease have no effect in preventing dementia (McGuinness, Craig, et al. 2009). Nutrients and Dietary Factors Several follow-up studies have reported a decreased risk of AD and dementia associated with increasing dietary or supplementary intake
Epidemiology of the Dementias
11
of antioxidants (e.g., vitamins E and C) (Barberger-Gateau et al. 2007), although some negative results are also reported (Gray et al. 2008). Similarly, studies also showed mixed results on the association of serum vitamin B12 and folate with the risk of dementia and AD (Luchsinger and Mayeux 2004). The Cochrane review of eight randomized clinical trials concludes that supplementations of folic acid and vitamin B12 have no benefits on cognition in healthy or cognitively impaired older people, although they are effective in reducing serum homocysteine levels (Malouf and Evans 2008). A higher adherence to “Mediterranean diet” (i.e., a dietary pattern with higher intake of fish, fruits, and vegetables rich in antioxidants) has been associated with a reduced risk of dementia independent of vascular factors and physical activity in some studies (Scarmeas et al. 2006, 2009), but not in the French Three-City Study (Féart et al. 2009). A diet rich in high polyunsaturated and fish-related fats is known to be associated with a lower risk of vascular disease; thus, it is plausible to extend the beneficial effects to dementia. In support of this hypothesis, a systematic review suggested that a high dietary intake of fish and omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) was associated with a decreased risk of cognitive decline (Fotuhi et al. 2009). However, this review also found that only four out of eight observational studies suggested a reduced risk of dementia and AD associated with consumption of fish and PUFAs independent of multiple potential confounders, and in two studies the protective effect disappeared after controlling for confounders such as demographics and income. Furthermore, two recent studies added no additional evidence for a possible role of high consumption of fish and PUFAs in reducing the risk of dementia and AD (Kröger et al. 2009; Devore et al. 2009). Finally, randomized clinical trials have failed to show any beneficial role for the use of PUFAs in the treatment and secondary prevention of dementia among elderly people (Fotuhi et al. 2009). High Serum Homocysteine Elevated total homocysteine (tHcy) is associated with an increased risk of cardiac and cerebrovascular disease and thus may increase dementia risk. The follow-up study of the Framingham cohort of older residents reported a nearly double-increased risk of AD and dementia associated with increase of one standard deviation in tHcy levels (Seshadri et al. 2002). A meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies revealed that hyperhomocysteine was associated with a pooled RR of 2.5 (95 percent CI, 1.4– 4.6) for AD (van Dam and van Gool 2009). Despite the association, the
12
Dementia
beneficial effect of reducing serum homocysteine levels by supplementing vitamin B12 and folate on cognitive function remains to be established. Neuroimaging study suggested that higher plasma tHcy levels are associated with smaller brain volume and silent brain infarcts, even in healthy middle-aged adults, suggesting that both neurodegenerative and vascular mechanisms may underlie the association of tHcy with brain aging and dementia (Seshadri et al. 2008). Inflammation Inflammation is known to play a pivotal role in the pathogenesis of atherosclerosis. A higher level of serum C-reactive protein (CRP) in midlife was linked to an increased risk of AD and VaD, suggesting that inflammatory markers may reflect both peripheral and cerebral vascular mechanisms related to dementia, and the process can be a measurable long time before the dementia syndrome is manifested (Schmidt et al. 2002). Follow-up studies of older adults also showed an association between high levels of serum inflammatory markers (e.g., CRP and interleukin-1,6) and an increased incidence of dementia and AD (Engelhart et al. 2004; Tan et al. 2007). In addition, the systematic review of observational studies confirms that long-term use of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) (e.g., more than 2 years) is associated with a decreased risk of AD and dementia (Etminan et al. 2003), which provides additional evidence supporting the involvement of inflammation in AD and dementia. Thus, it seems plausible to hypothesize that inflammatory mechanisms play a part in the neurodegenerative process. However, neuropathological studies found no evidence for an association between use of NSAIDs and the reduced burden of AD pathologies (Arvanitakis, Grodstein, et al. 2008). Furthermore, the clinical trial of celecoxib or naproxen in AD prevention failed to show any beneficial effect of these drugs against AD; instead, an increased risk of AD related to drug therapy was observed (Martin et al. 2008). Smoking Follow-up studies have frequently shown an increased risk of dementia and AD associated with cigarette smoking, although the association may vary by APOE ε4 allele status (Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2009; Alonso et al. 2009). Meta-analyses of follow-up studies indicate that current smoking, compared to never smoking, is associated with an increased risk for dementia, especially for AD, but the increased risk for VaD seems less
Epidemiology of the Dementias
13
evident (Anstey et al. 2007; Peters, Poulter, et al. 2008). Neuropathological data show that the number of neuritic plaques is increased with increasing amount of cigarette smoking (Tyas et al. 2003). Thus, in contrast to the protective effect initially suggested in earlier cross-sectional and casecontrol studies, prospective cohort studies have actually provided convincing evidence that cigarette smoking, even long-term secondhand smoking (Barnes et al. 2010), is a risk factor for dementia and AD. Smoking is known to cause damage to the vascular system, but it remains unclear whether and how smoking can lead to Alzheimer pathologies. Alcohol Consumption Alcohol abuse may cause “alcoholic” dementia. A population-based study found that heavier alcohol drinkers at middle age had more than a three-fold increased risk of developing dementia later in life, especially among the carriers of APOE ε4 allele (Anttila et al. 2004). By contrast, epidemiological studies often reported a reduced incidence of dementia and AD associated with light-to-moderate alcohol intake (e.g., 1–3 drinks per day) (Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2009), leading to the hypothesis that light-tomoderate alcohol consumption may protect against dementia and cognitive decline. Two systematic reviews of prospective studies showed that light-to-moderate alcohol drinkers had an approximately 30–40 percent reduced risk of AD and dementia (Peters, Peters, et al. 2008; Anstey et al. 2009). However, a neuroimaging study did not support any protective effect of moderate alcohol consumption on brain aging (Paul et al. 2008). Moreover, the apparent cognitive benefits of light-to-moderate alcohol intake could be due to potential biases that result from methodological limitations of the observational studies such as information bias, confounding of socioeconomic status and healthy lifestyles, and inconsistent approaches of alcohol assessments. Clustering of Vascular Factors and Disorders Vascular risk factors and related disorders often coexist among elderly people. Several studies have consistently shown that the risk of dementia increases with an increasing burden of vascular factors (Whitmer et al. 2005; Qiu, Xu, Winblad, et al. 2010). In addition, the risk indices at both middle age and late life provide a reasonable estimation for the probability of future development of dementia, in which a cluster of multiple cardiovascular risk factors plays a relevant role (Kivipelto et al. 2006; Barnes et al. 2009). Clinical observations have suggested that treatment of
14
Dementia
multiple vascular factors (e.g., high blood pressure, diabetes, and atherosclerotic disease) is associated with a slower cognitive decline in AD patients without cerebrovascular disease in a progressive gradient manner, that is, the more vascular factors that are treated, the smaller the decline in global cognitive function (Deschaintre et al. 2009). Finally, geneenvironment interaction, such as interactions of APOE ε4 allele severe atherosclerosis and high blood pressure, may be important in determining the risk of dementia (Hofman et al. 1997; Qiu, Winblad, et al. 2003). The metabolic syndrome is a constellation of obesity, dyslipidemia, high blood pressure, and hyperglycemia. Follow-up studies found little evidence for the association between the metabolic syndrome in late life and the risk of dementia and AD (Muller et al. 2007; Raffaitin et al. 2009), although some components of the syndrome (e.g., diabetes) have been linked to the dementias. It is likely that, due to age-related metabolic changes, a cluster of late-life specific factors in the metabolic syndrome may not be superior to some of its individual components in defining the risk of dementia. Psychosocial Factors Evidence from epidemiological research has been accumulating that some psychosocial factors and healthy lifestyle may postpone the onset of dementia, possibly by enhancing cognitive reserve. These factors include early-life high education, adult-life rich social network and social engagement, mentally stimulating activity, and regular physical exercise. High Educational Attainments Numerous longitudinal studies have consistently shown that a higher educational achievement in early life is associated with a decreased incidence of dementia, and of AD in particular (De Ronchi et al. 1998; Qiu et al. 2001; Ngandu et al. 2007). A meta-analysis of cohort studies reported that the lowest education, compared with the highest, was associated with an approximately 60 percent increased risk of dementia and AD (Caamano-Isorna et al. 2006). The reserve hypothesis has been proposed to interpret this association, such that education could enhance cognitive reserve, which provides compensatory mechanisms to cope with degenerative pathologies in the brain and therefore delay the onset of the dementia syndrome (Stern 2006; Fratiglioni and Wang 2007). In addition, high educational achievement can be a surrogate or an indicator of high intelligent quotient, high socioeconomic status, better living environment in early
Epidemiology of the Dementias
15
life, and less occupational toxic exposures experienced over adulthood; all these conditions favor a protective effect against dementia (De Ronchi et al. 1998; Qiu et al. 2001). Social Network and Social Engagement A systematic review of longitudinal studies suggested that a poor social network or social disengagement in late life was associated with an elevated risk of dementia (Fratiglioni et al. 2004). The dementia risk was also increased in older people with social isolation or with less frequent or unsatisfactory contacts with relatives and friends (Fratiglioni, Wang, et al. 2000). Furthermore, late-life low social engagement and a decline in social engagement from middle age to late life could double the risk of dementia (Wang et al. 2002; Saczynski et al. 2006). Finally, being widowed from midlife onwards was associated with a substantially increased risk of dementia, suggesting that living with a partner might imply cognitive and social challenges that have a potential protective effect against the development of dementia later in life (Håkansson et al. 2009). It is hypothesized that a rich social network and a high level of social engagement reflect better social support, which leads to better access to resources and material goods (Fratiglioni et al. 2004). In addition, large social networks can also provide intellectual stimulations that affect cognitive function and various health outcomes through behavioral, psychological, and physiological pathways. Finally, in line with the cognitive reserve hypothesis, neuropathological data have shown that the size of social networks could modify the association between Alzheimer pathologies and cognitive function, such that cognitive function remains higher in individuals with a heavier burden of global neuropathologies if they also have larger social networks (Bennett et al. 2006). Mentally Stimulating Activity Mentally stimulating activities at leisure time, such as reading, playing board games and musical instruments, knitting, gardening, and dancing, have been associated with a reduced risk of developing AD and dementia (Verghese et al. 2003; Akbaraly et al. 2009). A few studies have shown that a greater complexity of work, particularly the complex work with data or people, could reduce the risk of dementia (Andel et al. 2005; Karp et al. 2009), especially for VaD (Kröger et al. 2008), suggesting that greater mental requirements during the working life also play a relevant role. Complex mental activity could enhance cognitive reserve and delay the
16
Dementia
onset of dementia. In addition, a neuroimaging study reported that a high level of complex mental activity across the lifespan was correlated with a reduced rate of hippocampal atrophy, which means that mental activity may also provide brain reserve (Valenzuela et al. 2008). Physical Activity Regular physical exercise was associated with a delayed onset of dementia among cognitively normal elderly (Fratiglioni et al. 2004). Even low intensity physical exercise such as walking may reduce the risk of dementia (Abbott et al. 2004; Larson et al. 2006). A study of Medicare beneficiaries in the United States showed that higher levels of physical activity were associated with a gradual reduction in dementia risk, suggesting a possible dose-response association (Scarmeas et al. 2009). A recent systematic review of prospective studies revealed that the highest physical activity, compared with the lowest, reduced the risk of dementia and AD by approximately 30–45 percent (Hamer and Chida 2009). Regular physical activity is likely to promote vascular and circulatory health by reducing blood pressure, serum lipids, BMI or obesity, and blood glucose. Because physical activity also contains components of social and cognitive activities, it may reduce the risk or postpone the onset of dementia also by providing cognitive reserve. Miscellaneous Hormone Replacement Therapy Hormone replacement therapy in postmenopausal women has been frequently reported to be associated with a lower risk of AD and dementia in numerous observational studies (Zandi et al. 2002; Qiu, Kivipelto, et al. 2009). However, the large-scale clinical trial of the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHI-MS) showed that estrogen therapy alone or in combination with progestin did not reduce the incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI); instead, the active treatment with estrogen or estrogen plus progestin was found to be associated with a two-fold increased risk for dementia and MCI (Shumaker et al. 2004). It has been argued that in the WHI-MS hormone replacement therapy was given 10 to 15 years after the menopause when the “window of critical time” for putative beneficial effects of estrogen therapy on cognition may have been missed; thus, use of hormone therapy at a younger age close to the time of menopause may reduce the risk of dementia later in life (Harman et al. 2005).
Epidemiology of the Dementias
17
Occupational Exposures Manual work involving goods production was associated with an increased risk of AD and dementia, suggesting the possible role of occupational exposure to toxics in the development of dementia (Qiu, Karp, et al. 2003). Occupational exposure to heavy metals such as aluminum and mercury is suggested to be a risk factor for AD; even high consumption of aluminum from drinking water is associated with an elevated risk of AD and dementia (Rondeau et al. 2009). However, this remains to be confirmed by further studies. In addition, occupational exposure to extremely low-frequency electromagnetic fields (ELF-EMFs) has been related to an increased risk of dementia and AD in a few follow-up studies (Feychting et al. 2003; Qiu, Fratiglioni, et al. 2004). The meta-analysis of epidemiological studies suggests an association of occupational ELF-EMF exposure with AD (Garcia et al. 2008). The biological plausibility linking high ELFEMF exposure to Alzheimer pathologies has been previously described (Sobel and Davanipour 1996). Other Factors Traumatic brain injury has been extensively investigated as a possible risk factor for AD. The meta-analysis of case-control studies supported an association between a history of head injury and the increased risk of AD (Fleminger et al. 2003). In contrast, some longitudinal studies found that AD was not associated with head trauma or only associated with severe traumatic head injury (Himanen et al. 2006). Several studies have reported an association of depression with subsequent development of dementia and AD. A meta-analysis of cohort studies yielded a pooled RR of 1.9 (95 percent CI, 1.6–2.3) for AD, and the sensitive analysis suggested that depression could be a risk factor, rather than a prodrome, for AD (Ownby et al. 2006). However, it remains debatable regarding whether depression is a preclinical symptom or a pure risk factor for dementia and AD (Amieva et al. 2008). PREVENTION OF DEMENTIA Identification of modifiable risk and protective factors for dementia provides potential for the primary prevention of the disease (Fratiglioni et al. 2008; Middleton and Yaffe 2009). Evidence from recent epidemiological research supports the notion that preventive strategies aiming at postponing the onset of dementia can be implemented in the general community.
18
Dementia
Epidemiological Evidence for Intervention Toward Dementia: A Summary Epidemiological evidence supporting the potential etiological role of modifiable risk and protective factors in dementia and AD is summarized in Table 1.1. Evidence is considered strong when several high-quality studies, especially with regard to randomized controlled trials, consistently report the same finding; moderately strong evidence is also from high-quality studies but with a limited number, or the quality of studies is moderately high (e.g., population-based prospective studies) but with numerous reports, and the finding is supported by systematic reviews and metaanalyses. Evidence from randomized placebo-controlled trials for primary intervention against dementia is currently limited for reasons such as: (1) most clinical trials have been conducted among older adults (e.g., 65 years or older) when traditional vascular risk factors are less important in dementia due to age-related pathophysiological changes (e.g., statin therapy); (2) dementia has been only considered a secondary endpoint in most clinical trials (e.g., antihypertensive therapy), in which clear benefits for primary endpoints (e.g., coronary heart disease and stroke) are shown usually in a short period of observation; and (3) intervention measures have been implemented in a period (e.g., 2–3 years) that is not sufficient long to show any efficacy. At the moment, we can conclude that moderately strong evidence, mostly from prospective observational studies, supports the hypotheses that vascular and psychosocial factors over the lifespan are involved in the development and clinical manifestation of AD and dementia. Intervention Strategies Against Dementia Intervention Toward Vascular Factors and Related Disorders Most vascular risk factors and related disorders are modifiable or treatable and can serve as targets in the development of primary preventative strategies against dementia. For example, antihypertensive therapy has been shown to reduce the risk of dementia in observational studies, and this finding was partly confirmed by clinical trials. Furthermore, studies have confirmed that obesity and diabetes can be prevented by changing dietary habits and lifestyles, and that health education may help someone quit smoking. Finally, preventing recurrent cerebrovascular disease and maintaining sufficient cerebral blood perfusion seems to be critical for postponing expression of the dementia syndrome in older people. Thus, controlling high blood pressure and obesity, especially from middle age,
Epidemiology of the Dementias
19
Table 1.1 Summary of Epidemiological Evidence Supporting the Modifiable Etiological Factors of Dementia and Alzheimer ’s Disease Profile
Risk, protective, or precipitating factors
Vascular factors Midlife risk factors: High blood pressure, diabetes, high body mass index (obesity or overweight), hyperlipidemia or high cholesterol, and smoking Late-life risk factors: Very high and very low blood pressure, diabetes, atherosclerosis, heart disease, cerebral microvascular disease (e.g., white matter lesions and infarcts), plasma hyperhomocysteine, and smoking Late-life protective factors: Use of antihypertensive medications, use of non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, light-tomoderate alcohol consumption (note: the protective effect of alcohol intake may be due to information bias, residual confounding, etc.) Late-life precipitating factors or markers: Weight loss, low blood pressure in very old or decline in blood pressure, and low cholesterol or decline in serum cholesterol Psychosocial Protective factors (lifespan): High educafactors tion, rich social network, mentallystimulating activity, active social engagement, and regular physical activity
Epidemiological evidence Moderately strong
Moderately strong
Moderately strong
Limited
Moderately strong
and preventing diabetes and recurrent stroke could be the primary preventive measures against late-life dementia. Intervention Toward Psychosocial Factors and Lifestyles High educational achievements in early life can provide cognitive reserve that benefits the whole life in terms of cognitive health and delaying the onset of late-life dementia. Extensive social networks and active
20
Dementia
engagements in intellectually stimulating activities such as reading, doing crossword puzzles, and playing board games significantly lower the risk of dementia by providing cognitive reserve or by reducing psychosocial stress. Thus, it is likely that mentally and socially integrated lifestyles could postpone the onset of dementia. Regular physical exercise may reduce the risk of the dementias resulting from cerebral atherosclerosis. Leisure activities with all three components of physical, mental, and social activities may have the most beneficial effect on dementia prevention (Karp et al. 2006). Taken together, the most promising strategy for the primary prevention of dementia may be to encourage people implementing multiple preventative measures throughout the life course, including high educational attainment in childhood and early adulthood, an active control of vascular factors (e.g., smoking) and disorders (e.g., hypertension and diabetes) over adulthood, and maintenance of mentally, physically, and socially active lifestyles during middle age and later in life. CONCLUSIONS Dementia is a major cause of functional dependence, institutionalization, and mortality among elderly people. As the population ages in the decades to come, dementia will reach an epidemic level, a scenario that poses a serious threat not only to public health but also to the social and economic development of the modern society. Epidemiological studies have shown that vascular risk factors in middle age and later in life significantly contribute to the development and progression of the dementia syndrome, whereas extensive social network and active engagement in social, physical, and mental activities may delay the onset of the dementing disorders. Hence, one of the promising strategies to deal with the tremendous challenge from the epidemic of dementia is to implement appropriate intervention measures from the life-course perspective, such as achieving high education in early life and engaging in mentally stimulating activity over the course of adulthood to enhance cognitive reserve, and maintaining vascular health by adopting a healthy lifestyle and optimally controlling vascular diseases to reduce the burden of vascular lesions in the brain. These preventive measures will enable people to maintain cognitive ability in late life, even though they may have developed a high load of Alzheimer pathologies in their brain. REFERENCES Abbott, R. D., L. R. White, G. W. Ross, K. H. Masaki, J. D. Curb, and H. Petrovitch. 2004. Walking and dementia in physically capable elderly men. JAMA 292 (12): 1447–1453.
Epidemiology of the Dementias
21
Access Economics Pty Limited Report for Asia Pacific Members of Alzheimer ’s Disease International. 2006. Dementia in the Asia Pacific region: The epidemic is here. www.accesseconomics.com.au/publicationsreports/ showreport.php?id=99) (accessed March 5, 2010). Agüero-Torres, H., L. Fratiglioni, Z. Guo, M. Viitanen, and B. Winblad. 1999. Mortality from dementia in advanced age: A five-year follow-up study of incident dementia cases. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 52 (8): 737–743. Akbaraly, T. N., F. Portet, S. Fustinoni, J. F. Dartigues, S. Artero, O. Rouaud, J. Touchon, K. Ritchie, and C. Berr. 2009. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: Results from the Three-City Study. Neurology 73 (11): 854–861. Alonso, A., T. H. Mosley, R. F. Gottesman, D. Catellier, A. R. Sharrett, and J. Coresh. 2009. Risk of dementia hospitalisation associated with cardiovascular risk factors in midlife and older age: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 80 (11): 1194–1201. Amieva, H., M. Le Goff, X. Millet, J. M. Orgogozo, K. Peres, P. Barberger-Gateau, H. Jacqmin-Gadda, and J. F. Dartigues. 2008. Prodromal Alzheimer ’s disease: Successive emergence of the clinical symptoms. Annals of Neurology 64 (5): 492–498. Andel, R., M. Crowe, N. L. Pedersen, J. Mortimer, E. Crimmins, B. Johansson, and M. Gatz. 2005. Complexity of work and risk of Alzheimer ’s disease: A population-based study of Swedish twins. Journals of Gerontology: Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 60 (5): P251–258. Anstey, K. J., D. M. Lipnicki, and L. F. Low. 2008. Cholesterol as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: A systematic review of prospective studies with meta-analysis. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16 (5): 343–354. Anstey, K. J., H. A. Mack, and N. Cherbuin. 2009. Alcohol consumption as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: Meta-analysis of prospective studies. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17 (7): 542–555. Anstey, K. J., C. von Sanden, A. Salim, and R. O’Kearney. 2007. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. American Journal of Epidemiology 166 (4): 367–378. Anttila, T., E. L. Helkala, M. Viitanen, I. Kareholt, L. Fratiglioni, B. Winblad, H. Soininen, J. Tuomilehto, A. Nissinen, and M. Kivipelto. 2004. Alcohol drinking in middle age and subsequent risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in old age: A prospective population based study. British Medical Journal 329 (7465): 539. Arvanitakis, Z., F. Grodstein, J. L. Bienias, J. A. Schneider, R. S. Wilson, J. F. Kelly, D. A. Evans, and D. A. Bennett. 2008. Relation of NSAIDs to incident AD, change in cognitive function, and AD pathology. Neurology 70 (23): 2219–2225. Arvanitakis, Z., J. A. Schneider, R. S. Wilson, J. L. Bienias, J. F. Kelly, D. A. Evans, and D. A. Bennett. 2008. Statins, incident Alzheimer disease, change in cognitive function, and neuropathology. Neurology 70 (19): 1795–1802.
22
Dementia
Arvanitakis, Z., J. A. Schneider, R. S. Wilson, Y. Li, S. E. Arnold, Z. Wang, and D. A. Bennett. 2006. Diabetes is related to cerebral infarction but not to AD pathology in older persons. Neurology 67 (11): 1960–1965. Atti, A. R., K. Palmer, S. Volpato, B. Winblad, D. De Ronchi, and L. Fratiglioni. 2008. Late-life body mass index and dementia incidence: Nine-year follow-up data from the Kungsholmen Project. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56 (1): 111–116. Barberger-Gateau, P., C. Raffaitin, L. Letenneur, C. Berr, C. Tzourio, J. F. Dartigues, and A. Alperovitch. 2007. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: The ThreeCity Cohort Study. Neurology 69 (20): 1921–1930. Barnes, D. E., K. E. Covinsky, R. A. Whitmer, L. H. Kuller, O. L. Lopez, and K. Yaffe. 2009. Predicting risk of dementia in older adults: The late-life dementia risk index. Neurology 73 (3): 173–179. Barnes, D. E., T. J. Haight, K. M. Mehta, M. C. Carlson, L. H. Kuller, and I. B. Tager. 2010. Secondhand smoke, vascular disease, and dementia incidence: Findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 171 (3): 292–302. Bennett, D. A., J. A. Schneider, Y. Tang, S. E. Arnold, and R. S. Wilson. 2006. The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer ’s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: A longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology 5 (5): 406–412. Bertram, L., M. B. McQueen, K. Mullin, D. Blacker, and R. E. Tanzi. 2007. Systematic meta-analyses of Alzheimer disease genetic association studies: The AlzGene database. Nature Genetics 39 (1): 17–23. Beydoun, M. A., H. A. Beydoun, and Y. Wang. 2008. Obesity and central obesity as risk factors for incident dementia and its subtypes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews 9 (3): 204–218. Beydoun, M. A., A. Lhotsky, Y. Wang, G. Dal Forno, Y. An, E. J. Metter, L. Ferrucci, R. O’Brien, and A. B. Zonderman. 2008. Association of adiposity status and changes in early to mid-adulthood with incidence of Alzheimer ’s disease. American Journal of Epidemiology 168 (10): 1179–1189. Biessels, G. J., S. Staekenborg, E. Brunner, C. Brayne, and P. Scheltens. 2006. Risk of dementia in diabetes mellitus: A systematic review. Lancet Neurology 5 (1): 64–74. Blennow, K., M. J. de Leon, and H. Zetterberg. 2006. Alzheimer ’s disease. Lancet 368 (9533): 387–403. Brayne, C. 2006. Incidence of dementia in England and Wales: The MRC cognitive function and ageing study. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 20 (3): S47–S51. Caamano-Isorna, F., M. Corral, A. Montes-Martinez, and B. Takkouche. 2006. Education and dementia: A meta-analytic study. Neuroepidemiology 26 (4): 226–232. Canadian Study of Health and Aging Working Group. 2000. The incidence of dementia in Canada. Neurology 55 (1): 66–73.
Epidemiology of the Dementias
23
Chiu, H. F. K., and M. Y. Zhang. 2000. Dementia research in China. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 15 (10): 947–953. Corrada, M. M., R. Brookmeyer, D. Berlau, A. Paganini-Hill, and C. H. Kawas. 2008. Prevalence of dementia after age 90—Results from the 90+Study. Neurology 71 (5): 337–343. Craft, S. 2009. The role of metabolic disorders in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia: Two roads converged. Archives of Neurology 66 (3): 300–305. Cukierman-Yaffe, T., H. C. Gerstein, J. D. Williamson, R. M. Lazar, L. Lovato, M. E. Miller, L. H. Coker, et al. 2009. Relationship between baseline glycemic control and cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes and other cardiovascular risk factors: The Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes-Memory in Diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) trial. Diabetes Care 32 (2): 221–226. De Ronchi, D., L. Fratiglioni, P. Rucci, A. Paternico, S. Graziani, and E. Dalmonte. 1998. The effect of education on dementia occurrence in an Italian population with middle to high socioeconomic status. Neurology 50 (5): 1231–1238. Deschaintre, Y., F. Richard, D. Leys, and F. Pasquier. 2009. Treatment of vascular risk factors is associated with slower decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 73 (9): 674–680. Devore, E. E., F. Grodstein, F. J. van Rooij, A. Hofman, B. Rosner, M. J. Stampfer, J. C. Witteman, and M. M. Breteler. 2009. Dietary intake of fish and omega-3 fatty acids in relation to long-term dementia risk. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 90 (1): 170–176. Dong, M. J., B. Peng, X. T. Lin, J. Zhao, Y. R. Zhou, and R. H. Wang. 2007. The prevalence of dementia in the People’s Republic of China: A systematic analysis of 1980–2004 studies. Age and Ageing 36 (6): 619–624. Engelhart, M. J., M. I. Geerlings, J. Meijer, A. Kiliaan, A. Ruitenberg, J. C. van Swieten, T. Stijnen, A. Hofman, J. C. Witteman, and M. M. Breteler. 2004. Inflammatory proteins in plasma and the risk of dementia: The Rotterdam study. Archives of Neurology 61 (5): 668–672. Etminan, M., S. Gill, and A. Samii. 2003. Effect of non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs on risk of Alzheimer ’s disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. British Medical Journal 327 (7407): 128. Féart, C., C. Samieri, V. Rondeau, H. Amieva, F. Portet, J. F. Dartigues, N. Scarmeas, and P. Barberger-Gateau. 2009. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. JAMA 302 (6): 638–648. Ferri, C. P., M. Prince, C. Brayne, H. Brodaty, L. Fratiglioni, M. Ganguli, K. Hall, et al. 2005. Global prevalence of dementia: A Delphi consensus study. Lancet 366 (9503): 2112–2117. Feychting, M., F. Jonsson, N. L. Pedersen, and A. Ahlbom. 2003. Occupational magnetic field exposure and neurodegenerative disease. Epidemiology 14 (4): 413–419. Fitzpatrick, A. L., L. H. Kuller, O. L. Lopez, P. Diehr, E. S. O’Meara, W. T. Longstreth Jr., and J. A. Luchsinger. 2009. Midlife and late-life obesity and the
24
Dementia
risk of dementia: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Archives of Neurology 66 (3): 336–342. Fleminger, S., D. L. Oliver, S. Lovestone, S. Rabe-Hesketh, and A. Giora. 2003. Head injury as a risk factor for Alzheimer ’s disease: The evidence 10 years on; a partial replication. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 74 (7): 857–862. Fotuhi, M., P. Mohassel, and K. Yaffe. 2009. Fish consumption, long-chain omega-3 fatty acids and risk of cognitive decline on Alzheimer disease: A complex association. Nature Clinical Practice Neurology 5 (3): 140–152. Fratiglioni, L., L. J. Launer, K. Andersen, M. M. Breteler, J. R. Copeland, J. F. Dartigues, A. Lobo, J. Martinez-Lage, H. Soininen, and A. Hofman. 2000. Incidence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology 54 (11 Suppl 5): S10–S15. Fratiglioni, L., S. Paillard-Borg, and B. Winblad. 2004. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology 3 (6): 343–353. Fratiglioni, L., E. von Strauss, and C. Qiu. 2008. Epidemiology of the dementias of old age. In The Oxford Textbook of Old Age Psychiatry, ed. T. Dening, R. Jacoby, C. Oppenheimer, and A. Thomas, 391–406. London: Oxford University Press. Fratiglioni, L., and H. X. Wang. 2007. Brain reserve hypothesis in dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 12 (1): 11–22. Fratiglioni, L., H. X. Wang, K. Ericsson, M. Maytan, and B. Winblad. 2000. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: A community-based longitudinal study. Lancet 355 (9212): 1315–1319. Ganguli, M., and E. G. Rodriguez. 1999. Reporting of dementia on death certificates: A community study. Journal of American Geriatric Society 47 (7): 842–849. Garcia, A. M., A. Sisternas, and S. P. Hoyos. 2008. Occupational exposure to extremely low frequency electric and magnetic fields and Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology 37 (2): 329–340. Gatz, M., C. A. Reynolds, L. Fratiglioni, B. Johansson, J. A. Mortimer, S. Berg, A. Fiske, and N. L. Pedersen. 2006. Role of genes and environments for explaining Alzheimer disease. Archives of General Psychiatry 63 (2): 168–174. Gray, S. L., M. L. Anderson, P. K. Crane, J. C. Breitner, W. McCormick, J. D. Bowen, L. Teri, and E. Larson. 2008. Antioxidant vitamin supplement use and risk of dementia or Alzheimer ’s disease in older adults. Journal of American Geriatric Society 56 (2): 291–295. Green, R. C., L. A. Cupples, R. Go, K. S. Benke, T. Edeki, P. A. Griffith, M. Williams, et al. 2002. Risk of dementia among white and African American relatives of patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA 287 (3): 329–336. Gustafson, D. 2006. Adiposity indices and dementia. Lancet Neurology 5 (8): 713–720. Haag, M. D., A. Hofman, P. J. Koudstaal, B. H. Stricker, and M. M. Breteler. 2009. Statins are associated with a reduced risk of Alzheimer disease regardless
Epidemiology of the Dementias
25
of lipophilicity: The Rotterdam Study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 80 (1): 13–17. Håkansson, K., S. Rovio, E. L. Helkala, A. R. Vilska, B. Winblad, H. Soininen, A. Nissinen, A. H. Mohammed, and M. Kivipelto. 2009. Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: Population based cohort study. British Medical Journal 339: b2462. Hamer, M., and Y. Chida. 2009. Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: A systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychological Medicine 39 (1): 3–11. Harman, S. M., F. Naftolin, E. A. Brinton, and D. R. Judelson. 2005. Is the estrogen controversy over? Deconstructing the Women’s Health Initiative Study: A critical evaluation of the evidence. Annals of New York Academic Science 1052: 43–56. Hassing, L. B., A. K. Dahl, V. Thorvaldsson, S. Berg, M. Gatz, N. L. Pedersen, and B. Johansson. 2009. Overweight in midlife and risk of dementia: A 40-year follow-up study. International Journal of Obesity (Lond) 33 (8): 893–898. Hayden, K. M., P. P. Zandi, N. A. West, J. T. Tschanz, M. C. Norton, C. Corcoran, J. C. S. Breitner, and K. A. Welsh-Bohmer. 2009. Effects of family history and apolipoprotein E ε4 status on cognitive decline in the absence of Alzheimer dementia: The Cache County Study. Archives of Neurology 66 (11): 1378–1383. Hebert, L. E., P. A. Scherr, J. L. Bienias, D. A. Bennett, and D. A. Evans. 2003. Alzheimer disease in the US population: Prevalence estimates using the 2000 census. Archives of Neurology 60 (8): 1119–1122. Helmer, C., P. Joly, L. Letenneur, D. Commenges, and J. F. Dartigues. 2001. Mortality with dementia: Results from a French prospective community-based cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology 154 (7): 642–648. Helzner, E. P., N. Scarmeas, S. Cosentino, M. X. Tang, N. Schupf, and Y. Stern. 2008. Survival in Alzheimer disease: A multiethnic, population-based study of incident cases. Neurology 71 (19): 1489–1495. Himanen, L., R. Portin, H. Isoniemi, H. Helenius, T. Kurki, and O. Tenovuo. 2006. Longitudinal cognitive changes in traumatic brain injury: A 30-year followup study. Neurology 66 (2): 187–192. Hoffman, L. B., J. Schmeidler, G. T. Lesser, M. S. Beeri, D. P. Purohit, H. T. Grossman, and V. Haroutunian. 2009. Less Alzheimer disease neuropathology in medicated hypertensive than nonhypertensive persons. Neurology 72 (20): 1720–1726. Hofman, A., A. Ott, M. M. Breteler, M. L. Bots, A. J. Slooter, F. van Harskamp, C. N. van Duijn, C. Van Broeckhoven, and D. E. Grobbee. 1997. Atherosclerosis, apolipoprotein E, and prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease in the Rotterdam Study. Lancet 349 (9046): 151–154. Huang, W., C. Qiu, E. von Strauss, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2004. APOE genotype, family history of dementia, and Alzheimer disease risk: A sixyear follow-up study. Archives of Neurology 61 (12): 1930–1934.
26
Dementia
Ikeda, M., K. Hokoishi, N. Maki, A. Nebu, N. Tachibana, K. Komori, K. Shigenobu, R. Fukuhara, and H. Tanabe. 2001. Increased prevalence of vascular dementia in Japan: A community-based epidemiological study. Neurology 57 (5): 839–844. Jagger, C., K. Andersen, M. M. Breteler, J. R. Copeland, C. Helmer, M. Baldereschi, L. Fratiglioni, et al. 2000. Prognosis with dementia in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology 54 (11 Suppl 5): S16–S20. Jin, Y. P., M. Gatz, B. Johansson, and N. L. Pedersen. 2004. Sensitivity and specificity of dementia coding in two Swedish disease registries. Neurology 63 (4): 739–741. Jorm, A. F., and D. Jolley. 1998. The incidence of dementia: A meta-analysis. Neurology 51 (3): 728–733. Kalaria, R. N., G. E. Maestre, R. Arizaga, R. P. Friedland, D. Galasko, K. T. Hall, J. A. Luchsinger, et al. 2008. Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia in developing countries: Prevalence, management, and risk factors. Lancet Neurology 7 (9): 812–826. Karp, A., R. Andel, M. G. Parker, H. X. Wang, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2009. Mentally stimulating activities at work during midlife and dementia risk after age 75: Follow-up study from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17 (3): 227–236. Karp, A., S. Paillard-Borg, H. X. Wang, M. Silverstein, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2006. Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 21 (2): 65–73. Kawas, C., S. Gray, R. Brookmeyer, J. Fozard, and A. Zonderman. 2000. Agespecific incidence rates of Alzheimer ’s disease: The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology 54 (11): 2072–2077. Kivipelto, M., T. Ngandu, T. Laatikainen, B. Winblad, H. Soininen, and J. Tuomilehto. 2006. Risk score for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among middle aged people: A longitudinal, population-based study. Lancet Neurology 5 (9): 735–741. Kopf, D., and L. Frölich. 2009. Risk of incident Alzheimer ’s disease in diabetic patients: A systematic review of prospective trials. Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease 16 (4): 677–685. Korf, E. S., L. R. White, P. Scheltens, and L. J. Launer. 2004. Midlife blood pressure and the risk of hippocampal atrophy: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Hypertension 44 (1): 29–34. Kröger, E., R. Andel, J. Lindsay, Z. Benounissa, R. Verreault, and D. Laurin. 2008. Is complexity of work associated with risk of dementia? The Canadian Study of Health and Aging. American Journal of Epidemiology 167 (7): 820–830. Kröger, E., R. Verreault, P. H. Carmichael, J. Lindsay, P. Julien, E. Dewailly, P. Ayotte, and D. Laurin. 2009. Omega-3 fatty acids and risk of dementia: The Canadian Study of Health and Aging. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 90 (1): 184–192.
Epidemiology of the Dementias
27
Kukull, W. A., R. Higdon, J. D. Bowen, W. C. McCormick, L. Teri, G. D. Schellenberg, G. van Belle, L. Jolley, and E. B. Larson. 2002. Dementia and Alzheimer disease incidence: A prospective cohort study. Archives of Neurology 59 (11): 1737–1746. Larson, E. B., L. Wang, J. D. Bowen, W. C. McCormick, L. Teri, P. Crane, and W. Kukull. 2006. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Annals of Internal Medicine 144 (2): 73–81. Launer, L. J., H. Petrovitch, G. W. Ross, W. Markesbery, and L. R. White. 2008. AD brain pathology: Vascular origins? Results from the HAAS autopsy study. Neurobiology of Aging 29 (10): 1587–1590. Laurin, D., K. H. Masaki, L. R. White, and L. J. Launer. 2007. Ankle-to-brachial index and dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Circulation 116 (20): 2269–2274. Li, G., E. B. Larson, J. A. Sonnen, J. B. Shofer, E. C. Petrie, A. Schantz, E. R. Peskind, M. A. Raskind, J. C. S. Breitner, and T. J. Montine. 2007. Statin therapy is associated with reduced neuropathologic changes of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 69 (9): 878–885. Li, N. C., A. Lee, R. A. Whitmer, M. Kivipelto, E. Lawler, L. E. Kazis, and B. Wolozin. 2010. Use of angiotensin receptor blockers and risk of dementia in a predominantly male population: Prospective cohort analysis. British Medical Journal 340: b5465. Llibre Rodriguez, J. J., C. P. Ferri, D. Acosta, M. Guerra, Y. G. Huang, K. S. Jacob, E. S. Krishnamoorthy, et al. 2008. Prevalence of dementia in Latin America, India, and China: A population-based cross-sectional survey. Lancet 372 (9637): 464–474. Lobo, A., L. J. Launer, L. Fratiglioni, K. Andersen, A. Di Carlo, M. M. Breteler, J. R. Copeland, et al. 2000. Prevalence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology 54 (11 Suppl 5): S4–S9. Lopes, M. A., S. R. Hototian, G. C. Reis, H. Elkis, and C. M. C. Bottino. 2007. Systematic review of dementia prevalence 1994 to 2000. Dementia and Neuropsychologia 1 (3): 230–240. Lu, F. P., K. P. Lin, and H. K. Kuo. 2009. Diabetes and the risk of multi-system aging phenotypes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 4 (1): e4144. Luchsinger, J. A., and R. Mayeux. 2004. Dietary factors and Alzheimer ’s disease. Lancet Neurology 3 (10): 579–587. Malouf, R., and J. G. Evans. 2008. Folic acid with or without vitamin B12 for the prevention and treatment of healthy elderly and demented people. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4): CD004514. Martin, B. K., C. Szekely, J. Brandt, S. Piantadosi, J. C. S. Breitner, S. Craft, D. Evans, R. Green, and M. Mullan. 2008. Cognitive function over time in the Alzheimer ’s disease anti-inflammatory prevention trial (ADAPT): Results
28
Dementia
of a randomized, controlled trial of naproxen and celecoxib. Archives of Neurology 65 (7): 896–905. Matthews, F., and C. Brayne. 2005. The incidence of dementia in England and Wales: Findings from the five identical sites of the MRC CFA Study. PLoS Medicine 2 (8): e193. McGuinness, B., D. Craig, R. Bullock, and P. Passmore. 2009. Statins for the prevention of dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (2): CD003160. McGuinness, B., S. Todd, P. Passmore, and R. Bullock. 2009. Blood pressure lowering in patients without prior cerebrovascular disease for prevention of cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4): CD004034. Middleton, L. E., and K. Yaffe. 2009. Promising strategies for the prevention of dementia. Archives of Neurology 66 (10): 1210–1215. Miech, R. A., J. C. Breitner, P. P. Zandi, A. S. Khachaturian, J. C. Anthony, and L. Mayer. 2002. Incidence of AD may decline in the early 90s for men, later for women: The Cache County Study. Neurology 58 (2): 209–218. Mitchell, S. L., J. M. Teno, D. K. Kiely, M. L. Shaffer, R. N. Jones, H. G. Prigerson, L. Volicer, J. L. Givens, and M. B. Hamel. 2009. The clinical course of advanced dementia. New England Journal of Medicine 361 (16): 1529–1538. Muller, M., M. X. Tang, N. Schupf, J. J. Manly, R. Mayeux, and J. A. Luchsinger. 2007. Metabolic syndrome and dementia risk in a multiethnic elderly cohort. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 24 (3): 185–192. Mura, T., J. F. Dartigues, and C. Berr. 2010. How many dementia cases in France and Europe? Alternative projections and scenarios 2010–2050. European Journal of Neurology 17 (2): 252–259. Newman, A. B., A. L. Fitzpatrick, O. Lopez, S. Jackson, C. Lyketsos, W. Jagust, D. Ives, S. T. Dekosky, and L. H. Kuller. 2005. Dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease incidence in relationship to cardiovascular disease in the Cardiovascular Health Study cohort. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53 (7): 1101–1117. Ngandu, T., E. von Strauss, E. L. Helkala, B. Winblad, A. Nissinen, J. Tuomilehto, H. Soininen, and M. Kivipelto. 2007. Education and dementia: What lies behind the association? Neurology 69 (14): 1442–1450. Nitrini, R., C. M. C. Bottino, C. Albala, N. S. C. Capunay, C. Ketzoian, J. J. L. Rodriguez, G. E. Maestre, A. T. A. Ramos-Cerqueira, and P. Caramelli. 2009. Prevalence of dementia in Latin America: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. International Psychogeriatrics 21 (4): 622–630. Ott, A., M. M. Breteler, M. C. de Bruyne, F. van Harskamp, D. E. Grobbee, and A. Hofman. 1997. Atrial fibrillation and dementia in a population-based study: The Rotterdam Study. Stroke 28 (2): 316–321. Ownby, R. L., E. Crocco, A. Acevedo, V. John, and D. Loewenstein. 2006. Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry 63 (5): 530–538.
Epidemiology of the Dementias
29
Paul, C. A., R. Au, L. Fredman, J. M. Massaro, S. Seshadri, C. DeCarli, and P. A. Wolf. 2008. Association of alcohol consumption with brain volume in the Framingham study. Archives of Neurology 65 (10): 1363–1367. Peila, R., B. L. Rodriguez, and L. J. Launer. 2002. Type 2 diabetes, APOE gene, and the risk for dementia and related pathologies: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Diabetes 51 (4): 1256–1262. Peila, R., L. R. White, K. Masaki, H. Petrovitch, and L. J. Launer. 2006. Reducing the risk of dementia: Efficacy of long-term treatment of hypertension. Stroke 37 (5): 1165–1170. Pendlebury, S. T., and P. M. Rothwell. 2009. Prevalence, incidence, and factors associated with pre-stroke and post-stroke dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurology 8 (11): 1006–1018. Peters, R., N. Beckett, F. Forette, J. Tuomilehto, R. Clarke, C. Ritchie, A. Waldman, et al. 2008. Incident dementia and blood pressure lowering in the Hypertension in the Very Elderly Trial cognitive function assessment (HYVET-COG): A double-blind, placebo controlled trial. Lancet Neurology 7 (8): 683–689. Peters, R., J. Peters, J. Warner, N. Beckett, and C. Bulpitt. 2008. Alcohol, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly: A systematic review. Age and Ageing 37 (5): 505–512. Peters, R., R. Poulter, J. Warner, N. Beckett, L. Burch, and C. Bulpitt. 2008. Smoking, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly: A systematic review. BMC Geriatrics 8: 36. Purandare, N., A. Burns, K. J. Daly, J. Hardicre, J. Morris, G. Macfarlane, and C. McCollum. 2006. Cerebral emboli as a potential cause of Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia: Case-control study. British Medical Journal 332 (7550): 1119–1122. Qiu, C., L. Backman, B. Winblad, H. Agüero-Torres, and L. Fratiglioni. 2001. The influence of education on clinically diagnosed dementia incidence and mortality data from the Kungsholmen project. Archives of Neurology 58 (12): 2034–2039. Qiu, C., L. Fratiglioni, A. Karp, B. Winblad, and T. Bellander. 2004. Occupational exposure to electromagnetic fields and risk of Alzheimer ’s disease. Epidemiology 15 (6): 687–694. Qiu, C., A. Karp, E. von Strauss, B. Winblad, L. Fratiglioni, and T. Bellander. 2003. Lifetime principal occupation and risk of Alzheimer ’s disease in the Kungsholmen project. American Journal of Industrial Medicine 43 (2): 204–211. Qiu, C., M. Kivipelto, H. Agüero-Torres, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2004. Risk and protective effects of the APOE gene towards Alzheimer ’s disease in the Kungsholmen project: Variation by age and sex. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 75 (6): 828–833. Qiu, C., M. Kivipelto, and E. von Strauss. 2009. Epidemiology of Alzheimer ’s disease: occurrence, determinants, and strategies toward intervention. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 11 (2): 111–128.
30
Dementia
Qiu, C., B. Winblad, J. Fastbom, and L. Fratiglioni. 2003. Combined effects of APOE genotype, blood pressure, and antihypertensive drug use on incident AD. Neurology 61 (5): 655–660. Qiu, C., B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2005. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurology 4 (8): 487–499. Qiu, C., B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2009. Low diastolic pressure and risk of dementia in very old people: A longitudinal study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 28 (3): 213–219. Qiu, C., B. Winblad, A. Marengoni, I. Klarin, J. Fastbom, and L. Fratiglioni. 2006. Heart failure and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: A populationbased cohort study. Archives of Internal Medicine 166 (9): 1003–1008. Qiu, C., W. Xu, and L. Fratiglioni. 2010. Vascular and psychosocial factors for Alzheimer ’s disease: Epidemiological evidence toward intervention. Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease 20 (3): 689–697. Qiu, C., W. Xu, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2010. Vascular risk profiles for dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease in very old people: A population-based longitudinal study. Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease 20 (1): 293–300. Raffaitin, C., H. Gin, J. P. Empana, C. Helmer, C. Berr, C. Tzourio, F. Portet, J. F. Dartigues, A. Alperovitch, and P. Barberger-Gateau. 2009. Metabolic syndrome and risk for incident Alzheimer ’s disease or vascular dementia: The Three-City Study. Diabetes Care 32 (1): 169–174. Rodriguez, E. G., H. H. Dodge, M. A. Birzescu, G. P. Stoehr, and M. Ganguli. 2002. Use of lipid-lowering drugs in older adults with and without dementia: A community-based epidemiological study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 50 (11): 1852–1856. Rondeau, V., H. Jacqmin-Gadda, D. Commenges, C. Helmer, and J. F. Dartigues. 2009. Aluminum and silica in drinking water and the risk of Alzheimer ’s disease or cognitive decline: Findings from 15-year follow-up of the PAQUID cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology 169 (4): 489–496. Rosengren, A., I. Skoog, D. Gustafson, and L. Wilhelmsen. 2005. Body mass index, other cardiovascular risk factors, and hospitalization for dementia. Archives of Internal Medicine 165 (3): 321–326. Saczynski, J. S., L. A. Pfeifer, K. Masaki, E. S. Korf, D. Laurin, L. White, and L. J. Launer. 2006. The effect of social engagement on incident dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 163 (5): 433–440. Savva, G. M. and B. C. M. Stephan. 2010. Epidemiological studies of the effect of stroke on incident dementia: A systematic review. Stroke 41 (1): E41–E46. Scarmeas, N., J. A. Luchsinger, N. Schupf, A. M. Brickman, S. Cosentino, M. X. Tang, and Y. Stern. 2009. Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 302 (6): 627–637. Scarmeas, N., Y. Stern, R. Mayeux, and J. A. Luchsinger. 2006. Mediterranean diet, Alzheimer disease, and vascular mediation. Archives of Neurology 63 (12): 1709–1717.
Epidemiology of the Dementias
31
Schmidt, R., H. Schmidt, J. D. Curb, K. Masaki, L. R. White, and L. J. Launer. 2002. Early inflammation and dementia: A 25-year follow-up of the HonoluluAsia Aging Study. Annals of Neurology 52 (2): 168–174. Schneider, J. A., Z. Arvanitakis, W. Bang, and D. A. Bennett. 2007. Mixed brain pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-dwelling older persons. Neurology 69 (24): 2197–2204. Seshadri, S., A. Beiser, J. Selhub, P. F. Jacques, I. H. Rosenberg, R. B. D’Agostino, P. W. Wilson, and P. A. Wolf. 2002. Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease. New England Journal of Medicine 346 (7): 476–483. Seshadri, S., and P. A. Wolf. 2007. Lifetime risk of stroke and dementia: Current concepts, and estimates from the Framingham Study. Lancet Neurology 6 (12): 1106–1114. Seshadri, S., P. A. Wolf, A. S. Beiser, J. Selhub, R. Au, P. F. Jacques, M. Yoshita, I. H. Rosenberg, R. B. D’Agostino, and C. DeCarli. 2008. Association of plasma total homocysteine levels with subclinical brain injury. Archives of Neurology 65 (5): 642–649. Shumaker, S. A., C. Legault, L. Kuller, S. R. Rapp, L. Thal, D. S. Lane, H. Fillit, et al. 2004. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA 291 (24): 2947–2958. Slooter, A. J., M. Cruts, S. Kalmijn, A. Hofman, M. M. Breteler, C. Van Broeckhoven, and C. M. van Duijn. 1998. Risk estimates of dementia by apolipoprotein E genotypes from a population-based incidence study: The Rotterdam Study. Archives of Neurology 55 (7): 964–968. Sobel, E., and Z. Davanipour. 1996. Electromagnetic field exposure may cause increased production of amyloid beta and eventually lead to Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 47 (6): 1594–1600. Solomon, A., I. Kareholt, T. Ngandu, B. Winblad, A. Nissinen, J. Tuomilehto, H. Soininen, and M. Kivipelto. 2007. Serum cholesterol changes after midlife and late-life cognition: Twenty-one-year follow-up study. Neurology 68 (10): 751–756. Solomon, A., M. Kivipelto, B. Wolozin, J. F. Zhou, and R. A. Whitmer. 2009. Midlife serum cholesterol and increased risk of Alzheimer ’s and vascular dementia three decades later. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 28 (1): 75–80. Sonnen, J. A., E. B. Larson, K. Brickell, P. K. Crane, R. Woltjer, T. J. Montine, and S. Craft. 2009. Different patterns of cerebral injury in dementia with or without diabetes. Archives of Neurology 66 (3): 315–322. Stern, Y. 2006. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 20 (3 Suppl 2): S69–S74. Stewart, R., K. Masaki, Q. L. Xue, R. Peila, H. Petrovitch, L. R. White, and L. J. Launer. 2005. A 32-year prospective study of change in body weight and incident dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Archives of Neurology 62 (1): 55–60.
32
Dementia
Stewart, R., L. R. White, Q. L. Xue, and L. J. Launer. 2007. Twenty-six-year change in total cholesterol levels and incident dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Archives of Neurology 64 (1): 103–107. Tan, Z. S., A. S. Beiser, R. S. Vasan, R. Roubenoff, C. A. Dinarello, T. B. Harris, E. J. Benjamin, et al. 2007. Inflammatory markers and the risk of Alzheimer disease: The Framingham study. Neurology 68 (22): 1902–1908. Troncoso, J. C., A. B. Zonderman, S. M. Resnick, B. Crain, O. Pletnikova, and R. J. O’Brien. 2008. Effect of infarcts on dementia in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Annals of Neurology 64 (2): 168–176. Tschanz, J. T., C. Corcoran, I. Skoog, A. S. Khachaturian, J. Herrick, K. M. Hayden, K. A. Welsh-Bohmer, et al. 2004. Dementia: The leading predictor of death in a defined elderly population: The Cache County Study. Neurology 62 (7): 1156–1162. Tyas, S. L., L. R. White, H. Petrovitch, G. Webster Ross, D. J. Foley, H. K. Heimovitz, and L. J. Launer. 2003. Mid-life smoking and late-life dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Neurobiology of Aging 24 (4): 589–596. Valenzuela, M. J., P. Sachdev, W. Wen, X. H. Chen, and H. Brodaty. 2008. Lifespan mental activity predicts diminished rate of hippocampal atrophy. PLoS One 3 (7): e2598. van Dam, F., and W. A. van Gool. 2009. Hyperhomocysteinemia and Alzheimer ’s disease: A systematic review. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 48 (3): 425–430. van Oijen, M., F. J. de Jong, J. C. Witteman, A. Hofman, P. J. Koudstaal, and M. M. Breteler. 2007. Atherosclerosis and risk for dementia. Annals of Neurology 61 (5): 403–410. Verghese, J., R. B. Lipton, M. J. Katz, C. B. Hall, C. A. Derby, G. Kuslansky, A. F. Ambrose, M. Sliwinski, and H. Buschke. 2003. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine 348 (25): 2508–2516. Vermeer, S. E., N. D. Prins, T. den Heijer, A. Hofman, P. J. Koudstaal, and M. M. Breteler. 2003. Silent brain infarcts and the risk of dementia and cognitive decline. New England Journal of Medicine 348 (13): 1215–1222. von Strauss, E., M. Viitanen, D. De Ronchi, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 1999. Aging and the occurrence of dementia: Findings from a population-based cohort with a large sample of nonagenarians. Archives of Neurology 56 (5): 587–592. Wang, H. X., A. Karp, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2002. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: A longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. American Journal of Epidemiology 155 (12): 1081–1887. Whalley, L. J., F. D. Dick, and G. McNeill. 2006. A life-course approach to the aetiology of late-onset dementias. Lancet Neurology 5 (1): 87–96.
Epidemiology of the Dementias
33
Whitmer, R. A., A. J. Karter, K. Yaffe, C. P. Quesenberry, and J. V. Selby. 2009. Hypoglycemic episodes and risk of dementia in older patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. JAMA 301 (15): 1565–1572. Whitmer, R. A., S. Sidney, J. Selby, S. C. Johnston, and K. Yaffe. 2005. Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life. Neurology 64 (2): 277–281. Wolfson, C., D. B. Wolfson, M. Asgharian, C. E. M’Lan, T. Ostbye, K. Rockwood, and D. B. Hogan. 2001. A reevaluation of the duration of survival after the onset of dementia. New England Journal of Medicine 344 (15): 1111–1116. Xie, J., C. Brayne, F. E. Matthews, and Council Med Res. 2008. Survival times in people with dementia: Analysis from population based cohort study with 14 year follow-up. British Medical Journal 336 (7638): 258–262. Xu, W., C. Qiu, M. Gatz, N. L. Pedersen, B. Johansson, and L. Fratiglioni. 2009. Mid- and late-life diabetes in relation to the risk of dementia: A populationbased twin study. Diabetes 58 (1): 71–77. Xu, W., C. Qiu, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2007. The effect of borderline diabetes on the risk of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease. Diabetes 56 (1): 211–216. Zandi, P. P., M. C. Carlson, B. L. Plassman, K. A. Welsh-Bohmer, L. S. Mayer, D. C. Steffens, and J. C. Breitner. 2002. Hormone replacement therapy and incidence of Alzheimer disease in older women: The Cache County Study. JAMA 288 (17): 2123–2129. Zhang, Z. X., G. E. Zahner, G. C. Roman, J. Liu, Z. Hong, Q. M. Qu, X. H. Liu, et al. 2005. Dementia subtypes in China: Prevalence in Beijing, Xian, Shanghai, and Chengdu. Archives of Neurology 62 (3): 447–453.
Chapter 2
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia Wilm Quentin, Steffi G. Riedel-Heller, Melanie Luppa, Hanna Leicht, and Hans-Helmut König
Worldwide direct costs of dementia have been estimated to be US$156 billion (Wimo, Jonsson, and Winblad 2006), and annual costs of patients with neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia have been found to amount to US$39,514 (Murman et al. 2002). Similar results of cost-of-illness (COI) studies of dementia are frequently reported in the scientific literature, policy discourses, and the media alike. They contribute to the impression that dementia is “very costly.” And they make people worry that high costs per case combined with the projected increase in dementia prevalence in countries around the world will place a heavy burden on societies. Of course, the main purpose of COI studies is not to make people worry. They are intended to provide estimates about the economic impact of diseases on different aspects of health systems and societies in order to assist policymakers in decisions of planning and financing (Bloom et al. 2001). However, unfortunately, methodological characteristics and estimated results of COI studies vary widely. Consequently, the usefulness of COI studies has come under debate because high variability puts into question the reliability of their results (Koopmanschap 1998). This chapter aims to provide an overview to theory and practice of COI studies of dementia and uses the results of existing studies to outline the impact of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia on resource
36
Dementia
use and costs. It first reviews the theoretical health economics background of COI studies of dementia before exploring in more detail methodological characteristics of studies and sources of variation of their results. The following section then presents findings of a recent systematic literature review of COI studies of dementia by Quentin et al. (2010). Their findings demonstrate that results of COI-studies show consistent patterns while the exact size of costs varies greatly among studies. Drawing on the literature review and on results of existing COI studies analyzing the impact of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia, the subsequent section explores the likely role of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia in determining resource use and costs. Finally, the chapter summarizes the argument and concludes that neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia are important determinants of costs of care. However, the exact size of costs depends on many factors that should be controlled through greater standardization of COI studies of dementia. Results of existing studies always need to be interpreted by considering the specific design, the setting, and other methodological characteristics outlined in this chapter. BACKGROUND: HEALTH ECONOMICS AND DEMENTIA COI studies of dementia are part of the health economics literature, which makes use of economic concepts in order to assess issues related to the allocation of resources to and within the health sector (Folland, Goodman, and Stano 2007). One concept essential for understanding the results of COI studies is the opportunity costs or economic costs. In all societies resources are scarce and decisions have to be taken between alternative uses of resources. If resources are committed to one alternative, society gives up the opportunity to enjoy the benefits of the other. Therefore, the opportunity cost of one item is what is given up in order to obtain it. COI studies usually aim to estimate the opportunity costs of an illness, which are equal to the value of the forgone opportunity to use the resources used or lost due to the disease in another way. If there were no diseases at all, patients would be able to work, hospitals could be transformed into hotels, and doctors would have chosen to become bankers or painters. From a societal perspective, which is often adopted by economists in order to estimate the costs of dementia, the value of all resources that could have been employed in other ways to increase the welfare of society needs to be considered. As money is a medium to exchange and
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 37
store value, the monetary value of the resources used or lost due to illness indicates what is given up as a result of the illness. In order to estimate the opportunity costs of an illness, analysts usually proceed in two steps: First, they measure the quantity of resources consumed or lost due to illness (usually by following a sample of selected patients); and second, they attach monetary values (unit costs) to identified resources. In order to get an idea of the resources that are consumed, and of the determinants of resource consumption, it is useful to think about the normal course of the disease. In progressive diseases like dementia, resource consumption is highly dependent on the stage of the disease. In early stages, demented patients are usually cared for in the community. They receive informal care and support in activities of daily living (ADLs) from mostly female caregivers (Harrow et al. 2004). As the disease progresses and functional ability deteriorates, patients become increasingly dependent on others. Caregivers sometimes have to cut down working hours in order to take care of their parents or relatives and may even incur excess healthcare costs themselves (Moore, Zhu, and Clipp 2001). At the same time, increasing dementia severity leads to augmenting demand for formal community support services. Eventually, many patients require institutional nursinghome care once the subjective caregiver burden becomes overwhelming (Yaffe et al. 2002). In addition, demented patients have been found to be high users of healthcare services (Hill et al. 2006; Bynum et al. 2004), even though the evidence in this field is not unambiguous (Kane and Atherly 2000). Attaching monetary values to all of the resources used (e.g., formal medical and informal care) and lost (lost productivity of patients and caregivers) and adding them up yields the opportunity cost of dementia. In theory, the methodology appears relatively straightforward. However, in practice, things are more complicated. METHODOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS AND SOURCES OF VARIATION IN DEMENTIA COST-OF-ILLNESS STUDIES COI studies vary widely in their methodological characteristics. They differ in their methods of sample selection and data collection and in the employed cost-estimation techniques. Methodological characteristics of COI studies are important since they determine what is measured and what is reported in their results. This section provides an overview to methodological differences between COI studies of dementia and highlights the impact that these differences can have on estimated results.
38
Dementia
Differences in Sample Selection and Data Collection Table 2.1 presents different approaches of COI studies to solving the methodological problems of sample selection and data collection. Since different approaches influence estimated results, they become important sources of variation between studies. The last column in Table 2.1 presents examples of characteristics of existing studies. Sample Selection As mentioned above, in most COI studies resource consumption for a sample of patients is measured. Important sources of variation between
Table 2.1 General Design Features and Sources of Variation between COI Studies of Dementia Methodological Problem Source of Variation
Characteristics of Studies (examples)
Sample selection Identification of patients
Population screening Physician diagnosis Dementia (unspecified) Alzheimer ’s disease vs. vascular dementia, etc. Positive screening test (e.g., MMSEa) Proportion of patients living in institutional care settings Proportion of patients in different stages of disease Proportion of patients with neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia Definition of severity (e.g., by MMSE, CDRb) Number of stages Questionnaire Analysis of medical claims data Retrospective Prospective 1 month 4 years
Definition of dementia
Sample composition
Classification of patients into disease stages Data collection
Source of information Study design Time period
a b
MMSE = Mini Mental State Examination CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating Scale
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 39
studies arise from different methods of identifying patients, different definitions of dementia, the resulting divergence in the composition of samples, and from differences in the methods used to classify patients into different stages of disease. The method for identification of patients, for example, whether an entire population is screened or whether patients are recruited from general practitioners or specialists, influences the sample composition: Studies using population screenings in order to identify patients are more likely to include a larger number of patients in earlier stages of the disease than studies recruiting patients from specialist practices. The resulting differences between samples (e.g., higher numbers of patients living in institutional-care settings) are bound to influence estimated costs. Similarly, the study definition of dementia can influence sample composition. In cases where an unspecified dementia diagnosis is sufficient for study inclusion, the sample may include varying proportions of patients with specific types of dementia. For example, the proportion of patients with vascular dementia (VaD) included in a study of “dementia patients” could be higher than one would expect in the general population. Consequently, resource consumption of “demented patients” would be overestimated as VaD patients have been found to have higher costs in earlier stages of dementia than patients with Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) (Wimo and Winblad 2003). Furthermore, studies analyzing stage dependency of costs can differ in their approach to the classification of patients into different stages of severity. On the one hand, they can apply different measures of severity. Some use the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) in order to assess the cognitive state of their sampled patients and classify patients accordingly. Other studies use measures of functional disability such as dependency in activities of daily living or instrumental activities of daily living in order to classify patients. On the other hand, researchers can classify patients into different numbers of stages of severity. Of course, depending on the way patients are classified into different stages of disease, reported cost estimates for mild, moderate, and severe dementia vary considerably. Data Collection Concerning data collection, studies can use various sources of information, can collect data retrospectively or prospectively, and may cover different periods of time. The method of data collection is likely to influence the accuracy of measurement. The most accurate estimates of resource consumption are likely to result from prospective studies relying on multiple sources of information and covering long periods of time.
40
Dementia
However, sources of information used in existing studies vary widely. They include interviews with patients, caregivers, and physicians either by phone or in person as well as administered or mailed questionnaires. Established questionnaires for data collection are the Resource Utilisation in Dementia (RUD) Instrument (Wimo et al. 2000) and the Client Service Receipt Inventory (Beecham and Knapp 1992). In addition, patient records or registries and medical claims databases of different payers are used for identifying costs of formal care. The study design determines whether data is collected retrospectively or prospectively, or whether both methods of data collection are used. Prospective studies have the advantage that they allow greater standardization of data collection, for example, by providing diaries to patients and caregivers in order to facilitate recording of resource consumption. In addition, they make it possible to follow patients over the course of the disease, which is an advantage as it allows estimating incidence costs of dementia (see below). Furthermore, studies differ in the time periods covered by data collection. Some studies collect data on resource use for only one month (e.g., Langa et al. 2001). Other studies track patients over a time period of up to four years (Zhu et al. 2008). Shorter time periods, are in general, more likely to produce biased estimates as the impact of extraordinary events or seasonal variation has a stronger influence than if data are collected over a longer time period. On the other hand, if patients, caregivers, or physicians have to recall resource consumption over a longer time period, it is more likely that important elements are omitted. Many studies, therefore, use several data-collection points over a long period of time in order to increase the robustness of their estimates. Cost-Estimation Technique The problem of high variation between COI studies of dementia is further compounded by differences in cost estimation techniques employed by different studies. Table 2.2 shows methodological problems in estimating costs of dementia that can be solved in different ways. COI studies adopt different approaches to the definition of costs, the measurement of resource use, and the valuation of resources. The last column in Table 2.2 presents examples of characteristics of existing studies. Defining Costs of Dementia The perspective of a study is important as it determines which cost categories have to be considered as a cost of a disease (Drummond et al.
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 41 Table 2.2 Cost-Estimation Technique and Sources of Variation between COI Studies of Dementia Methodological Problem
Source of Variation
Characteristics of Studies (examples)
Defining costs of dementia
Perspective
Society Specific payers Families Formal medical care Formal non-medical care Informal care Indirect costs Prevalence cost study Incidence cost study Estimation of total costs of demented patients Estimation of net costs of dementia Bottom-up approach Top-down approach Including all time spent with the patients Including only time spent providing assistance with ADLa and IADLb Use of national cost schedules Use of prices Use of cost study Replacement cost approach Opportunity cost approach
Inclusion of cost categories
Prevalence vs. incidence costs Objectives
Measurement of resource use
Costing approach Measurement of informal care
Valuation of used resource
Valuation of formal care
Valuation of informal care
a
Activities of daily living. Instrumental activities of daily living.
b
2005). For example, from a societal perspective, lost earnings of patients and informal caregiving time are important costs of dementia. However, from other perspectives, for example, from the perspective of Medicare, they are completely irrelevant. A study adopting a Medicare perspective would be concerned exclusively with estimating the financial costs of those resources that are covered under Medicare.
42
Dementia
Theoretically, the number of included cost categories should follow logically from the adopted perspective. A study adopting a societal perspective should include all of the following cost categories: inpatient care (IP), outpatient care (OP) (e.g., ambulatory care, hospital outpatient, emergency room, general practitioner), drugs (nootropics and others), nonmedical care (physiotherapy, nursing home care, domestic care, home help, day care, home health visits, and so on), informal care (e.g. support by family members), and indirect costs (lost productivity). However, the number of included cost categories varies considerably between studies and it is not always consistent with the stated perspective. The definition of the costs of dementia also determines whether prevalence or incidence costs are estimated. Most COI studies of dementia estimate prevalence costs, meaning that they are concerned with costs of demented patients during a specific year. Prevalence cost studies are less difficult to perform than incidence cost studies as they only look at the costs of patients during a specific year. Incidence cost studies follow patients over the course of the disease and usually aim to estimate costs from onset of the disease until death. They are more difficult since they require prospective analysis of patients over an extended period of time. However, the generated information is very useful as it can be more easily incorporated into economic evaluations of different interventions (Koopmanschap 1998). Furthermore, depending on the objectives of the study, COI studies of dementia may aim to estimate either total costs or net costs of the disease. Total-cost studies of dementia aim to estimate total costs of all care received by their sampled patients. Consequently, their results include all costs associated with the entire variety of pathologies that frequently occur in elderly populations (e.g., cardiovascular disease or cancer). Net-cost studies try to account for the fact that not all costs incurred by demented patients are actually caused by dementia (Langa et al. 2001). The ideal COI study estimating net costs of dementia would be able to appropriately identify all costs due to dementia and omit all costs attributable to other reasons (Akobundu et al. 2006). Different approaches to estimating net costs of dementia exist. However, it remains difficult to exactly identify the costs specifically due to a disease (Lee, Meyer, and Clouse 2001). Measurement of Resource Use Different costing approaches exist that allow estimating costs of a disease. Costs can be estimated either by using a bottom-up approach or a top-down approach. Most COI studies of dementia use a bottom-up approach. They
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 43
measure resource use by looking at individual services and aggregating resources until total resource use of patients has been estimated. Then monetary values are assigned. The bottom-up approach can proceed even further up and estimate national costs by multiplying estimated costs per patient with prevalence data for the number of patients in the country. Conversely, the top-down approach starts from highly aggregated data, for example, national registries and national accounts, and proceeds downwards. In the process, average resource use of patients is estimated by disaggregating total costs into smaller units and allocating them to specific diseases. The top-down approach has the advantage that it can be used to produce comparable cost estimates across different diseases (Koopmanschap 1998), facilitating comparative analyses of burden of disease. However, top-down studies are usually less detailed than bottom-up studies and do not generate information about informal care and indirect costs. Since informal care is an important cost category in COI studies of dementia, differences in measurement and valuation of informal care can have a particularly large impact on estimated results. When measuring the time requirements for informal care, some studies look at all time spent with the patient, others only include time spent providing ADL and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) assistance, while still others include informal care only up to a maximum of 16 hours per day in order to allow for at least 8 hours of sleep of caregivers. Depending on the chosen method, total estimated hours of informal caregiving time differ between studies. Valuation of Resource Use The valuation of consumed resources can have a large impact on estimated costs. It ultimately determines the monetary value of the resources that are consumed and lost due to illness. Often studies use secondary sources of information like national cost schedules in order to attach monetary values to measured resources. In other cases, market prices of used resources are determined. However, only few studies explicitly describe the assigned values. Therefore, it is often difficult to assess the appropriateness of chosen values. The valuation of informal care is even more complicated as no market prices are available that could be used as unit costs. In theory, several solutions to this problem exist (McDaid 2001). Most dementia COI studies use either the opportunity cost approach or the replacement cost approach. The opportunity cost approach tries to estimate costs of forgone opportunities by asking caregivers about how they would make use of their time if they were not engaged in the provision of informal
44
Dementia
care. Depending on their answers, caregiving time is then valued either at the market wage rate, at a rate for the contribution to household production or the rate for leisure time. The alternative replacement cost approach assigns monetary values to caregiver time according to what it would cost to replace informal caregiving activities with formal care. If the alternative approaches result in different monetary values per hour of informal caregiving time, they can have a considerable impact on estimated costs of informal care. A SYSTEMATIC LITERATURE REVIEW OF COST-OF-ILLNESS STUDIES OF DEMENTIA The previous section has illustrated the high degree of variability among COI studies of dementia that results from differences in sample selection, data collection, and cost-estimation technique. A recent systematic literature review by Quentin et al. (2010) provides an overview to methods and results of COI studies of dementia focusing on stage dependency of costs. Their findings document the high degree of variability described in the previous section. Yet, by summarizing costs for different stages of disease, their results demonstrate that estimates of existing studies show consistent patterns of increasing costs from mild over moderate to severe dementia. This section presents methods, results, and limitations of their literature review. Review Methods Quentin et al. (2010) performed a systematic literature search in Medline (Pubmed), Cochrane Library, and NHS Economic Evaluations Database (NHSEED) between May and July 2008. They searched for COI studies of dementia from Europe and North America, restricting their search to studies that appeared after 1996 since earlier studies had been included in two prior literature reviews (Wimo, Ljunggren, and Winblad 1997; Ernst and Hay 1997). Twenty-eight studies were selected that fulfilled the following criteria: The primary objective of the study was the estimation of costs of dementia; the study had a sample size greater than 20; it included at least the cost categories of formal nonmedical care or informal care; and the main outcome of the study was reported as average costs per patient and time period presented separately for different stages of disease. Studies were divided into two groups: (1) total-cost studies assessing total costs of demented individuals, and (2) net-cost studies, which tried to discern net costs of dementia (costs specifically caused by dementia). Within these groups, subgroups were formed according to the care setting
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 45
of patients included in the study. Furthermore, reported costs of different cost categories (e.g., inpatient care, outpatient care, nursing home care, family caregiving) were aggregated to the categories of formal care and informal care. When the original studies presented cost estimates for more than three stages of disease (mild, moderate, severe), presented results were assigned to the appropriate stages of dementia on the basis of reported MMSE scores (mild: ≥18, moderate: 10–17, severe: ≤9) or the original studies’ definitions. When studies presented costs for only two stages of disease, the lower estimate was classified as mild dementia and the higher estimate as severe dementia unless specified otherwise by the original studies’ authors. All cost estimates were converted into 2006 USD purchasing power parities (PPP) (OECD 2008). Review Results Study Characteristics Table 2.3 summarizes characteristics of the 28 included studies. Their region of origin was evenly distributed between Europe (n = 14) and North America (n = 14). Among European studies, most studies were from Scandinavia and the United Kingdom, while eastern and southern Europe, with the exception of Spain, were not represented in the sample. Concerning the dementia type, 21 studies looked at costs of patients with AD, of which six studies also included patients with VaD or other dementias. Two studies aimed to identify differences between costs of AD patients and VaD patients (Andersen et al. 1999; Wimo and Winblad 2003). Three studies looked at patients with “cognitive impairment,” and four studies did not specify the type of dementia. Even within the same diagnosis, definitions of dementia varied. Seventeen studies required a formal dementia diagnosis based on established criteria (e.g., National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer ’s Disease and Related Disorders Association; NINCDS-ADRDA). Seven studies required a positive screening test (e.g., MMSE) or did not specify their definition of dementia. Most studies relied on interviews to collect information on resource use by patients during a certain time period prior to the interview. Concerning patient sample characteristics, the median sample size was 337 patients, reported mean age was mostly in the late seventies, and most samples were predominantly female. Concerning cost-estimation methods, studies differed widely. Sixteen studies analyzed costs from a societal perspective, four from the perspective of specific payers, and four from the perspective of families. Most studies included several cost categories of formal care. Nineteen
46
Dementia
Table 2.3 Characteristics of Included COI Studies of Dementia (N = 28) Characteristic Region: N (%) United States Canada Europe Sample Size Median (range) Mean Included Diagnoses: N (%) (Multiple diagnoses possible) Alzheimer’s disease Vascular dementia Other dementias Dementia (unspecified) Cognitive impairment Diagnostic Criteria: N (%) Formal diagnoses (e.g. according to NINCDS-ADRDAa) Physician diagnosis (unspecified) Other (e.g., MMSE) / not described Method for Data Collection / Data source: N (%) (Multiple methods possible) Interview Questionnaire Patient records Medical claims Not described Adopted Perspective: N (%) (Multiple perspectives possible) Society Payers Families Not described Included Cost Categories: N (%) (Multiple categories possible) Formal nonmedical care Ambulatory care Inpatient care Informal care Drugs Study Objectives Total costs Net costs Both
11 (39%) 3 (11%) 14 (50%) 337 (50 – 8.736) 880
21 (75%) 6 (21%) 4 (14%) 4 (14%) 3 (11%) 17 (61%) 4 (14%) 7 (25%)
21 (75%) 4 (14%) 3 (11%) 3 (11%) 1 (4%)
16 (57%) 5 (18%) 4 (14%) 4 (14%)
23 (82%) 20 (71%) 19 (68%) 19 (68%) 19 (68%) 4 (14%) 5 (18%)
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 47 Table 2.3 (Continued) Funding Source Pharmaceutical companies Public funds Other (e.g. Alzheimer’s Association)/not described
16 (57%) 6 (21%) 6 (21%)
Source: Adapted from König et al., Stadienspezifische Kosten der Demenz: Ergebnisse eines systematischen Literaturuberblicks, in Versonrgungsforschung fur demenziell erkrankte Menschen, ed. Dibelius and Maier, with permission. a National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke– Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association.
studies estimated cost of informal care. In addition, studies pursued different objectives: 19 studies looked at total costs of demented patients while 9 others tried to estimate net costs of dementia or looked at both total and net costs of dementia. When looking at the sources of funding, most studies included financial contributions from pharmaceutical companies but several studies were financed exclusively through public funds. More information on the methods for valuation of informal care is provided in the original review article (Quentin et al. 2010). Estimated Costs Table 2.4 summarizes results of analyzed studies. It presents estimated costs from total-cost studies and net-cost studies separately for patients living in different care settings, and it further differentiates between costs for formal and informal care. The table shows the range of estimated average costs from analyzed studies and the median of their results. At first sight, the large range of estimated costs in each field might appear confusing. However, after close examination, consistent patterns of increasing costs from mild over moderate to severe dementia emerge. In most total-cost studies, costs more than double from mild to severe dementia. For example, Table 2.4 shows that the range of average total costs estimated by different studies for community-dwelling patients in the mild stage of dementia was between US$7,124 and $56,192, whereas average total costs reported for patients in the severe stage were between $8,544 and $115,450. Accordingly, the median of estimated results reported in different studies increased from about $18,000 to about $36,000. In net-cost studies, the increase of costs was even more pronounced: The median of estimated costs for community-dwelling patients increased from about $18,000 to about $63,000. However, in general, costs estimated in net-cost studies tended to be smaller than costs estimated in comparable total-cost studies.
48
Dementia
Table 2.4 Results of Included COI Studies of Dementia by Study Objectives, Level of Severity, Care Setting, and Cost Category Estimated Costs per Year in USD-PPP (2006) Number of studies Range of costs [Median] Care Setting
Cost Category
Mild Dementia
Moderate Dementia
Severe Dementia
Total Cost Studies (24 studies) Communitydwelling patients
Formal care costs
n=9 1,268–14,388 [6,341]
n=6 5,234–32,283 [9,837]
n=9 4,255–36,428 [13,649]
n=8 783–41,677 [14,212]
n=6 615–54,544 [15,937]
n=8 1,225–78,504 [36,354]
Total costs
n=7 7,124–56,192 [18,041]
n=7 5,849–87,057 [25,331]
n=8 8,544–115,450 [36,354]
Formal care costs
n = 13 2,255–29,127 [10,261]
n = 11 7,592–34,897 [21,537]
n = 13 11,764–56,118 [29,549]
Informal care costs
n=6 3,932–13,039 [5,841]
n=5 4,050–9,001 [7,496]
n=6 2,392–14,261 [8,071]
Total costs
n=8 3,992–35,338 [12,340]
n=7 16,462–34,264 [25,492]
n=8 21,977–74,555 [38,204]
Formal care cost
n=2 n=2 21,990–42,319 28,665–44,941 [31,640] [36.803]
n=2 35,518–48,162 [41,840]
Informal care costs
Mixed group of patients
Institutionalized patients
Informal care costs Total costs
n=2 1,028–1,875 [1,452]
n=2 790–3,576 [2,183]
n=2 n=2 23,865–42,319 32,240–45,731 [33,092] [38,986]
n=2 834–2,306 [1,570] n=2 37,825–48,994 [43,410]
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 49 Table 2.4 (Continued) Net Cost Studies (9 Studies) Communitydwelling patients
Formal care costs
n=7 1,007–11,143 [3,330]
n=7 166–28,996 [7,208]
n=7 986–33,285 [9,816]
n=8 197–41,677 [5,647]
n=8 29–54,544 [12,042]
n=8 639–78,504 [33,103]
Total costs
n=7 1,470–52,947 [18,388]
n=7 195–83,770 [28,767]
n=7 2,890–112,307 [62,916]
Formal care costs
n=3 2,553–11,720 [8,373]
n=2 13,433–18,567 [16,000]
n=3 4,595–27,359 [26,052]
Informal care costs
Mixed group of patientsa
Source: Adapted from König et al., Stadienspezifische Kosten der Demenz: Ergebnisse eines systematischen Literaturuberblicks, in Versonrgungsforschung fur demenziell erkrankte Menschen, ed. Dibelius and Maier, with permission. Note: USD-PPP = US$–purchasing power parities. a Studies only report costs of formal care.
Furthermore, the patterns of increasing costs and the proportion of formal and informal care differ between care settings. In mild dementia, totalcost studies of patients living in institutional-care settings found higher costs (about US$33,000) than total-cost studies of community-dwelling patients (about $18,000). In contrast, in severe dementia, estimated costs of patients living in different care settings were similar. In studies analyzing samples of community-dwelling patients, costs increased mostly for informal care and accounted for about two-thirds of costs in the severe stage. In studies including community-dwelling and institutionalized patients (mixed group), costs for formal care increased more strongly from mild to severe dementia, which could be attributed to increasing proportions of institutionalized patients from mild to severe dementia in this group of studies. In studies of institutionalized patients, the proportion of informalcare costs remained relatively small across all stages of dementia. Limitations Reported results vary considerably even within the presented subgroups of studies and within the cost categories of formal and informal
50
Dementia
care. On the one hand, these variations are related to limitations of the original studies. For example, some studies excluded certain cost categories or excluded patients with severe comorbidities; other studies used extremely high values for the valuation of informal care. On the other hand, variations are related to limitations of the review method, which had to ignore certain differences between studies in order to be able to compare their results. The review formed subgroups according to the care setting of patients and aggregated various cost categories into the categories of formal and informal care. However, studies differed considerably within subgroups. The samples of patients analyzed in the original studies varied greatly: Some studies included “dementia patients”; others recruited only AD patients. Studies used various methods to classify patients into different stages of disease; and informal-care costs were estimated through very different approaches. These limitations need to be considered when drawing on the results of the review by Quentin et al. (2010). However, despite all methodological differences between studies and variation of results, the patterns of increasing costs are clear enough to allow drawing conclusions for the effect of neuropsychiatric symptoms on costs of dementia. COST-OF-ILLNESS OF DEMENTIA: THE ROLE OF NEUROPSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMS Two findings of the presented literature review are particularly important for a discussion about the role of neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in determining resource use and costs of dementia: First, the review shows that informal care accounts for an important part of total resources used and constitutes the majority of costs of dementia care in communitydwelling patients; second, it indicates that costs of institutionalization are considerable already in early stages of dementia. The implications of these findings for costs of NPS can be explored by looking at existing studies of the importance of NPS in determining caregiver burden and nursing home placement. In addition, some studies exist that have specifically looked at the effect of NPS on cost-of-illness of dementia. Caregiver Burden and Determinants of Nursing Home Placement: The Role of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms Multiple studies have assessed the role of neuropsychiatric symptoms in determining caregiver burden and nursing home placement. Almost 30 years ago, Greene et al. (1982) found behavioral problems to be important
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 51
determinants of caregiver burden. More recently, these findings were confirmed by studies that looked at the specific type of neuropsychiatric symptom with the highest impact on caregiver burden as measured by standardized interviews of caregivers. For example, Craig et al. (2005) have found sleep disturbance, aggression/agitation, and depression/ dysphoria to be particularly distressing to caregivers. Another stream of research has looked at the time spent by caregivers on dealing with NPS of dementia. Beeri et al. (2002) found that caregivers spent about one-third of their time with managing behavioral and psychiatric symptoms. And Murman et al. (2002) found that patients with NPS required 3.5 hours more of caregiving per day than patients without these symptoms. Nursing home placement is the result of a complex set of interacting variables, including caregiver, patient, social, and cultural variables (Yaffe et al. 2002). However, high caregiver burden and presence of NPS of dementia are two particularly important factors influencing the likeliness of patients to be admitted to nursing homes. Even when controlling for other factors, Yaffe et al. (2002) found caregiver burden and age over 75 years to be the most important determinants of nursing home placement. Phillips and Diwan (2003) estimated that patients with NPS enter nursing homes nearly two years earlier than those without. These findings combined with the results of the literature review by Quentin et al. (2010) suggest that NPS are important determinants of costs of care for demented patients. Informal-care costs account for the majority of costs in community-dwelling patients and NPS are important determinants of caregiver burden. Consequently, total costs of care related to NPS of dementia can be expected to be considerable. In addition, neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia contribute to increasing costs by leading to earlier nursing home placement. Since costs of nursing home placement are important even in the early stages of disease, the impact on total costs is likely to be important. COI Studies Focusing on Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia A small number of studies have looked specifically at the contribution of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia on costs of dementia care. Table 2.5 provides an overview to the characteristics of two of these studies, which compared groups of patients with different levels of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia. Both studies are from North America and estimated total costs of dementia care of a sample of patients which they divided into two groups: one group with a low score on the neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI) and one group with a high NPI score. Herrmann
52
Dementia
Table 2.5 Characteristics of COI Studies Focusing on Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia Author (year) General study characteristics Country Dementia type Method for data collection Sample characteristics Size Mean age % receiving long-term care (patients with low/high NPI) Funding source Perspective Objectives Included cost categories Inpatient care Outpatient care Drugs Nonmedical Informal Indirect Valuation of informal care Unit cost value (in 2006 US$-PPP)
Herrmann et al. 2006
Murman et al. 2002
Canada Dementia (AD/VaD/other) Mailed questionnaire
USA Alzheimer ’s disease Interview
500 76.3 0%
128 76.2 (12%/27%)
Industry Society Total cost study
National Institute on Aging n/d (Society) Total cost study
+ + + + + +
+ + + + + -
98.01/day
5.82/hr, 9.02/hr, 10.68/hr (low, mid-range, high estimate)
Note: AD = Alzheimer ’s disease; n/d = not described; NPI = neuropsychiatric inventory; VaD = vascular dementia.
et al. (2006) studied patients living in the community, whereas Murman et al. (2002) included a certain number of patients receiving long term care. Both included a wide range of formal and informal care costs. In addition, Herrmann et al. (2006) included indirect costs by estimating the opportunity costs of the time that patients were prevented from performing their regular activities. Table 2.6 presents the results of the two COI studies focusing on neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia. It is important to note that the studies estimated costs for disparate groups of patients. Herrmann et al. (2006)
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 53 Table 2.6 Results of COI Studies Focusing on Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia Mean annual costs per patient (in 2006 USD-PPP) Patients with low NPI Author (year) Herrmann et al. 2006 Murman et al. 2002
Patients with high NPI
DefiniDefinition of tion of Group Formal Informal Total Group Formal Informal Total NPI = 0 2,479
2,098
NPI <13 10,074 13,039
7,091
NPI >0
4,255
4,877
16,119
23,113 NPI ≥13 20,485 14,261 44,993
Note: NPI = neuropsychiatric inventory.
defined their “low NPI group” as patients with no neuropsychiatric symptoms, whereas the “low NPI group” of Murman et al. (2002) included all patients with an NPI score less than 13. Both studies found that costs of patients in the high NPI group were about twice as high as costs in the low NPI group. However, the size of costs varied considerably, which is not particularly surprising as the assessed groups of patients were very different. Both studies also used regression analyses in order to determine the effect of a one-point increase in NPI score on total costs of dementia care. Herrmann et al. (2006) estimated that costs would increase by US$346 for every one-point increase in NPI, while Murman et al. (2002) estimated the effect to lie somewhere between US$281 and $466 depending on the valuation of informal care (all in 2006 US$-PPP). The estimated effect of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia on costs, therefore, appears to be similar. However, it has to be considered that the estimate of Herrmann et al. (2006) includes indirect costs (the value of the time that patients were prevented from performing their regular activities), which they found to be considerable, whereas Murman et al. (2002) did not include this cost category. Presumably, further differences existed in the measurement and valuation of informal care, which are, however, difficult to evaluate. CONCLUSION This chapter has described the theoretical background and methodological characteristics of COI studies of dementia. It has highlighted the importance of considering methodological differences between studies when interpreting their results. Different approaches to sample selection, data collection, definition of costs, and measurement and valuation
54
Dementia
of resources determine the size of estimated costs in COI studies of dementia. Users of results of COI studies should be careful with generalizing results of COI studies since they are always specific to the study setting, the sample of patients, and the cost-estimation techniques. However, if these methodological characteristics are considered, estimated results can be useful for determining the impact of dementia on specific aspects of health systems and societies. The literature review by Quentin et al. (2010) demonstrated that costs increase from mild over moderate to severe dementia and that costs differ by care setting and depend on whether total or net costs of dementia are estimated. Informal-care costs were confirmed to account for the majority of costs in community-dwelling patients and costs of patients living in institutional-care settings were important already in the early stages of disease. Since neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia are an important determinant of caregiver burden and nursing home placement, their impact on costs of dementia is considerable. Existing studies have found that costs of patients with neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia are much higher than costs of patients without these symptoms. However, again, methodological differences between studies complicate comparisons of their results. An international consensus statement exists that defines how to measure benefits in dementia treatment trials (Katona et al. 2007), and reference cases have been developed for the presentation of cost-effectiveness results (NICE 2008). A similar international consensus on a dementia-specific reference case for conducting COI studies of dementia and presenting their results could improve quality of studies, facilitate comparisons, and increase certainty that results are reliable and transferable to other settings. ACKNOWLEDGMENT: This publication is part of the German Research Network on Dementia (KND) and the German Research Network on Degenerative Dementia (KNDD) and was funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (grants KND: 01GI0102, 01GI0420, 01GI0422, 01GI0423, 01GI0429, 01GI0431, 01GI0433, 01GI0434; grants KNDD: O1GI0710, 01GI0711, 01GI0712, 01GI0713, 01GI0714, 01GI0715, 01GI0716). REFERENCES Akobundu, E., J. Ju, L. Blatt, and C. D. Mullins. 2006. Cost-of-illness studies: A review of current methods. Pharmacoeconomics 24 (9): 869–890. Andersen, C. K., J. Sogaard, E. Hansen, A. Kragh-Sorensen, L. Hastrup, J. Andersen, K. Andersen, A. Lolk, H. Nielsen, and P. Kragh-Sorensen. 1999. The cost of dementia in Denmark: The Odense Study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10 (4): 295–304.
Cost of Illness Studies and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Dementia 55 Beecham, J., and M. Knapp. 1992. Costing psychiatric interventions. In Measuring Mental Health Needs, ed. G. Thornicroft, C. R. Brewin, and J. K. Wing, 179–190. London: Gaskell. Beeri, M. S., P. Werner, M. Davidson, and S. Noy. 2002. The cost of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) in community dwelling Alzheimer ’s disease patients. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 17 (5): 403–408. Bloom, B. S., D. J. Bruno, D. Y. Maman, and R. Jayadevappa. 2001. Usefulness of US cost-of-illness studies in healthcare decision making. Pharmacoeconomics 19 (2): 207–213. Bynum, J. P., P. V. Rabins, W. Weller, M. Niefeld, G. F. Anderson, and A. W. Wu. 2004. The relationship between a dementia diagnosis, chronic illness, Medicare expenditures, and hospital use. J Am Geriatr Soc 52 (2): 187–194. Craig, D., A. Mirakhur, D. J. Hart, S. P. McIlroy, and A. P. Passmore. 2005. A cross-sectional study of neuropsychiatric symptoms in 435 patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13 (6): 460–468. Drummond, M. F., M. J, Sculpher, G, W. Torrance, B. J. O’Brien, and G. L. Stoddart. 2005. Cost analysis. In Methods for the Economic Evaluation of Health Care Programmes, 3rd ed., 49–102. New York: Oxford University Press. Ernst, R. L., and J. W. Hay. 1997. Economic research on Alzheimer disease: A review of the literature. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 11 (Suppl 6): 135–145. Folland, S., A, C. Goodman, and M. Stano. 2007. Introduction. In The Economics of Health and Health Care, 5th ed., 1–19. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice-Hall. Greene, J. G., R. Smith, M. Gardiner, and G. C. Timbury. 1982. Measuring behavioural disturbance of elderly demented patients in the community and its effects on relatives: A factor analytic study. Age Ageing 11 (2): 121–126. Harrow, B. S., D. F. Mahoney, A. B. Mendelsohn, M. G. Ory, D. W. Coon, S. H. Belle, and L. O. Nichols. 2004. Variation in cost of informal caregiving and formalservice use for people with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 19 (5): 299–308. Herrmann, N., K. L. Lanctot, R. Sambrook, N. Lesnikova, R. Hebert, P. McCracken, A. Robillard, and E. Nguyen. 2006. The contribution of neuropsychiatric symptoms to the cost of dementia care. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 21 (10): 972–976. Hill, J., H. Fillit, S. K. Thomas, and S. Chang. 2006. Functional impairment, healthcare costs and the prevalence of institutionalisation in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease and other dementias. Pharmacoeconomics 24 (3): 265–280. Kane, R. L., and A. Atherly. 2000. Medicare expenditures associated with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 14 (4): 187–195. Katona, C., G. Livingston, C. Cooper, D. Ames, H. Brodaty, and E. Chiu. 2007. International Psychogeriatric Association consensus statement on defining and measuring treatment benefits in dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 19 (3): 345–354. Koopmanschap, M. A. 1998. Cost-of-illness studies. Useful for health policy? Pharmacoeconomics 14 (2): 143–148.
56
Dementia
Langa, K. M., M. E. Chernew, M. U. Kabeto, A. R. Herzog, M. B. Ofstedal, R. J. Willis, R. B. Wallace, L. M. Mucha, W. L. Straus, and A. M. Fendrick. 2001. National estimates of the quantity and cost of informal caregiving for the elderly with dementia. J Gen Intern Med 16 (11): 770–778. Lee, D. W., J. W. Meyer, and J. Clouse. 2001. Implications of controlling for comorbid conditions in cost-of-illness estimates: A case study of osteoarthritis from a managed care system perspective. Value Health 4 (4): 329–334. McDaid, D. 2001. Estimating the costs of informal care for people with Alzheimer ’s disease: Methodological and practical challenges. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 16 (4): 400–405. Moore, M. J., C. W. Zhu, and E. C. Clipp. 2001. Informal costs of dementia care: Estimates from the National Longitudinal Caregiver Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 56 (4): S219–228. Murman, D. L., Q. Chen, M. C. Powell, S. B. Kuo, C. J. Bradley, and C. C. Colenda. 2002. The incremental direct costs associated with behavioral symptoms in AD. Neurology 59 (11): 1721–1729. NICE. 2008. Guide to the methods of technology appraisal. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE). OECD. 2008. Health Data 2008: Statistics and Indicators for 30 Countries. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). Phillips, V. L., and S. Diwan. 2003. The incremental effect of dementia-related problem behaviors on the time to nursing home placement in poor, frail, demented older people. J Am Geriatr Soc 51 (2): 188–193. Quentin, W., S. G. Riedel-Heller, M. Luppa, A. Rudolph, and H. H. Konig. 2010. Cost-of-illness studies of dementia: A systematic review focusing on stage dependency of costs. Acta Psychiatr Scand 121 (4): 243–259. Wimo, A., L. Jonsson, and B. Winblad. 2006. An estimate of the worldwide prevalence and direct costs of dementia in 2003. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 21 (3): 175–181. Wimo, A., G. Ljunggren, and B. Winblad. 1997. Costs of dementia and dementia care: A review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12 (8): 841–856. Wimo, A., G. Nordberg, W. Jansson, and M. Grafstrom. 2000. Assessment of informal services to demented people with the RUD instrument. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15 (10): 969–971. Wimo, A., and B. Winblad. 2003. Societal burden and economics of vascular dementia: Preliminary results from a Swedish-population-based study. Int Psychogeriatr 15 (Suppl 1): 251–256. Yaffe, K., P. Fox, R. Newcomer, L. Sands, K. Lindquist, K. Dane, and K. E. Covinsky. 2002. Patient and caregiver characteristics and nursing home placement in patients with dementia. JAMA 287 (16): 2090–2097. Zhu, C. W., R. Torgan, N. Scarmeas, M. Albert, J. Brandt, D. Blacker, M. Sano, and Y. Stern. 2008. Home health and informal care utilization and costs over time in Alzheimer ’s disease. Home Health Care Serv Q 27 (1): 1–20.
Chapter 3
A Stroke of Bad Luck: CADASIL and Friedrich Nietzsche’s “Dementia” or Madness Paul M. Butler
Philosophy is its own time raised to the level of thought. G. W. F. Hegel (1821/1991) The early eighteenth-century Prussian philosopher Georg Wilhelm Friedrich Hegel envisaged history as a dynamic, dialectic system—an ineluctable process of unfolding epochs. Reason, seen as the highest form of human cognition, is forever ensconced within these historical movements. Regardless of the absolute veridicality of Hegel’s thought, he proposes an interesting way to think about reason and history (Hegel 1837/1997). Logical analysis is restricted to comprehending its object using the tools available within a given stage of history. Likewise, science and the art of medicine are bound by the limits of historically constrained reason. This is well illustrated by pondering the vastly different ways human reason has apprehended natural reality across time (i.e., from the Ptolemaic epicyles to Steven Hawking’s arrow of time or Aelius Galen’s circulatory model to William Harvey’s heart-pump model). Medical diagnoses will always only be as accurate as the categories that contain them. Take, for instance, the history of stroke and the diagnosis of “softening of the brain” in the early 1800s. In 1814 Jean Andre Rochoux maintained that apoplexy or ramollissement (softening) of the brain was exclusively the result of hemorrhagic bleeding. No other categories or
58
Dementia
ways of thinking about brain lesions existed at this time in medical history. It was not until 1823, when Leon Rostan proposed that ossification of the arteries was associated with parenchymous brain lesions that the concept of brain softening was divided into hemorrhagic and occlusive-based lesions (Paciaroni and Bogousslavsky 2009). And so it is today in clinical neurology; our reason is trapped in history. Of course, multitudinous diagnostic categories now exist as tools for the practicing physician compared to just 100 years ago, but pathologic etiologies yet to be discovered still hover beyond our conceptual grasp. What was seen as “softening of the brain” in 1810 and diagnosed as an embolic stroke due to atrial fibrillation in 2010 will be phrased in a molecular cardiac basis (an undiscovered channelopathy?) in the future. With this line of thought in mind, I introduce here a potential diagnosis unknown to physicians of the late nineteenth century to explain the dementia of Friedrich Nietzsche. When Nietzsche was brought to Dr. Otto Binswanger ’s clinic in Jena, Switzerland, in January 1889, the diagnostic category most fitting to explain his sudden onset of bizarre ideas, grandiosity, dementia, and apparent Argyll-Robertson pupils was paresis paralytica (tertiary syphilis) (Volz 1990). This diagnosis was very reasonable at that time in medical history because dementia presenting in a middle-aged adult male was nearly always due to syphilitic infection. So what physicians saw in 1889 as syphilitic paresis is ascertained, at least in my estimation, as genetically testable CADASIL (cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy) with dementia and perhaps, in the indeterminate future, reason will diagnostically supplant our present-day diagnosis with a new category, such as Type II CADASIL, endophenotype 10b with features of epigenetic modulation (for want of a better imaginary example). OVERVIEW OF NIETZSCHE’S CASE PRESENTATION For my life’s terrible and almost unremitting martyrdom makes me thirst for the end, and there have been some signs which allow me to hope that the stroke which will liberate me is not too distant. Nietzsche (1880/1996) I conducted a comprehensive review of Nietzsche’s medical records and over 500 letters written by Nietzsche (see Nietzsche 1971, 1985, 1996; Frenzel 1967; Volz 1990). Translations from primary sources reveal that he suffered from shifting headaches beginning in adolescence. Further, this evidence clearly demonstrates that Nietzsche was specifically tormented
A Stroke of Bad Luck
59
by migraine with aura and with motor symptoms giving him an IHS classification of (familial, vide infra) hemiplegic migraine: IHS1.2.4 (International Headache Society 2004). Nietzsche’s headaches emerged during his mid-twenties, peaked in his thirties, and attenuated in his forties. They were trigger-sensitive to weather, travel, and dietary habits and exhibited prolonged duration, photosensitivity, emesis, gastrointestinal pain, and fatigue. Medical records and correspondence demonstrate these headache attacks occurred for many hours, often causing tonic eye cramps and reversible paralysis of the right oculomotor muscles. His right eyelid would droop and his gaze would move into the right lower temporal quadrant. Dizziness, disturbance of speech, temporary loss of consciousness, facial hemi-paralysis, and feelings of numbness accompanied the headaches. Nietzsche described a “flickering in front of the eye” and “numerous danger signals” prior to the onset of these attacks (Volz 1990). Nietzsche’s correspondence also suggests mood disorder. He entertained thoughts of death, experiencing profound depression during the 1870s and early 1880s with an emergent hypomania in the early to mid1880s. There is a sudden progression to fulminant mania by the time of his mental collapse at the end of 1888. Nietzsche’s intense depression alternated with euphoric moods. He wrote letters, within the same month to the same individual, stating that he “hungered for the end” believing that “the cerebral coup de grace is close enough at hand.” Just days later he wrote, “My joyous thirst for knowledge brings me to heights where I can triumph over all torment and despair. On the whole I’m happier than ever before in my life.” In an explosion of 10 days of racing thoughts and flights of ideas during the winter of 1882, Nietzsche produced book 1 of Thus Spake Zarathustra. Just months later, he wrote book 2 over another 10-day stretch of prolonged creativity. For a relatively isolated individual, Nietzsche exhibited pressured speech and loose concatenations of thought in a written form known clinically as hypergraphia. A progressive and predominately right-sided retinal inflammation was noted throughout his life in medical records, in addition to a rapidly developing myopia and eventual blindness. Several physicians commented on this “mysterious inflammatory mechanism” that caused “light granulations” and “retinitis pigmentosa.” In 1884 Nietzsche reported a sudden change in vision not connected with headache that likely reflected a stroke-induced Charles Bonnet syndrome. He described “blots, veils, and darkening” in his visual fields as an “opacification.” With his eyes closed Nietzsche experienced hallucinations filled with fantastic colorful flowers that flowed and changed in brilliant displays of movement. A sudden change in Nietzsche’s mental status occurred during the final week of 1888.
60
Dementia
He sent off cryptic letters signed “The Anti-Christ” and “The Man on the Cross.” This was followed by multiple nonresolving ischemic-like attacks ultimately causing subcortical dementia initially presenting as a pseudobulbar palsy, gait disturbance, and emotional lability. Nietzsche developed a labile affect, tearful outbursts and inappropriate laughter, dysfunction in cranial nerves IX-XII—Binswanger noted tongue deviation to the right and spasmodic left shoulder jerking—in addition to left body upper motor neuron dysfunction evidenced by left ankle clonus and an exaggerated patellar reflex. Under full-time familial care, Nietzsche slowly became bedridden and demented, and died in 1900 after suffering several strokes.
NIETZSCHE’S CASE DETAILS Family History Because of Nietzsche’s fame, his family medical history is partly known. Nietzsche was born in 1844 to an already sick father, Karl Ludwig Nietzsche. In brief, Nietzsche’s melancholic father also experienced intense headaches from adolescence until death at age 35. Beginning in his thirties, he dealt with increasingly labile mood, epileptic-like fits, and multiple strokes leading to facial hemi-paralysis, blindness, and eventual dementia and death. Autopsy revealed a “softening of the brain” affecting 25% of brain tissue. Additionally, records state that Karl’s father, Friedrich August Ludwig Nietzsche, suffered from similar symptoms. So three successive paternal generations, Friedrich August, Karl Ludwig, and Friedrich Nietzsche, suffered from similar symptoms and untimely deaths.
Nietzsche’s Health from 1844 to 1888 Ocular Disturbances Nietzsche struggled with his vision throughout life and saw many eye doctors. An ophthalmologist, Dr. Vater, diagnosed Nietzsche with myopia in early childhood. Dr. Schelbach examined Nietzsche’s vision at age five and noted congenital differences in Nietzsche’s pupils (anisocoria). The right pupil was abnormally shaped, significantly larger than the left, and reacted slowly to light. Dr. Schelbach’s records note the young Nietzsche’s mother, Franziska, also had an uneven right pupil slow to react to light. Even though the right eye was congenitally worse, Nietzsche still needed eye correction for both eyes. His vision deteriorated from early
A Stroke of Bad Luck
61
childhood at varying rates until eventual blindness occurred before death. In 1873 Dr. Schiesse again noted the right pupillary shape disparity in addition to small pigment granulations on the retina of both sides. He diagnosed “retinitis pigmentosa” in both eyes and “strabismus convergens” in the right eye. In 1877 Dr. Kruger also noticed an inflammatory process present in both retinas that seemed radically changed from previous years. He diagnosed this inflammation as “chorio-retinitis centralis.” He further observed retinal exudates that were spreading toward the macula of both eyes. In January 1884 Nietzsche reported a sudden change in vision not connected with headache. He experienced “blots, veils, and darkening” in his visual fields, which he described in one word as “opacification.” Resa Schirnhofer, a friend of Nietzsche, visited him in 1884 during this abrupt change in vision, and later wrote that he saw fantastic colors after closing his eyes—colorful flowers that flowed and changed in brilliant displays of movement. Hallucinations result from many potential causes, such as schizophrenia, temporary psychosis, and Parkinson’s disease, but the relationship between vision loss and hallucinatory experiences in Nietzsche suggests Charles Bonnet syndrome (CBS). CBS is characterized by vivid, elaborate, and recurrent visual hallucinations in the absence of external stimuli in individuals with preserved intellectual functioning. Associated with anomalies at any juncture of the visual system, the condition can be episodic, periodic, or chronic. Macular degeneration and stroke-induced lesion in the visual system accounts for approximately 75–85% of CBS cases. Headaches As early as 11 years old, Nietzsche missed school due to headaches and eye pain. His letters suggest that he began to suffer from increasingly frequent and intense headache attacks throughout his 20s and 30s. Sometime between 1870 and 1871 he began to suffer from migrainous attacks. The situation became progressively worse over the next decade. He complains in a letter to his colleague and friend Carl von Gersdorff, who Nietzsche had befriended at Schulpforta, “I’ve been through a very bad time, and there may be an even worse one to come. My stomach could no longer be tamed, even with an absurdly strict diet. Chronic headaches of the fiercest sort, which lasted for days. Vomiting on an empty stomach, for hours on end. In short the machine seemed to want to disintegrate, and I won’t deny having wished several times that it would do just that. Great fatigue, difficulty getting about, hypersensitivity to light . . .” (Nietzsche 1971). Writing to Dr. Otto Eiser in January of 1880 he
62
Dementia
asserts, “And yet!—constant pain, a feeling much like seasickness several hours each day, a semi-paralysis which makes speaking difficult and, for a change of pace, furious seizures (the last involved three days and nights of vomiting; I lusted for death)” (Nietzsche 1971). Nietzsche registered several important somatic complaints that are consistent with a diagnosis of migraine with aura (MA). His MA attacks involved fully reversible homonymous visual symptoms, unilateral sensory loss, and dysphasic speech. His headaches were prolonged and accompanied by photosensitivity, emesis, gastrointestinal pain, and fatigue. Medical evidence suggests that individuals more susceptible to triggers are likely to have headaches with a longer history of duration, more premonitory symptoms, throbbing, pressure, stabbing, nausea, photophobia, rhinorrhea, tearing of eyes, and higher headache frequency. These individuals are likely to choose rest during headaches and suffer from anxiety, depression, mood swings, and general pain. Nietzsche’s MA attacks match well with this picture. His MAs lessened in the 1880s but returned suddenly in the summer 1888. Mood Disorders Nietzsche’s correspondence also supports a diagnosis of major depression during the 1870s and early 1880s with an emergent hypomania in the early to mid-1880s. Just prior to the expression of subcortical dementia at the end of 1888 and beginning of 1889, Nietzsche’s behavior shifted from hypomanic to manic and was accompanied by a psychotic break from reality. In the early 1880s, Nietzsche reached the nadir of his depression. His writings were not being acknowledged for their brilliance, plans of love and marriage with the young Russian Lou Salomé ended abruptly, and he distanced himself from his mother and sister. In August of 1883 he wrote, “I am now working like a man who is ‘putting his house in order before departing.’” Nietzsche’s intense depression began to alternate with a mood of euphoria. Beginning in the 1870s, Nietzsche’s struggle with melancholic moods and suicidal ideation became apparent to his closest friends and colleagues. In 1877 Nietzsche writes to Malwida von Meysenbug, a close friend he met through his prior friendship with Richard Wagner, “on the ship I had only the blackest thoughts, my only doubts about suicide concerned where the sea might be deepest, so that one would not be immediately fished out again and have to pay a debt of gratitude to one’s rescuers in a terrible mass of gold—sometimes such a feeling of emptiness comes over me that I want to scream” (Nietzsche 1996).
A Stroke of Bad Luck
63
In March 1883 he writes, “My dear friend—I’ve lost interest in everything. Deep down, an unyielding black melancholy. And weariness too. Most of the time I’m in bed. I’ve suffered too much and sacrificed too much; I feel so incomplete, so inexpressibly conscious of having bungled and botched my whole creative life. It’s all hopeless. I won’t do anything worthwhile again. Why do anything anymore!” (Nietzsche 1971). Just a year later he writes an ecstatic letter to Peter Gast asserting, Thoughts have emerged on my horizon the likes of which I’ve never seen—I won’t even hint at what they are, but shall maintain my own unshakeable calm. I suppose now I’ll have to live a few years longer! Ah, my friend, I sometimes think that I lead a highly dangerous life, since I’m one of those machines that can burst apart. The intensity of my feelings makes me shudder and laugh. Several times I have been unable to leave my room, for the ridiculous reason that my eyes were inflamed. Why? Because I’d cried too much on my wanderings the day before. Not sentimental tears, mind you, but tears of joy, to the accompaniment of which I sang and talked nonsense, filled with a new vision far superior to that of other men. (Nietzsche, 1971) In an explosion of ten days of racing thoughts and flight of ideas during the winter of 1882, Nietzsche produced Book One of Thus Spake Zarathustra. Just months later, he wrote Book Two over another ten-day stretch of prolonged creativity (Nietzsche 1971). While the evidence from his letters first suggests a diagnosis of major depression, as time elapsed his depressive episodes gave rise to cycling moods consistent with bipolar. For a relatively isolated individual, the pressured speech and loose concatenations of thought were expressed in written form, known as hypergraphia. Nietzsche’s Health from 1888 to 1900 In late December 1888 Nietzsche experienced a profound change in mental status. In early January 1889 he allegedly collapsed while in public. Nietzsche was brought to Dr. Ludwig Wille’s psychiatric clinic near the Alsatian border for three days. He was an unruly patient, bursting into song or scream at any moment and demonstrating constant motor agitation. His gait was stumbling and not steady, and he seemed stiff at times. Next, Nietzsche was moved to Basel, where the patient records state, “Pupillary disparity, right larger than left, reaction sluggish. Convergent strabismus-acute myopia. Tongue heavily furred, no deviation, no tremor! Facial nerve almost normal; right nasolabial fold slightly contracted. Exaggerated patellar reflex; plantar reflexes normal” (Volz 1990).
64
Dementia
Within a few days Nietzsche was moved to a clinic in Jena that was managed by a well-known physician, Dr. Otto Binswanger (who first described Binswanger ’s disease, coined by Alois Alzheimer). Both Wille and Binswanger diagnosed Nietzsche with paralytica progressiva due to syphilis. With this assessment, both doctors assumed Nietzsche only had two years to live. Dr. Binswanger ’s medical notes on Nietzsche give, Pupils right wide, left rather narrower, left contracted with slight irregularity, all reactions normal on left, on right only reaction to convergence, consensual reactions only on left . . . symmetrical smile, tongue non-tremulous with deviations to right . . . Romberg negative . . . screws left shoulder up spasmodically when walking . . . slight ankle clonus on left . . . head percussion not sensitive, facial nerves sensitive. After several weeks, Nietzsche seemed to improve with no further deterioration. His mother surreptitiously removed him from the clinic to bring him home to care for him full-time. By 1893 he was completely bedridden and retained no memories of his life as a writer. During 1898 and 1899, Nietzsche suffered at least two more strokes that left him unable to speak or walk. On August 24, 1900, Nietzsche died either from another stroke or a pneumonia-like infection. Nietzsche was buried alongside his father ’s grave next to the parsonage in Röcken, Germany. DIAGNOSIS AND DISCUSSION Table 3.1 displays a historical list of diagnoses in the literature posited to explain Nietzsche’s condition. Following the emergence of new diagnostic categories since 1889, novel diagnostic possibilities now exist to explain Nietzsche’s constellation of symptoms. Nietzsche’s persistent medical issues are explainable with one unifying, genetically testable diagnosis: cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL). The diagnosis of CADASIL explains Friedrich’s, Karl Ludwig’s, and Friedrich August Nietzsche’s condition. In this section I present our diagnosis and consider competing ideas. CADASIL is likely the cause of Nietzsche’s illness. It takes all of his relevant findings into account: retinal abnormalities, migraine with aura, mood disorders, early onset history of stroke-like episodes, pseudobulbar palsy, dementia, and three generations of paternal family history. A positive genetic test of Nietzsche’s DNA for a NOTCH3 gene mutation would be diagnostic.
A Stroke of Bad Luck
65
Table 3.1 List of Previous Diagnostic Attempts to Explain Nietzsche’s Illness Diagnosis
Source
Paresis paralytica (neurosyphilis)
Binswanger 1889; Mobius 1902; Lange-Eichbaum 1930 Hildebrandt 1926 Cybulska 2000 Schain 2001 Sax 2003 Orth and Trimble 2006 Owen, Schaller, and Binder 2007
Slow-growing benign brain tumor Bipolar disease and multi-infarct dementia Schizophrenia Meningioma of right optic nerve Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) Meningioma of right medial sphenoid wing Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL)
Butler (this chapter), Hemelsoet, Hemelsoet, and Devreese 2008
CADASIL is a genetic mutation in the NOTCH3 protein on chromosome 19p13.2-p13.1. This autosomal dominant condition leads to microangiopathy of the brain. The presentation of CADASIL is variable even among family members. CADASIL is suspected when stroke-like episodes occur before the age of 60, accompanied by MA, executive dysfunction, or behavioral abnormalities. TIAs (transient ischemic attacks) occur in 85% of symptomatic individuals with the average age of onset at 46 (range 19–67 years). Ischemic episodes are recurrent, leading to severe disability usually including gait disturbance, urinary incontinence, and pseudobulbar palsy. Eighty-five percent of patients develop cognitive dysfunction and eventual dementia. These symptoms fit Nietzsche’s disease progression. He presented with gait disturbance and pseudobulbar palsy in 1889, slowly progressing over the course of years to a demented, bedridden, and paralyzed state. Migraine occurs in about 40% of CADASIL patients with the first attack occurring at a mean age of 26 years. Of the CADASIL patients with migraine 90% have MA. In some families with CADASIL, MA is the most prominent symptom. Again, Nietzsche’s presentation clinically matches. His correspondences evince the development of MA by his mid- to late 20s. Approximately 30% of individuals with CADASIL develop psychiatric disturbances, with depression, bipolar, and personality changes being most common. Undoubtedly Nietzsche struggled with major depression and mood swings that suggest bipolar disorder. Full-blown mania with delusions of grandeur afflicted him by the end of 1888.
66
Dementia
Epilepsy is less common (~10% of CADASIL patients) and tends to develop in middle age. Although Nietzsche mentioned having a seizure in a letter cited earlier from January 1880, it is hard to interpret exactly what Nietzsche meant by his limited use of that term. However, seizures clearly afflicted Nietzsche’s father. Most likely, Karl’s episodes of loss of consciousness were either seizure-induced or TIA-induced syncope. Both seizure and TIAs are common in CADASIL, so differentiating between them is unimportant for diagnosis in this case. Nietzsche’s family history supports the finding that CADASIL’s clinical manifestations are variable even among relatives. Most recently, retinal abnormalities have been linked to CADASIL. Retinal vascular abnormalities, inflammation, and vision loss have all been implicated. Nietzsche’s right-sided retinal exudates, vision loss, and alleged eye movement abnormalities were likely expressions of CADASIL. Recent research findings suggest that CADASIL can lead to diminished optic nerve fiber layers, retinal vasculopathies, retinal inflammation, visual field loss, eye movement abnormalities, and visual-spatial defects. By reasonable assumption, Nietzsche has a positive family history. His father suffered from numerous stroke-like episodes before the age of 60, struggled with depression, and developed pseudobulbar affect and potential palsy. There is evidence of seizures, hemi-facial paralysis, dysarthria, cognitive decline, dementia, and eventual death from stroke. CADASIL explains this array of seemingly disparate symptoms. Although less is known about him, Karl’s father was afflicted by a similar array of symptoms. This pattern fits the autosomal dominance inheritance pattern of CADASIL. Genetics studies have traced original mutations back to the 1600s in some Northern European communities. It is therefore reasonable to assume the existence of CADASIL mutations in Nietzsche’s patrilineage based on historical, demographic, and founder effect studies. CADASIL as cause for Nietzsche’s illness is testable. Because the common NOTCH3 mutations are well established and currently tested for genetically, it is possible to obtain nuclear DNA from Nietzsche, amplify the gene of interest via polymerase chain reaction techniques, and test for CADASIL with 90% sensitivity. The author is actively pursuing this research goal. DNA can be extracted from minute salivary samples sealed in time between envelope folds or stamps adhered by Nietzsche during his lifetime. Theoretically, preserved DNA samples from envelopes sealed by Nietzsche’s saliva could be amplified in the region of the NOTCH3 gene and tested for CADASIL. This would provide incontrovertible evidence for this diagnosis. Concurrent to the preparation of this publication, an independent research effort by Hemelsoet, Hemelsoet, and Devreese (2008) also
A Stroke of Bad Luck
67
suggests CADASIL as a potential diagnosis for Nietzsche. Herein, we extend the confidence in the diagnosis of CADASIL by including evidence for three successive generations of disease, Nietzsche’s history of migraine with aura, psychiatric disturbance, fits, and retinal abnormalities. Without evidence to extend the paternal history of disease to three generations, CADASIL becomes less likely. With a two-generation model, the recessive form of CADASIL, known as CARASIL (Maeda’s syndrome), must remain on the differential diagnosis in addition to other more common nongenetic sources of multi-infarct dementia, such as Binswanger ’s disease (ironic to Nietzsche’s case) or cerebrovascular disease. Our evidence of three successive paternal generations with similar symptomology greatly increases confidence in the diagnosis of CADASIL (Volz 1990). Further, I suggest clear evidence that Nietzsche suffered from migraine with aura and not migraine without aura as argued by Hemelsoet et al. Migraine with aura and not migraine without aura is clinical evidence of CADASIL (Oberstein, Boon, and Dichgins 2006.) Further, we explain Nietzsche’s retinal abnormalities as a manifestation of CADASIL pathology (Parisi et al. 2007; Robinson 2001; Warner 2004). His blindness and subsequent Charles Bonnet syndrome also fit with our diagnosis. The neurosyphilis hypothesis has repeatedly been questioned even at the time of Nietzsche’s diagnosis. Sax (2003) summarized the key weaknesses in the syphilis hypothesis: the lack of documentation of syphilitic infection, Nietzsche’s prolonged life after his 1889 collapse, the laterality of his symptoms, lack of tremulous tongue, and the extended history of his headaches. Also, the alleged Argyll-Robertson pupils noted in Nietzsche’s medical notes from Binswanger were due to congenital anisocoria, which was a condition unknown to the physicians at the Jena clinic. Owen, Schaller, and Binder (2007) suggest that Nietzsche had a slowgrowing medial sphenoid meningioma. This is plausible because intracranial mass lesions can cause lateral visual symptoms, headaches, cranial nerve dysfunction, psychiatric disturbances, and dementia. Several ideas argue against a brain mass as cause for all of Nietzsche’s illnesses. There is a significant female-to-male predominance in medial sphenoid meningiomas, headaches are rare and tend to be dull and brief, tumors typically emerge in the sixth and seventh decades of life, and common predisposing factors include family history, focal trauma, and radiation exposure—none of which apply to Nietzsche (Demchuk and Forsyth 1997; Zachariah 2008). Cybulska (2000) suggests Nietzsche suffered from bipolar disorder followed by multi-infarct dementia. Cybulska’s diagnosis lacks definitive testability and cannot explain the retinal findings, family history,
68
Dementia
and cranial nerve findings. Likewise, several other diagnostic possibilities, such as schizophrenia, frontotemporal dementia (FTD), and mitochondrial myopathy–encephalopathy–lactic acidosis–stroke syndrome (MELAS) fail to make sense of the paternal family history and Nietzsche’s seemingly disparate illnesses. MELAS is worth consideration because its heterogeneous manifestations potentially explain Nietzsche’s retinal disturbances, headaches, vomiting, psychiatric disturbance, and stroke-like episodes. However, Nietzsche did not have a maternal history indicative of MELAS. With the exception of congenital anisocoria, records suggest Franziska was healthy throughout her life; she died from cancer at the age of 71, her mother lived to 82, and her daughter (Elisabeth, Nietzsche’s sister) lived to 89.
CONCLUSION Nietzsche has a positive three-generation family history—his father suffered from numerous stroke-like episodes before the age of 60, struggled with depression, and developed pseudobulbar affect and potential palsy. There is evidence of seizures, hemi-facial paralysis, dysarthria, cognitive decline, dementia, and eventual death from stroke (“softening of the brain”). CADASIL explains this array of seemingly disparate symptoms. Although less is known, Karl’s father was afflicted by a similar array of symptoms. This pattern fits the autosomal dominant inheritance pattern of CADASIL (Rufa et al. 2007). Genetics studies have traced original mutations back to the 1600s in some Northern European communities. It is therefore reasonable to assume the existence of CADASIL mutations in Nietzsche’s patrilineage based on historical, demographic, and founder effect studies (Mykkanen et al. 2004). CADASIL as cause for Nietzsche’s illness is testable. Because the common NOTCH3 mutations are well established, it is possible to obtain Nietzsche’s nuclear DNA from historical samples, amplify the region of interest (19p13.1–19p13.2) via PCR techniques, and test for CADASIL. The author is actively pursuing this research goal: DNA can be extracted from minute salivary samples sealed in time between envelope folds or stamps adhered by Nietzsche during his lifetime. The best analytical tools at the time of Binswanger ’s diagnosis pointed to syphilitic paresis of the insane. Present-day reason leads us to believe Nietzsche (and his paternal lineage) suffered from a rare genetic mutation in the NOTCH3 gene. Neurologists of the future will likely comprehend the diagnostic complexities with conceptual categories yet-to-be revealed.
A Stroke of Bad Luck
69
POSTSCRIPT: THE INFLUENCE OF CADASIL ON NIETZSCHE’S LIFE AND THOUGHT This research is important for understanding Nietzsche’s biography, and potentially for the interpretation of his life’s work. If Nietzsche was indeed afflicted by CADASIL, understanding the course and pathophysiology of this condition gives reason to Nietzsche’s extreme suffering with one unifying diagnosis. Only Nietzsche fully comprehended the depth of his suffering, how powerfully it shaped his life and thought. To illustrate the effect CADASIL had on Nietzsche’s life and thought, I will first consider Nietzsche’s supposed stylistic preference for pithy and profound statements in the form of aphorisms. Due to CADASIL-induced retinopathy, migraines with aura, and epileptic fits, Nietzsche’s vision continually degraded with temporary blindness occurring during some of his worst attacks. These attacks occurred from several hours to days. His struggle intensified throughout the 1870s until he was forced to resign his professorship in 1879. During these fits, friends or family would care for him, reading aloud to him or recording his dictations as Nietzsche lay still in a dimly lit room. Elisabeth, Nietzsche’s sister, recalled him saying he would have been a bookworm if it were not for his fits, migrainous attacks, and ill health (Volz 1990). Nietzsche was often allowed brief windows of time to collect his own thoughts without the invasion of ill health. This likely shaped his stylistic preference in part—driving him to master the form of aphorism. Nietzsche filled countless notebooks with aphoristic insights from early adulthood until the full development of subcortical dementia in 1889. These aphoristic notes were the basis for many of his publications, such as Beyond Good and Evil, The Antichrist, and Genealogy of Morals. In The Antichrist, Nietzsche writes, “it is my ambition to say in ten sentences what everyone else says in a book.” Nietzsche became a dedicated master of the aphorism, in part because of the impact of CADASIL during his productive life. After resigning from professorship at the University of Basel in 1879, Nietzsche spent the 10 years prior to his collapse moving seasonally from Northern Italy, Austria, France, Germany, and Switzerland. Nietzsche’s personal library contained books on weather patterns, which he likely consulted, as he sought the perfect climate to attenuate the various triggers to his intense migraine attacks. Nietzsche accompanied this itinerant lifestyle with an ascetic diet—he refrained from consuming tea, alcohol, and tobacco, for instance. These behaviors were built around Nietzsche’s trialand-error approach to control all of the factors that triggered his CADASIL-associated attacks, and led to a largely isolated life, which Nietzsche
70
Dementia
intermittently filled with periods of brilliant literary productivity. One of Nietzsche’s colleagues, Paul Deussen, commented on Nietzsche’s eccentric behavior and lifestyle (due to negotiating his illnesses) after visiting the demented Nietzsche in the April 1889, writing, No one can say to what extent the seeds of insanity were already present as a disposition in this highly talented mind. But if Nietzsche had not diligently separated himself from human society, in which he occupied such an honorable position, if he had kept his position, established a family, and allowed the fruits of his mind to mature slowly, instead of pursuing his thoughts in solitude with ascetic over-exertion of his energies on tiring walks during the day and at night compelling elusive sleep by stronger and stronger narcotics— who knows whether he might not still be living with us in full health and be able to offer us, instead of the torso of his posthumous works, the perfected divine image of an eccentric but highly noteworthy worldview. (Deussen 1890/1922) The pain Nietzsche endured due to CADASIL strongly shaped him as a thinker, an artist, and a philosopher. From a young age Nietzsche demonstrated musical talent. He composed pieces and played improvisational piano with considerable skill. One of the titles of his compositions from 1861 arranged when he was only 17 years old was entitled Schmerz ist der Grundton der Natur or translated “Pain is the elemental tone of Nature.” Lou Salomé, an alleged romantic and intellectual interest of Nietzsche’s, later wrote that he was “a sadomasochist toward himself” attempting to find some contorted version of pleasure in the psychic and physiologic pain he was forced to endure (Volz 1990). In 1879 following the completion of The Wanderer and His Shadow, Nietzsche wrote in a letter, “The completed Wanderer is to me something almost unbelievable . . . the entire “humanity” with the 2 supplements is from a time of the most bitter and continual pains—and yet seems to me to be a thing full of health. This is my triumph.” Nietzsche firmly believed that “everything deep loves a mask.” Nietzsche continually layered meanings into his writings that interleaved his own personal experiences within depth psychological insight, aesthetics, and philosophical conjecture. Nietzsche’s CADASIL-induced Charles Bonnet Syndrome experience echoes in his section of the Logic of the Dream. In Human, All Too Human, he wrote, “If we close our eyes, the brain produces a host of light-impressions and colours” and likely in reference to his life’s pain he later wrote in Twilight of the Idols, “increscunt
A Stroke of Bad Luck
71
animi, virescit volnere virtus” (“the spirit grows, strength is restored by wounding”) (Nietzsche 2003, 2006). Summarizing the profound physical suffering and mental anguish he endured, Nietzsche proffered, in combating my sick conditions I always instinctively chose the right means. . . . I took myself in hand, I myself made myself healthy again. . . . I made out of my will to health, to life, my philosophy. . . . For pay heed to this: it was in the years of my lowest vitality that I ceased to be a pessimist: the instinct for self-recovery forbade me to a philosophy of indigence and discouragement. (Nietzsche 1989) Nietzsche wrote those words just months before the onset of his subcortical dementia. Four years earlier he asserted in Beyond Good and Evil (1955): “I have come to realize what every great philosophy up to now has been: the personal confession of its originator, a type of involuntary and unaware memoir.” Still earlier, in an unpublished notebook from 1873, he wrote, “For what purpose humanity is there should not even concern us: why you are there, that you should ask yourself: and if you have no ready answer, then set for yourself goals, high and noble goals, and perish in pursuit of them! I know of no better life purpose than to perish in attempting the great and the impossible” (Nietzsche 2009). As many opinions exist as the number of profound thinkers who have attempted to interpret Nietzsche’s philosophical teaching (e.g., Heidegger 1961/1984; Lampert 1993; Rosen 1995). If Nietzsche did in fact suffer from CADASIL, this does not adjudicate among the many laudable attempts to delineate Nietzsche’s philosophical thought. I merely suggest that the pathophysiology of CADASIL profoundly shaped the person of Nietzsche, his life events, thought, and philosophy. Nietzsche built many of his philosophical teachings, such as will to power, eternal return of the same, and the idea of the übermensch, around his personal experience with physical and mental pain. Nietzsche proffered, A philosopher who has traversed many kinds of health, and keeps traversing them, has passed through an equal number of philosophies; he simply cannot keep from transposing his states every time into the most spiritual form and distance: this art of transfiguration is philosophy. (Nietzsche 1974) ACKNOWLEDGMENT: Special thanks go to Dorothe Poggel, PhD for help with translation of original German medical records. Also, I thank Peter
72
Dementia
Bergethon, MD for help contributing commentary and constructive criticism for the manuscript. Special thanks also go to Patrick McNamara, PhD and Benjamin Wolozin, PhD, MD for early support of this project. REFERENCES Cybulska, E. M. 2000. The madness of Nietzsche: A misdiagnosis of the millennium? Journal of Hospital Medicine 61 (8): 571–575. Demchuk, A., and P. Forsyth. 1997. Headache in the cancer patient. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology, ed. P. K. Vinken, G. W. Bruyn, and C. J. Vecht, 25 (69): Neuro-Oncology, pt. 3, 241–266. Deussen, Paul. 1890/1922. Erinnerungen an Friedrich Nietzsche. Leipzig: Brockhaus. As cited in Conversations with Nietzsche: A Life in the Words of His Contemporaries, ed. Sander L. Gilman, trans. David J. Parent. New York: Oxford University Press. Frenzel, Ivan. 1967. Friedrich Nietzsche: An Illustrated Biography. New York: Pegasus. Hegel, Georg Wilhelm Friedrich. 1821/1991. Elements of the Philosophy of Right. Edited by Allen W. Wood, translated by H. B. Nisbet. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hegel, Georg Wilhelm Friedrich. 1837/1997. Reason in History. Translated by Robert S. Hartman. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Heidegger, Martin. 1961/1984. Nietzsche: Volumes 1 and 2, The Will to Power as Art and The Eternal Recurrence of the Same. Translated by David Farrell Krell. San Francisco: HarperCollins. Hemelsoet, D., K. Hemelsoet, and D. Devreese. 2008. The neurological illness of Friedrich Nietzsche. Acta Neurologica Belgica 108: 9–16. Hildebrandt, K. 1926. Gesundheit und krankheit in Nietzsches leben und werk. Berlin: Karger. International Headache Society, Headache Classification Subcommittee. 2004. The international classification of headache disorders. Cephalalgia, 24. Koszka, C. 2009. Friedrich Nietzsche (1844–1900): A classical case of mitochondrial encephalomyopathy with lactic acidosis and stroke-like episodes (MELAS) syndrome? Journal of Medical Biography 17: 161–164. Lampert, L. 1993. Nietzsche and Modern Times: A Study of Bacon, Descartes, and Nietzsche. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Lange-Eichbaum, W. 1930. Nietzsche als psychiatrisches problem. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift, 1538. Mobius, P. J. 1902. Ueber das pathologische bei Nietzsche. Wiesbaden: J. F. Bergmann. Mykkanen, K., M. L. Savontaus, V. Juvonen, et al. 2004. Detection of the founder affect in Finnish CADASIL families. Journal of European Human Genetics 12: 813–819. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1955. Beyond Good and Evil. Translated by Marianne Cowan. Chicago: Henry Regnary.
A Stroke of Bad Luck
73
Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1971. Nietzsche: A Self-portrait from His Letters. Edited and translated by Peter Fuss and Henry Shapiro. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1974. The Gay Science: With a Prelude in Rhymes and an Appendix of Songs. Translated by Walter Kaufmann. New York: Vintage Books. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1985. Selected Letters: Nietzsche. Translated by A. N. Ludovici and edited by O. Levy. London: Soho Book Company. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1989. On the Genealogy of Morals and Ecce Homo. Translated by Walter Kaufmann and R. J. Hollingdale. New York: Vintage Books. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1996. Selected Letters of Friedrich Nietzsche. Edited and translated by Christopher Middleton. Chicago: Hackett Publishing. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 2003. Twilight of the Idols and the Antichrist. Translated by R. J. Hollingdale. New York: Penguin Books. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 2006. Human, All Too Human: A Book for Free Spirits. Translated by R. J. Hollingdale. New York: Cambridge University Press. Nietzsche, Friedrich. 2009. Writings from the Early Notebooks. Edited by Raymond Geuss and Alexander Nehamas. New York: Cambridge University Press. Oberstein, S. L., E. Boon, and M. Dichgans. 2006. CADASIL: Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy. http://www.nih.gov/genetics/CADASIL (accessed November 1, 2009). Orth, M., and M. R. Trimble. 2006. Friedrich Nietzsche’s mental illness—general paralysis of the insane vs. frontotemporal dementia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandanavia, 439–445. Owen, C., C. Schaller, and D. K. Binder. 2007. The madness of Dionysus: A neurosurgical perspective on Friedrich Nietzsche. Journal of Neurosurgery Online 61 (3): 626–632. Paciaroni, M., and J. Bogousslavsky. 2009. How did stroke become of interest to neurologists? Neurology 73: 724–728. Parisi, V., F. Pierelli, G. Coppola, et al. 2007. Reduction of optic nerve fiber layer thickness in CADASIL. European Journal of Neurology 14 (6): 627–631. Robinson, W. 2001. Retinal findings in cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL). Survey of Ophtalmology 45 (5): 445–448. Rosen, S. 2004. The Mask of Enlightenment: Nietzsche’s Zarathustra. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Rufa, A., F. Guideri, M. Acampa, et al. 2007. Cardiac autonomic nervous system and risk of arrhythmias in cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL). Stroke 38: 276–280. Sax, L. 2003. What was the cause of Nietzsche’s dementia? Journal of Medical Biography 11: 47–54. Schain, R. 2001. The Legend of Nietzsche’s Syphilis. Westport, CT: Greenwood. Volz, P. 1990. Nietzsche im labyrinth seiner krankeit: Eine medizininische-biographische untersuchung. Würburg: Königshausen and Neumann.
74
Dementia
Vukicevic, M., and K. Fitzmaurice. 2008. Butterflies and black lacy patterns: The prevalence and characteristics of Charles Bonnet hallucinations in an Australian population. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 36: 659–665. Warner, J. 2004. Vasculopathies affecting the eye. Journal of Neuro-Ophthalmology 24: 164–169. Zachariah, S. 2008. Meningioma, sphenoid wing. http://www.emedicine.com (accessed March 1, 2008).
Chapter 4
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia Laura E. Middleton
Age is the greatest risk factor for dementia, with the prevalence of dementia nearly doubling with every five years of age. The oldest-old, which generally refers to people 85 years of age and older, are the fastest growing demographic in the United States. Increasing longevity over the coming decades is expected to cause a dramatic increase in the prevalence of dementia. The resources required to care for people with dementia will rise along with the prevalence. Healthcare systems are largely unprepared for the expected rise in prevalence and for the complex care many people with dementia require. People with severe dementia depend on caregivers or medical staff to complete basic activities of daily living such as eating, bathing, and toileting. Co-morbid illnesses are common in people with dementia and require concurrent treatment; however, pharmaceutical treatments can exacerbate cognitive impairment, especially if multiple medications are taken concurrently. Outside of health care, significant demands are placed on the caregivers of people with dementia, often spouses. Caregivers frequently have lost productivity and increased absenteeism (Alzheimer ’s Association 2008). Furthermore, caregivers are at increased risk for adverse health outcomes such as depressive and anxiety disorders (Schulz and Martire 2004). Dementia does not appear to be an inevitable part of aging. Some people do not develop dementia despite extreme old age and even in the presence of neuropathic features normally associated with dementia. As a
76
Dementia
result, increasing attention is been paid to identifying successful prevention strategies. It is important to note that prevention may not be “all or none.” Prevention may translate into less severe symptoms or delayed onset of disease. However, if the onset of Alzheimer ’s disease (the most common form of dementia) can be delayed by five years, the expected prevalence would decrease by 1 million cases after 10 years and more than 4 million cases after 50 years in the United States (Brookmeyer, Gray, and Kawas 1998). Current pharmaceutical treatment for dementia can only modestly improve symptoms and cannot cure or prevent dementia. As a result, prevention of dementia through identification and modification of risk factors is critical. Researchers have identified many risk and protective factors through observational studies. Clinical trials confirming the relationship are often still preliminary. In this chapter, we will discuss some of the most promising strategies for the prevention of dementia, including cognitive activity, physical activity, social engagement, diet, and vascular risk-factor control.
PREVENTION STRATEGIES Cognitive Activity People who engage in higher levels of cognitive activity appear to have lower risk of dementia than those who participate in less. Aside from age, education is arguably the most established risk factor for dementia. People who are more educated have lower rates of Alzheimer ’s disease and allcause dementia than those with less education (Stern 2009). High levels of education are also associated with slower cognitive decline during normal aging (Albert et al. 1995; Colsher and Wallace 1991; Snowdon, Ostwald, and Kane 1989). Interestingly, people who are more highly educated may have faster cognitive decline after the onset of Alzheimer ’s disease than those who are less educated, though not all studies agree (Fritsch et al. 2002; Stern 2009). Some researchers suggest that this occurs because people with more education can withstand greater neuropathic load before they show symptoms of Alzheimer ’s disease. There is some evidence to support this theory. One study reported that people who were more educated had greater neuropathic loads before presenting symptoms of dementia compared to those who were less educated (Bennett et al. 2003). This ability to withstand neuropathic load is referred to as “cognitive reserve”; occupational attainment and leisure activity are also thought to contribute to cognitive reserve (Stern 2009).
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
77
Research has emerged to suggest that cognitive activity, more generally, is also associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline and dementia (Stern 2009). Several prospective observational studies indicate that people who engage in mentally stimulating activities—such as learning, reading, or playing games—at younger ages (Carlson, Helms, et al. 2008) or older ages (Fratiglioni and Wang 2007) are less likely to develop dementia compared to those people who do not engage in these activities. Moreover, interventional trials have demonstrated that cognitive training can improve cognitive performance in older adults regardless of baseline cognitive status (normal cognition; mild cognitive impairment, MCI; or dementia). In the ACTIVE trial, a large clinical trial of 2802 elderly people, training in memory, reasoning, and speed of processing were associated with improvements in cognitive performance equivalent to a 7- to 14-year reduction of normal aging effects (Ball et al. 2002). However, the benefits of cognitive training in this study and others appeared to be specific to the domain trained. Cognitive training does not appear to generalize across domains or improve daily functioning (Acevedo and Loewenstein 2007; Ball et al. 2002). Furthermore, there is some evidence to suggest that older people with memory impairment may be less able to make gains from memory training than those without impairment (Unverzagt et al. 2007). However, people with memory impairment appear to be equally able to make gains in reasoning and reaction time with training. Although the role of cognitive training in people with dementia is unclear, cognitive activity appears to be a promising strategy to improve cognition in old age—and may thereby prevent or reduce the risk of dementia. However, because interventions to date show little benefit to daily function, future trials should investigate whether adapted multidomain interventions, designed to mimic daily life, might be effective in improving global cognition and daily functioning. Simple interventions that include mental activities such as playing games or learning a new skill, which are associated with reduced rates of dementia and cognitive decline in observational studies (Wilson et al. 2002), might be effective in interventions. Trials should also investigate whether cognitive interventions might prevent the onset of dementia by including a long follow-up period. Physical Activity The evidence for physical activity as a potentially protective factor against the risk of dementia has expanded greatly over the last decade. Studies using a variety of ages, definitions of exercise, and countries have concluded that people who are more physically active have a lower risk
78
Dementia
of dementia. Specifically, physically active people may have a lower incidence of Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia, though the association is more consistent for the former (Ravaglia et al. 2008; Rockwood and Middleton 2007). The positive relationship between physical activity and the risk of dementia seems to hold true for physical activity both at older and younger ages. Most observational studies have examined physical activity in older populations (at least 65 years) and have had only a short follow-up time (approximately 5 years). Nearly all conclude that people who are physically active at older ages have 10–45% less risk of dementia at follow up than those who are inactive (Rockwood and Middleton 2007). A number of studies have investigated the association between midlife physical activity and late life cognitive impairment. People who are more physically active at mid-life seem to have a lower incidence of both Alzheimer ’s disease and all-cause dementia in late life, especially if the physical activity is performed during leisure time (Rockwood and Middleton 2007; Rovio et al. 2005, 2007). People who are active at mid-life also have lower risk of MCI in late life than those who are inactive (Geda et al. 2010). Few studies have examined the relationship between physical activity in early life and cognition in old age. However, it appears that people who are physically active in early life also have better cognition in old age. Two studies indicated that people who were active in early life (teens to 30s) had better information processing speed and slower memory decline in later life (Dik et al. 2003; Richards, Hardy, and Wadsworth 2003). In another study, people who were physically active at teen age had lower risk of cognitive impairment in late life. Interestingly, physical activity status at teen age was more strongly related to reduced likelihood of cognitive impairment in late life than physical activity status at age 30, age 50, or in late life in this study (Middleton et al. 2010). It is reasonable to suggest that a longer duration of exercise is better than shorter, even though the benefits of exercise can be realized at any point in the life span. Age should not be a contra-indication to taking up an exercise program, other things being equal. Significantly, physical activity is associated with augmented rates of stable or improved cognition and reduced rates of cognitive decline in people of all cognitive abilities (Lytle et al. 2004; Middleton et al. 2009; Weuve et al. 2004). Regardless of cognitive status, those people who are physically active seem to have better cognitive function and slower cognitive decline than those who are sedentary. However, it is unclear from observational studies whether physical activity improves cognition, delays impairment, or whether, in some cases, it prevents cognitive impairment entirely.
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
79
Interventional studies have confirmed that even short periods of exercise training can improve cognitive performance. A meta-analysis concluded that people who were not previously physically active showed improvements in cognitive functioning after exercising for as little as four months (Angevaren et al. 2008). Exercise interventions may also reduce the rate of cognitive decline in people with cognitive impairment (Lautenschlager et al. 2008). Executive function appears to be the cognitive domain most benefited by exercise (Colcombe and Kramer 2003). The mechanisms by which physical activity affects cognition are likely complex and multifactorial. People who exercise have higher levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factors, which are implicated in neuroplasticity and neurological repair. Physical activity also reduces vascular risk; vascular risk factors are, as discussed later, associated not only with increased risk of vascular dementia but also of Alzheimer ’s disease. In addition, rats with high levels of voluntary physical activity also have less -amyloid plaque formation, a hallmark of Alzheimer ’s disease (Dishman et al. 2006; Ott et al. 1999). Despite the promising results from controlled trials to date, the trials of exercise interventions in relation to cognition have generally been low to moderate in both size and quality (Angevaren et al. 2008). Larger trials are needed to definitively determine the role of physical activity in the maintenance of cognitive performance and the prevention of dementia in old age. Such trials are underway. For example, the Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders (LIFE) Study will begin in 2010 and will randomize 1600 people to either exercise or control groups and will follow them for an average of 2.7 years. The LIFE Study includes cognitive function as a secondary outcome. While we wait for results from ongoing trials, however, physical activity can be carefully recommended—if not for cognitive impairment, then for other health outcomes strongly linked to physical activity such as cardiovascular disease and some types of cancer (Warburton, Nicol, and Bredin 2006). Social Engagement Higher social engagement, measured in a variety of manners, appears to be associated with reduced risk of dementia. People who have an extensive social network have lower likelihood of dementia than those with few social connections (Seidler et al. 2003). Participation in socially engaging leisure activities—such as visits with friends and relatives, going to movies, clubs, centers, and church/synagogues, and volunteering—is also associated with reduced risk of dementia (Fratiglioni and Wang 2007).
80
Dementia
Some suggest that social activity may increase cognitive reserve, similar to cognitive activity, so that people who are socially active can maintain cognitive performance even with neuropathic features normally associated with dementia (Fratiglioni and Wang 2007). Indeed, one study indicated that people with broader social networks had better cognitive performance, especially for memory, at a given neuropathic load (Bennett et al. 2006). However, the direction of this relationship is less clear. It may be that people who are able to maintain cognitive performance despite neuropathic feature normally associated with dementia are also more able to maintain social networks. The results of one study suggest as much. In this study, the relationship between low social engagement and high risk of dementia was restricted to those subjects who experienced a decline in social engagement from mid-life to late life (Saczynski et al. 2006). This suggests that low social engagement may be an early symptom of cognitive impairment rather than a risk factor. There are no controlled trials that examined social engagement on its own in relation to dementia risk or cognitive outcomes. As a result, the importance of social engagement in a successful prevention strategy is still unclear. However, a volunteering intervention that was designed to include social, cognitive, and physical components showed a trend towards improved cognition in the intervention group compared to a control group. The volunteering intervention appeared to be most beneficial to those with baseline cognitive impairment (Carlson, Saczynski, et al. 2008). Further studies are needed to determine whether social interventions might curb cognitive decline. However, the interactions between social activity, cognitive activity, and physical activity are difficult to disengage (Figure 4.1). One study concluded that each component is equally important in the protection against dementia (Karp et al. 2006). As a result, interventions that include cognitive, social, and physical components might be the best strategy to reduce the risk of cognitive impairment; research should further investigate this possibility. A larger, controlled trial should be instigated to evaluate whether multidomain interventions (cognitive, physical, and social) might be able to improve cognitive outcomes in those at risk for dementia. Diet Many risk factors for dementia (hypertension, diabetes, and obesity) and pathologic features (inflammation) associated with dementia can be modified by diet. Thus, it is reasonable to suggest that the risk of dementia
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
81
Figure 4.1
The cognitive, social, and physical components of leisure activity may evenly contribute to the prevention of dementia. Many leisure activities have all three types (cognitive, social, and physical) of stimulation.
itself could be modified by diet. Results from several observational studies support this hypothesis. In most studies, adherence to a Mediterranean diet is associated with lower likelihood of Alzheimer ’s disease and all-cause dementia, as is greater consumption of fruit and vegetables, which is characteristic of a Mediterranean diet (Barberger-Gateau et al. 2007; Feart et al. 2009; Scarmeas et al. 2006). Adherence to a Mediterranean diet may also slow cognitive decline (Feart et al. 2009). Other studies found that people who consume high amounts of fish have lower dementia risk and slower cognitive decline (Barberger-Gateau et al. 2002; Kalmijn et al. 1997; Morris et al. 2003; van Gelder et al. 2007).
82
Dementia
The reason for the association between fish, fruit, and vegetable intake and dementia risk has not been definitely identified but may be related to anti-oxidative, anti-inflammatory, or metabolic effects. On a component level, some attribute the relationship between Mediterranean diet and cognition to antioxidants and/or polyunsaturated fatty acids—consumption of each is associated with reduced risk of dementia and improved cognition in observational studies (Gillette Guyonnet et al. 2007). However, it may be that overall diet is more important than any one component. The interest in antioxidants in relation to dementia risk stemmed from the observation that oxidative stress may contribute to neuropathic features associated with Alzheimer ’s disease. This observation led to the hypothesis that a high dietary intake of antioxidants might slow cognitive decline and lower the risk of dementia. Indeed, in some studies, people with higher intake of vitamin E and C (both antioxidants) through diet or supplements have slower cognitive decline and a lower risk of Alzheimer ’s disease. However, the relationship has not been consistent. Other large, prospective observational studies found no association between vitamin intake and dementia risk (Gillette Guyonnet et al. 2007). Furthermore, randomized controlled trial evidence has, at best, been inconsistent, with most studies finding no relationship between vitamin E supplementation and cognitive performance (Gillette Guyonnet et al. 2007; Isaac, Quinn, and Tabet 2008; Kang et al. 2006; Yaffe, Clemons, et al. 2004). This suggests that the association between antioxidants and cognitive impairment in observational studies may be due to uncontrolled confounding or other biases, rather than causation. Alternatively, vitamin supplementation may only be beneficial for those who are vitamin deficient. Studies regarding consumption of long-chain omega-3 fatty acids, one type of essential polyunsaturated fatty acid common in many types of fish, have been similarly inconclusive (Fotuhi, Mohassel, and Yaffe 2009; van de Rest et al. 2008). Despite associations in observational studies, randomized controlled trials have not found a consistent association between omega-3 fatty acid supplementation and cognitive outcomes (Fotuhi, Mohassel, and Yaffe 2009). Omega-3 fatty acid supplementation also had no effect on memory and attention in cognitively healthy elderly people (van de Rest et al. 2008). However, most studies have been limited by a short follow up period. The relationship between diet and dementia is likely confounded by numerous variables such as education, physical activity, vascular disease, and socioeconomic status. This may explain the inconsistent results of observational and controlled trials. Alternatively, it may be that one supplement is not sufficient to improve cognition or prevent dementia
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
83
but that overall diet and lack of deficiencies is more important in optimizing cognitive outcomes. Interventions should examine the effect of supplementation on cognition in people who are deficient versus sufficient. Furthermore, an intervention focused on comprehensive dietary education and modification may have more of an effect than individual supplements. However, given that adherence to a Mediterranean diet is associated with reduced risk of mortality and cardiovascular disease (Sofi et al. 2008), people who adopt healthy diets are likely to have positive health outcomes regardless of the effect on cognitive functioning. Increasing attention has recently been paid to vitamin D as a means to prevent dementia. Although evidence is very preliminary and generally cross-sectional, some studies suggest that higher serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D may be associated with better global cognition (Annweiler et al. 2009). Vitamin D is also associated with a number of risk factors for dementia including diabetes, cerebrovascular disease, and depression (Grant 2009). Future prospective observational and controlled trials should examine vitamin D intake in relation to cognition, particularly in people deficient in Vitamin D as is common in institutionalized elderly people. Vascular Risk-Factor Reduction Although Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia have traditionally been viewed as distinct disorders, it is now generally agreed that the two rarely occur in isolation. Both types of dementia share many risk factors and pathologic features with atherosclerosis (Launer 2002). Even mild cerebrovascular disease appears to increase the risk of cognitive impairment for any level of Alzheimer ’s disease pathology (Snowdon et al. 1997). Thus, control of vascular risk factors might reduce the likelihood or severity of dementia, regardless of type. Traditional cardiovascular risk factors such as hypertension, dyslipidemia, and diabetes appear to increase the risk of developing dementia in old age (Table 4.1). Hypertension is arguably the most studied vascular risk factors in relation to cognition, with inconsistent results. People with hypertension in mid-life had increased likelihood of dementia in late life in a number of observational studies (Launer et al. 2000; Qiu, Winblad, and Fratiglioni 2005). However, the relationship between late life hypertension and cognitive impairment is less clear. Both high systolic blood pressure and low systolic blood pressure in late life have been associated with augmented risk for dementia (Qiu, Winblad, and Fratiglioni 2005; Wu et al. 2003). Another study found no association between late life blood pressure and the incidence of dementia (Johnson et al. 2008). The reason for
84
Dementia
Table 4.1 Summary of Evidence Regarding Vascular Risk Factors for Dementia Risk Factor
Observational Studies
Hypertension
• Antihypertensives have • Mid-life hypertennot consistently reduced sion is associated the risk of dementia/ with increased risk of cognitive impairment dementia in late life. The association between among people with hypertension. However, late-life hypertension cognitive outcomes have and dementia is less generally been secondconsistent. • People with hypertenary and studies may be sion who take antihyunderpowered pertensive medications have reduced risk of dementia compared to those who do not in most studies. • In most studies, people • In a trial with no with diabetes in mid- or control group, diabetlate life have higher risk ics showed improved cognition with glycemic of MCI and dementia. • Diabetics have faster management. cognitive decline in normal aging. • Diabetics may have slower cognitive decline after dementia onset, possibly due to onset with less severe neuropathic features. • People with high levels • Two large, randomizedcontrolled trials sugof low-density lipoproteins have increased risk gested that statins do of cognitive impairment not improve cognitive outcomes. and vascular dementia in late life. • Statin therapy does not appear to reduce the risk of cognitive impairment among those with dyslipidemia.
Diabetes
Dyslipidemia
Trials
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
85
Table 4.1 (Continued) Obesity
Metabolic Syndrome
• Obesity at mid-life is associated with higher risk of dementia in late life. • In late life, very high BMI or very low BMI may be associated with increased likelihood of dementia, possibly because obesity is a risk factor for dementia but weight loss may be an early symptom of the disease. • People with metabolic syndrome have increased risk of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline in late life.
—
—
• The effects of each vascular risk factor may be additive.
these inconsistent results is unknown but it may reflect that hypertension is a risk factor for dementia but that hypotension is an early symptom of the disease. If hypertension in mid-life is a risk factor for dementia, then it follows that antihypertensives have the potential to reduce the risk of dementia in people with hypertension. In observational studies, this appears to be the case. In a number of studies, people with hypertension who took antihypertensives had a lower risk of dementia than those who do not (Korf et al. 2004; Skoog 2009; Skoog et al. 2005). However, the results from controlled trials have been less consistent (Fillit et al. 2008; Peters et al. 2008; Prince et al. 1996). The largest study, the Hypertension in the Very Elderly Trial (HYVET), was not entirely conclusive but favored treatment of hypertension to improve cognitive outcomes when the results were combined into a meta-analysis with previous studies. However, most trials—including HYVET—were designed to examine other primary outcomes (Peters et al. 2008; Prince et al. 1996). The trials did not include detailed cognitive
86
Dementia
measures and may not have been sufficiently powered to detect a relationship between antihypertensives and cognitive outcomes. There is general accord that people with diabetes in late life have higher risk of dementia and MCI compared to those who do not (Luchsinger, Reitz, et al. 2007; Ott et al. 1999; Yaffe, Blackwell, et al. 2004), though some studies show no association between the two (Luchsinger and Gustafson 2009). Similarly, mid-life diabetes is associated with augmented risk of dementia in some but not all studies. The relationship with diabetes seems to be stronger and more consistent for vascular dementia and vascular cognitive impairment than for Alzheimer ’s disease and amnestic MCI (Hassing et al. 2002; Luchsinger and Gustafson 2009; MacKnight et al. 2002; Whitmer 2007). Counterintuitively, studies report that people who are treated for diabetes have a greater likelihood of dementia than those who are not (Luchsinger et al. 2001; Ott et al. 1999); however, this relationship is likely confounded by severity, where people who receive treatment have more severe diabetes than those who are not treated. Diabetics with hypoglycemic episodes, a complication of uncontrolled diabetes, also have a higher risk of dementia (Whitmer et al. 2009). Diabetes is also associated with faster cognitive decline in normal aging (Gregg et al. 2000). In contrast, diabetics have slower cognitive decline after Alzheimer ’s disease onset (Sanz et al. 2009). The slower decline after Alzheimer ’s disease onset may occur because diabetics have less severe Alzheimer ’s disease neuropathic features at onset. Indeed, there is evidence to suggest that people with dementia who have type 2 diabetes have fewer plaques and neurofibrillary tangles than people with dementia only, indicating that people with diabetes may have more severe symptoms of cognitive impairment for a given level of neuropathology (Beeri et al. 2005). Results from one controlled trial suggested that glycemic control by medical management improved cognitive outcomes in type 2 diabetics; however the trial was not placebo controlled (Ryan et al. 2006). Consequently, the results are very preliminary. The Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes Study (ACCORD-MIND) examined whether intense glycemic control improved cognitive outcomes relative to standard care in type 2 diabetics who are 60 years or older (Williamson et al. 2007). At baseline, those with better insulin control as measured by A1C had better cognitive performance on a number of tests (Cukierman-Yaffe et al. 2009). Although the longitudinal cognitive results have not yet been released, the main ACCORD study was terminated early due to an excess of deaths in the intense glycemic control group
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
87
relative to the standard control group (Hoogwerf 2008). It is unlikely that any potential cognitive benefits could justify the excess deaths associated with intense glycemic control. Other vascular risk factors are also associated with increased likelihood of cognitive impairment, though evidence is more limited. Dyslipidemia is associated with enhanced risk of cognitive impairment in old age. People with high levels of low-density lipoproteins in late life have increased risk of cognitive impairment and dementia with stroke (Moroney et al. 1999; Yaffe et al. 2002). A number of observational trials have examined whether statin therapy is associated with reduced risk of cognitive impairment in people with dyslipidemia but the results are not convincing. Most crosssectional studies found a link between statin use and lower likelihood of dementia but longitudinal studies did not (Rockwood 2006). Furthermore, two large, randomized controlled trials concluded that statin therapy did not improve cognitive outcomes (Heart Protection Study Collaborative Group 2002; Trompet et al. 2009). Consequently, current evidence suggests that statin therapy may not have a role in dementia prevention. Obesity at mid-life, as measured using body mass index (BMI) or waist circumference, is also associated with higher likelihood of dementia in late life (Gustafson et al. 2003; Whitmer et al. 2005). However, the relationship is less consistent in later-life. In people 76 years and older, the relationship between BMI and dementia may resemble a U-shaped curve, where those with very high or low BMI have increased risk of dementia. (Luchsinger, Patel, et al. 2007) It is possible that high body fat is a risk factor for dementia while weight loss is a symptom of dementia pathology prior to diagnosis. A cluster of three or more vascular risk factors (hypertension, hyperglycemia, abdominal obesity, and/or low high-density lipoprotein) is referred to as the metabolic syndrome. Not surprisingly, people with the metabolic syndrome also have augmented risk of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline in a number of studies (Komulainen et al. 2007; Vanhanen et al. 2006; Yaffe et al. 2007). One study suggested that the effects of each vascular risk factor were approximately additive (Yaffe 2007). Interestingly, another study found that metabolic syndrome was associated with slower cognitive decline in the oldest old (van den Berg et al. 2007). Why this is so is unclear but may reflect differential survival in this age group. The mechanisms linking vascular risk factors to cognitive impairment are likely multifactorial. Since it is more common to have multiple vascular risk factors than just one, it is difficult to establish mechanistic links between individual vascular risk factors and dementia. The direct relationship between hypertension, cerebrovascular disease (in its most
88
Dementia
severe case, stroke), and subsequent dementia is well established. Cerebrovascular disease may also be the mechanistic pathway linking obesity, diabetes, and dyslipidemia with cognitive impairment. The degenerative changes in the cerebrovascular vessels may also cause dysfunction of both the endothelium and blood-brain barrier, causing the endothelial cells to produce an excess of free radicals and subsequent oxidative stress. This may result in increased blood-brain barrier permeability to proteins, leading to -amyloid accumulation (Duron and Hanon 2008). There is also a growing body of work that suggests a direct link between insulin and Alzheimer ’s disease pathology. Specifically, in vitro studies indicate that insulin causes a significant increase in extracellular -amyloid levels (Luchsinger and Gustafson 2009). Consequently, people with insulin resistance, such as type II diabetics, or those with precursor hyperinsulemia, may have increases in -amyloid levels caused by elevated insulin levels. In addition, adipose tissue secretes both metabolic and inflammatory factors (Launer et al. 1995). Specifically, the secretion of inflammatory adipocytokines may be involved in neurodegenerative pathways. It is unclear, however, whether adipose tissue is directly linked to cognitive impairment or whether the adipose tissue is a marker of insulin resistance and hyperinsulinemia (Launer et al. 1995). Another mechanistic pathway may be cholesterol, which is a key regulator of neuronal function thought to contribute to regulation of -amyloid plaque deposition in the brain (Fillit et al. 2008). There are several areas that still need to be studied with regards to vascular risk management and cognition. Lifestyle management of vascular risk factors should be examined in relation to cognition in people with high vascular risk. In addition, a large, randomized controlled trial of medical management for glycemic control in diabetics is needed to definitely determine whether standard glycemic control improves cognitive outcomes; however, given the cardiovascular benefits of standard glycemic control, a randomized controlled trial to examine the cognitive benefits is unlikely to occur. Despite the inconsistent or missing results regarding vascular risk management (Table 4.1), it is relatively agreed that people with vascular risk factors are also at augmented risk for dementia compared to those without vascular risk factors. As a result, clinicians treating people with vascular risk factors should be aware of and screen for symptoms of cognitive impairment. Preventative strategies—which may include lifestyle management and medications targeting dementia pathologic features—may be efficacious in reducing the likelihood of dementia in this high-risk group.
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
89
Figure 4.2
Interaction between promising strategies for the prevention of dementia.
CONCLUSION Understanding how behavioral and biological factors might alter the risk of dementia is crucial to the prevention of the disease. Observational studies have identified factors including cognitive activity, physical activity, social activity, vascular risk factors, and diet that could be important both in identifying people at risk for dementia and for interventional strategies to reduce the risk. Though preliminary interventional studies have been less conclusive, future trials should continue to examine the impact of risk-factor modification on cognitive outcomes. Given that the risk factors are largely correlated—people who are more active generally have a
90
Dementia
better diet and lower vascular risk, it may be that living a healthy, engaged life is the best way to prevent dementia and that any one single factor is insufficient to prevent the disease (Figure 4.2). Future trials should continue to examine the implication of risk factor modification and, in particular, how we might combine interventions for optimal results. In the most optimistic view, dementia could be delayed or even prevented by these interventions. At worst, people will improve their overall health and enjoy a more cognitively and socially engaging life. ACKNOWLEDGMENT: Dr. Middleton is supported in part by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Fellowship. REFERENCES Acevedo, A., and D. A. Loewenstein. 2007. Nonpharmacological cognitive interventions in aging and dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 20 (4): 239–249. Albert, M. S., K. Jones, C. R. Savage, L. Berkman, T. Seeman, D. Blazer, and J. W. Rowe. 1995. Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychol Aging 10 (4): 578–589. Alzheimer ’s Association. 2008. Alzheimer ’s disease facts and figures 2008. http:// www.alz.org/national/documents/report_alzfactsfigures2008.pdf (accessed March 24, 2008). Angevaren, M., G. Aufdemkampe, H. J. Verhaar, A. Aleman, and L. Vanhees. 2008. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (3): CD005381. Annweiler, C., G. Allali, P. Allain, S. Bridenbaugh, A. M. Schott, R. W. Kressig, and O. Beauchet. 2009. Vitamin D and cognitive performance in adults: A systematic review. Eur J Neurol 16 (10): 1083–1089. Ball, K., D. B. Berch, K. F. Helmers, J. B. Jobe, M. D. Leveck, M. Marsiske, J. N. Morris, et al. 2002. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288 (18): 2271–2281. Barberger-Gateau, P., L. Letenneur, V. Deschamps, K. Peres, J. F. Dartigues, and S. Renaud. 2002. Fish, meat, and risk of dementia: Cohort study. BMJ 325 (7370): 932–933. Barberger-Gateau, P., C. Raffaitin, L. Letenneur, C. Berr, C. Tzourio, J. F. Dartigues, and A. Alperovitch. 2007. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: The ThreeCity cohort study. Neurology 69 (20): 1921–1930. Beeri, M. S., J. M. Silverman, K. L. Davis, D. Marin, H. Z. Grossman, J. Schmeidler, D. P. Purohit, et al. 2005. Type 2 diabetes is negatively associated with Alzheimer ’s disease neuropathology. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 60 (4): 471–475. Bennett, D. A., J. A. Schneider, Z. Arvanitakis, J. F. Kelly, N. T. Aggarwal, R. C. Shah, and R. S. Wilson. 2006. Neuropathology of older persons without
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
91
cognitive impairment from two community-based studies. Neurology 66 (12): 1837–1844. Bennett, D. A., R. S. Wilson, J. A. Schneider, D. A. Evans, C. F. Mendes de Leon, S. E. Arnold, L. L. Barnes, and J. L. Bienias. 2003. Education modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older persons. Neurology 60 (12): 1909–1915. Brookmeyer, R., S. Gray, and C. Kawas. 1998. Projections of Alzheimer ’s disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. Am J Public Health 88 (9): 1337–1342. Carlson, M. C., M. J. Helms, D. C. Steffens, J. R. Burke, G. G. Potter, and B. L. Plassman. 2008. Midlife activity predicts risk of dementia in older male twin pairs. Alzheimers Dement 4 (5): 324–331. Carlson, M. C., J. S. Saczynski, G. W. Rebok, T. Seeman, T. A. Glass, S. McGill, J. Tielsch, K. D. Frick, J. Hill, and L. P. Fried. 2008. Exploring the effects of an “everyday” activity program on executive function and memory in older adults: Experience Corps. Gerontologist 48 (6): 793–801. Colcombe, S., and A. F. Kramer. 2003. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 14 (2): 125–130. Colsher, P. L., and R. B. Wallace. 1991. Longitudinal application of cognitive function measures in a defined population of community-dwelling elders. Ann Epidemiol 1 (3): 215–230. Cukierman-Yaffe, T., H. C. Gerstein, J. D. Williamson, R. M. Lazar, L. Lovato, M. E. Miller, L. H. Coker, et al. 2009. Relationship between baseline glycemic control and cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes and other cardiovascular risk factors: The action to control cardiovascular risk in diabetes-memory in diabetes (ACCORD-MIND) trial. Diabetes Care 32 (2): 221–226. Dik, M., D. J. Deeg, M. Visser, and C. Jonker. 2003. Early life physical activity and cognition at old age. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 25 (5): 643–653. Dishman, R. K., H. R. Berthoud, F. W. Booth, C. W. Cotman, V. R. Edgerton, M. R. Fleshner, S. C. Gandevia, et al. 2006. Neurobiology of exercise. Obesity (Silver Spring) 14 (3): 345–356. Duron, E., and O. Hanon. 2008. Vascular risk factors, cognitive decline, and dementia. Vasc Health Risk Manag 4 (2): 363–381. Feart, C., C. Samieri, V. Rondeau, H. Amieva, F. Portet, J. F. Dartigues, N. Scarmeas, and P. Barberger-Gateau. 2009. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. JAMA 302 (6): 638–648. Fillit, H., D. T. Nash, T. Rundek, and A. Zuckerman. 2008. Cardiovascular risk factors and dementia. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother 6 (2): 100–118. Fotuhi, M., P. Mohassel, and K. Yaffe. 2009. Fish consumption, long-chain omega-3 fatty acids and risk of cognitive decline or Alzheimer disease: A complex association. Nat Clin Pract Neurol 5 (3): 140–152. Fratiglioni, L., and H. X. Wang. 2007. Brain reserve hypothesis in dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 12 (1): 11–22.
92
Dementia
Fritsch, T., M. J. McClendon, K. A. Smyth, and P. K. Ogrocki. 2002. Effects of educational attainment and occupational status on cognitive and functional decline in persons with Alzheimer-type dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 14 (4): 347–363. Geda, Y. E., R. O. Roberts, D. S. Knopman, T. J. Christianson, V. S. Pankratz, R. J. Ivnik, B. F. Boeve, E. G. Tangalos, R. C. Petersen, and W. A. Rocca. 2010. Physical exercise, aging, and mild cognitive impairment: A populationbased study. Arch Neurol 67 (1): 80–86. Gillette Guyonnet, S., G. Abellan Van Kan, S. Andrieu, P. Barberger-Gateau, C. Berr, M. Bonnefoy, J. F. Dartigues, et al. 2007. IANA task force on nutrition and cognitive decline with aging. J Nutr Health Aging 11 (2): 132–152. Grant, W. B. 2009. Does vitamin D reduce the risk of dementia? J Alzheimers Dis 17 (1): 151–159. Gregg, E. W., K. Yaffe, J. A. Cauley, D. B. Rolka, T. L. Blackwell, K. M. Narayan, and S. R. Cummings. 2000. Is diabetes associated with cognitive impairment and cognitive decline among older women? Study of Osteoporotic Fractures Research Group. Arch Intern Med 160 (2): 174–180. Gustafson, D., E. Rothenberg, K. Blennow, B. Steen, and I. Skoog. 2003. An 18-year follow-up of overweight and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Intern Med 163 (13): 1524–1528. Hassing, L. B., B. Johansson, S. E. Nilsson, S. Berg, N. L. Pedersen, M. Gatz, and G. McClearn. 2002. Diabetes mellitus is a risk factor for vascular dementia, but not for Alzheimer ’s disease: A population-based study of the oldest old. Int Psychogeriatr 14 (3): 239–248. Heart Protection Study Collaborative Group. 2002. MRC/BHF Heart Protection Study of cholesterol lowering with simvastatin in 20,536 high-risk individuals: A randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet 360 (9326): 7–22. Hoogwerf, B. J. 2008. Does intensive therapy of type 2 diabetes help or harm? Seeking accord on ACCORD. Cleve Clin J Med 75 (10): 729–737. Isaac, M. G., R. Quinn, and N. Tabet. 2008. Vitamin E for Alzheimer ’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (3): CD002854. Johnson, K. C., K. L. Margolis, M. A. Espeland, C. C. Colenda, H. Fillit, J. E. Manson, K. H. Masaki, et al. 2008. A prospective study of the effect of hypertension and baseline blood pressure on cognitive decline and dementia in postmenopausal women: The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 56 (8): 1449–1458. Kalmijn, S., L. J. Launer, A. Ott, J. C. Witteman, A. Hofman, and M. M. Breteler. 1997. Dietary fat intake and the risk of incident dementia in the Rotterdam Study. Ann Neurol 42 (5): 776–782. Kang, J. H., N. Cook, J. Manson, J. E. Buring, and F. Grodstein. 2006. A randomized trial of vitamin E supplementation and cognitive function in women. Arch Intern Med 166 (22): 2462–2468.
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
93
Karp, A., S. Paillard-Borg, H. X. Wang, M. Silverstein, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2006. Mental, physical, and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 21 (2): 65–73. Komulainen, P., T. A. Lakka, M. Kivipelto, M. Hassinen, E. L. Helkala, I. Haapala, A. Nissinen, and R. Rauramaa. 2007. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive function: A population-based follow-up study in elderly women. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 23 (1): 29–34. Korf, E. S., L. R. White, P. Scheltens, and L. J. Launer. 2004. Midlife blood pressure and the risk of hippocampal atrophy: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Hypertension 44 (1): 29–34. Launer, L. J. 2002. Demonstrating the case that AD is a vascular disease: Epidemiologic evidence. Ageing Res Rev 1 (1): 61–77. Launer, L. J., K. Masaki, H. Petrovitch, D. Foley, and R. J. Havlik. 1995. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late life cognitive function: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. JAMA 274 (23): 1846–1851. Launer, L. J., G. W. Ross, H. Petrovitch, K. Masaki, D. Foley, L. R. White, and R. J. Havlik. 2000. Midlife blood pressure and dementia: The Honolulu-Asia aging study. Neurobiol Aging 21 (1): 49–55. Lautenschlager, N. T., K. L. Cox, L. Flicker, J. K. Foster, F. M. van Bockxmeer, J. Xiao, K. R. Greenop, and O. P. Almeida. 2008. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: A randomized trial. JAMA 300 (9): 1027–1037. Luchsinger, J. A., and D. R. Gustafson. 2009. Adiposity, type 2 diabetes, and Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 16 (4): 693–704. Luchsinger, J. A., B. Patel, M. X. Tang, N. Schupf, and R. Mayeux. 2007. Measures of adiposity and dementia risk in elderly persons. Arch Neurol 64 (3): 392–398. Luchsinger, J. A., C. Reitz, B. Patel, M. X. Tang, J. J. Manly, and R. Mayeux. 2007. Relation of diabetes to mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 64 (4): 570–575. Luchsinger, J. A., M. X. Tang, Y. Stern, S. Shea, and R. Mayeux. 2001. Diabetes mellitus and risk of Alzheimer ’s disease and dementia with stroke in a multiethnic cohort. Am J Epidemiol 154 (7): 635–641. Lytle, M. E., J. Vander Bilt, R. S. Pandav, H. H. Dodge, and M. Ganguli. 2004. Exercise level and cognitive decline: The MoVIES project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 18 (2): 57–64. MacKnight, C., K. Rockwood, E. Awalt, and I. McDowell. 2002. Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia, Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular cognitive impairment in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 14 (2): 77–83. Middleton, L. E., D. E. Barnes, L. Y. Lui, and K. Yaffe. 2010. Physical activity over the life course and its association with cognitive performance and impairment in old age. J Am Geriatr Soc 58 (7): 1322–1326.
94
Dementia
Middleton, L. E., A. Mitnitski, N. Fallah, S. A. Kirkland, K. Rockwood. 2009. Changes in cognition and mortality in relation to exercise in late life: A population based study. PLoS One 3 (9): e3124. Moroney, J. T., M. X. Tang, L. Berglund, S. Small, C. Merchant, K. Bell, Y. Stern, and R. Mayeux. 1999. Low-density lipoprotein cholesterol and the risk of dementia with stroke. JAMA 282 (3): 254–260. Morris, M. C., D. A. Evans, J. L. Bienias, C. C. Tangney, D. A. Bennett, R. S. Wilson, N. Aggarwal, and J. Schneider. 2003. Consumption of fish and n-3 fatty acids and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 60 (7): 940–946. Ott, A., R. P. Stolk, F. van Harskamp, H. A. Pols, A. Hofman, and M. M. Breteler. 1999. Diabetes mellitus and the risk of dementia: The Rotterdam Study. Neurology 53 (9): 1937–1942. Peters, R., N. Beckett, F. Forette, J. Tuomilehto, R. Clarke, C. Ritchie, A. Waldman, et al. 2008. Incident dementia and blood pressure lowering in the Hypertension in the Very Elderly Trial Cognitive function assessment (HYVET-COG): A double-blind, placebo controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 7 (8): 683–689. Prince, M. J., A. S. Bird, R. A. Blizard, and A. H. Mann. 1996. Is the cognitive function of older patients affected by antihypertensive treatment? Results from 54 months of the Medical Research Council’s trial of hypertension in older adults. BMJ 312 (7034): 801–805. Qiu, C., B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2005. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol 4 (8): 487–499. Ravaglia, G., P. Forti, A. Lucicesare, N. Pisacane, E. Rietti, M. Bianchin, and E. Dalmonte. 2008. Physical activity and dementia risk in the elderly: findings from a prospective Italian study. Neurology 70 (19 Pt 2): 1786–1794. Richards, M., R. Hardy, and M. E. Wadsworth. 2003. Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 56 (4): 785–792. Rockwood, K. 2006. Epidemiological and clinical trials evidence about a preventive role for statins in Alzheimer ’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 185: 71–77. Rockwood, K., and L. Middleton. 2007. Physical activity and the maintenance of cognitive function. Alzheimers Dement 3 (2 Suppl): S38–44. Rovio, S., I. Kareholt, E. L. Helkala, M. Viitanen, B. Winblad, J. Tuomilehto, H. Soininen, A. Nissinen, and M. Kivipelto. 2005. Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease. Lancet Neurol 4 (11): 705–711. Rovio, S., I. Kareholt, M. Viitanen, B. Winblad, J. Tuomilehto, H. Soininen, A. Nissinen, and M. Kivipelto. 2007. Work-related physical activity and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 22 (9): 874–882.
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
95
Ryan, C. M., M. I. Freed, J. A. Rood, A. R. Cobitz, B. R. Waterhouse, and M. W. Strachan. 2006. Improving metabolic control leads to better working memory in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 29 (2): 345–351. Saczynski, J. S., L. A. Pfeifer, K. Masaki, E. S. Korf, D. Laurin, L. White, and L. J. Launer. 2006. The effect of social engagement on incident dementia: The Honolulu-Asia aging study. Am J Epidemiol 163 (5): 433–440. Sanz, C., S. Andrieu, A. Sinclair, H. Hanaire, and B. Vellas. 2009. Diabetes is associated with a slower rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 73 (17): 1359–1366. Scarmeas, N., Y. Stern, R. Mayeux, and J. A. Luchsinger. 2006. Mediterranean diet, Alzheimer disease, and vascular mediation. Arch Neurol 63 (12): 1709–1717. Schulz, R., and L. M. Martire. 2004. Family caregiving of persons with dementia: Prevalence, health effects, and support strategies. AJGP 12 (3): 240–249. Seidler, A., T. Bernhardt, A. Nienhaus, and L. Frolich. 2003. Association between the psychosocial network and dementia—A case-control study. J Psychiatr Res 37 (2): 89–98. Skoog, I. 2009. Antihypertensive treatment and dementia. Pol Arch Med Wewn 119 (9): 524–525. Skoog, I., H. Lithell, L. Hansson, D. Elmfeldt, A. Hofman, B. Olofsson, P. Trenkwalder, and A. Zanchetti. 2005. Effect of baseline cognitive function and antihypertensive treatment on cognitive and cardiovascular outcomes: Study on Cognition and Prognosis in the Elderly (SCOPE). Am J Hypertens 18 (8): 1052–1059. Snowdon, D. A., L. H. Greiner, J. A. Mortimer, K. P. Riley, P. A. Greiner, and W. R. Markesbery. 1997. Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study. JAMA 277 (10): 813–817. Snowdon, D. A., S. K. Ostwald, and R. L. Kane. 1989. Education, survival, and independence in elderly Catholic sisters, 1936–1988. Am J Epidemiol 130 (5): 999–1012. Sofi, F., F. Cesari, R. Abbate, G. F. Gensini, and A. Casini. 2008. Adherence to Mediterranean diet and health status: meta-analysis. BMJ 337:a1344. Stern, Y. 2009. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47 (10): 2015–2028. Trompet, S., P. van Vliet, A. J. de Craen, J. Jolles, B. M. Buckley, M. B. Murphy, I. Ford, et al. 2009. Pravastatin and cognitive function in the elderly. Results of the PROSPER study. J Neurol 257 (1): 85–90. Unverzagt, F. W., L. Kasten, K. E. Johnson, G. W. Rebok, M. Marsiske, K. M. Koepke, J. W. Elias, et al. 2007. Effect of memory impairment on training outcomes in ACTIVE. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 13 (6): 953–960. van de Rest, O., J. M. Geleijnse, F. J. Kok, W. A. van Staveren, C. Dullemeijer, M. G. Olderikkert, A. T. Beekman, and C. P. de Groot. 2008. Effect of fish oil on cognitive performance in older subjects: A randomized, controlled trial. Neurology 71 (6): 430–438.
96
Dementia
van den Berg, E., G. J. Biessels, A. J. de Craen, J. Gussekloo, and R. G. Westendorp. 2007. The metabolic syndrome is associated with decelerated cognitive decline in the oldest old. Neurology 69 (10): 979–985. van Gelder, B. M., M. Tijhuis, S. Kalmijn, and D. Kromhout. 2007. Fish consumption, n-3 fatty acids, and subsequent 5-y cognitive decline in elderly men: The Zutphen Elderly Study. Am J Clin Nutr 85 (4): 1142–1147. Vanhanen, M., K. Koivisto, L. Moilanen, E. L. Helkala, T. Hanninen, H. Soininen, K. Kervinen, Y. A. Kesaniemi, M. Laakso, and J. Kuusisto. 2006. Association of metabolic syndrome with Alzheimer disease: A population-based study. Neurology 67 (5): 843–847. Warburton, D. E., C. W. Nicol, and S. S. Bredin. 2006. Health benefits of physical activity: The evidence. CMAJ 174 (6): 801–809. Weuve, J. H., J. E. Manson, M. M. Breteler, J. H. Ware, and F. Grodstein. 2004. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA 292 (12): 1454–1461. Whitmer, R. A. 2007. Type 2 diabetes and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 7 (5): 373–380. Whitmer, R. A., E. P. Gunderson, E. Barrett-Connor, C. P. Quesenberry Jr., and K. Yaffe. 2005. Obesity in middle age and future risk of dementia: A 27-year longitudinal population based study. BMJ 330 (7504): 1360. Whitmer, R. A., A. J. Karter, K. Yaffe, C. P. Quesenberry Jr., and J. V. Selby. 2009. Hypoglycemic episodes and risk of dementia in older patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. JAMA 301 (15): 1565–1572. Williamson, J. D., M. E. Miller, R. N. Bryan, R. M. Lazar, L. H. Coker, J. Johnson, T. Cukierman, K. R. Horowitz, A. Murray, and L. J. Launer. 2007. The Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes Study (ACCORD-MIND): Rationale, design, and methods. Am J Cardiol 99 (12A): 112i–122i. Wilson, R. S., D. A. Bennett, J. L. Bienias, N. T. Aggarwal, C. F. Mendes De Leon, M. C. Morris, J. A. Schneider, and D. A. Evans. 2002. Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology 59 (12): 1910–1914. Wu, C., D. Zhou, C. Wen, L. Zhang, P. Como, and Y. Qiao. 2003. Relationship between blood pressure and Alzheimer ’s disease in Linxian County, China. Life Sci 72 (10): 1125–1133. Yaffe, K. 2007. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive disorders: Is the sum greater than its parts? Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 21 (2): 167–171. Yaffe, K., E. Barrett-Connor, F. Lin, and D. Grady. 2002. Serum lipoprotein levels, statin use, and cognitive function in older women. Arch Neurol 59 (3): 378–384. Yaffe, K., T. Blackwell, A. M. Kanaya, N. Davidowitz, E. Barrett-Connor, and K. Krueger. 2004. Diabetes, impaired fasting glucose, and development of cognitive impairment in older women. Neurology 63 (4): 658–663.
Promising Strategies for Preventing Dementia
97
Yaffe, K., T. E. Clemons, W. L. McBee, and A. S. Lindblad. 2004. Impact of antioxidants, zinc, and copper on cognition in the elderly: A randomized, controlled trial. Neurology 63 (9): 1705–1707. Yaffe, K., M. Haan, T. Blackwell, E. Cherkasova, R. A. Whitmer, and N. West. 2007. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline in elderly Latinos: Findings from the Sacramento Area Latino Study of Aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc 55 (5): 758–762.
Chapter 5
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle: Implications for Healthy Brain Aging and Dementia Prevention Michael J. Valenzuela
The concept of brain reserve capacity is most often invoked when explaining individual differences in clinical outcomes from brain injury or disease (Satz 1993). When used as a “black box” in this fashion it is intuitively appealing. Ever since formal investigation of head-injured patients began in times of war, clinicians and researchers have been amazed at the possible diversity of personal outcomes. Sometimes relatively small contusions can lead to devastating consequences; in others massive brain injury does not in the end produce a discernable difference in day-to-day function. Indeed, the volume of disrupted brain tissue is but a poor predictor of clinical symptoms (Grafman et al. 1986). Similar neuroclinical discordance occurs in stroke injury as well (Desmond et al. 2000). So while most neuroscientists and clinicians would agree that the brain has some form of reserve capacity that differs significantly between individuals, the nature of this capacity has remained frustratingly difficult to define. A large part of this difficulty is because the notion of “reserve” can be analyzed at many levels. This is most apparent in the field of dementia, where arguably “reserve” has undergone the most research (Valenzuela 2008; Stern 2002). In the following section, different interpretations of reserve in dementia-related research will be contrasted, with the caveat that a clinical effect should not be confused with the action of several potential mediating
100
Dementia
mechanisms. “Reserve” is a singular term, but it is highly unlikely that only a single mode of action underlies the brain’s remarkable variability in clinical response to insult. Multiple, as yet undefined, interacting “reserve capacities” are therefore at play, and so the term risks losing explanatory power. Anchoring the phenomena at the behavioral level is suggested as perhaps the most tractable strategy. Cognitive lifestyle is introduced as one objective approach. This has already been shown to predict longitudinal changes in cognitive function and brain morphology. Next a brief review of the possible neuronal mediators of cognitive lifestyle will follow and more specifically address how these could lead to the well-established finding that those with a more active cognitive lifestyle benefit from a significant reduction in dementia risk. The main theme here is that all the ingredients of a rich cognitive lifestyle stimulate neuroplasticity in the brain, ranging from activity-dependent gene expression to the adaptation of large-scale cortical networks. Given dementia is, in the end, a failure of neuroplasticity, this has significant consequences for potential preventative strategies. In the third and final section of this chapter, whether neuroplasticity can be co-opted for preventative purposes against dementia is critically assessed. Trials of structured complex mental activity training in the form of cognitive brain training are assessed in the areas of normal healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and shown to have great promise. These themes are brought together with clinical recommendations as well as views on how the field can continue to grow and shed light on this most interesting of brain-behavior interactions.
DEFINITION AND IMPLICATIONS OF COGNITIVE LIFESTYLE Neurocentric Perspectives on Brain Reserve Since the time of Tomlinson, Blessed, and Roth’s (1970) pioneering dementia studies, there has existed a central paradox for the field: why do some individuals who died with significant levels of Alzheimer disease (AD) pathology have intact cognition immediately prior to death? While initially considered to be clinical rarities (Roth 1986), more recent population-based studies have shown that 33% of individuals with nontrivial AD at death were not demented in life (Neuropathology Group 2001). Obviously there is something unique about these individuals, but what could this be? Katzman et al. (1988) were the first to propose a possible explanation for these cases. These individuals were observed to manifest three main differences compared to individuals who had clinically succumbed to their
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
101
disease. First, they contained a greater number of large pyramidal neurons throughout the neocortex. Second, they had heavier brain weights, and third, they had performed at the highest levels on antemortem cognitive tests. Overall, these individuals “had incipient Alzheimer ’s disease but did not show it clinically because of this greater reserve” (144). Given these observations and since neurons cannot be counted in life, early brain reserve research focused primarily on gross brain parameters. Intracranial volume (ICV), head circumference (Borenstein et al. 2001), and even head width (Jorm et al. 1997) have been used as basic proxies for maximal brain weight, which in turn was suggested to provide an estimate of neuronal numbers. This position has a number of problems. First, whether maximal brain volume or weight is highly correlated with neuronal number is debatable. Second, since a more sophisticated model has been missing for specifying which neurons and where in the brain numerical differences may be most important, aggregate numbers and therefore volume has been overemphasized. There is a long and ignoble history of attempts to link gross brain volume or weight to general cognitive features (Gould 1991). But most important, the key test for a putative neuronal corollary of reserve is that variations in this quantity can account for variance in a clinical outcome, in this case incident dementia. Studies have in general failed to show an inverse linear association between dementia incidence and the full range of ICV; an increased risk appears to be restricted to the low to very low ICV ranges (Schofield et al. 1997), or when in the presence of an additional risk factor such as APOE ε4 (Borenstein et al. 2001). Another straightforward problem for a “hard” neurocentric interest in maximal brain volume is that it restricts the explanatory variable to a nonmodifiable property. Maximum ICV and head circumference are generally achieved by puberty (Mortimer 1997) and reflect genetic variance in neuronal quantum as well as developmental, nutritional and environmental factors in early life (Altman et al. 1968). More important, since these measures do not generally change after the onset of adulthood, does this mean that our underlying brain reserve is fixed? As will be reviewed in the next section, the weight of evidence from the epidemiological, clinical, and experimental literature suggests quite the opposite. Cognitive Perspective Katzman and colleagues had of course noted both cognitive and neurological differences in their sample. So another perspective of reserve has been to focus on “how well we use what has been left behind” rather than
102
Dementia
how much of it we had in the first place (Stern 2002; Mortimer 1988). Two distinctions are possible. If we substitute “neuronal numbers” with “neuropsychological competence” or “IQ,” a threshold model can be applied whereby high cognitive reserve individuals simply perform better on cognitive tests to start with and therefore require a larger decrement before crossing a diagnostic threshold (Satz 1993). This model suggests no interaction with the underlying disease process and predicts no differential rates in cognitive decline. Only neuropsychological starting points differ. It has therefore been termed a passive version of reserve (Stern 2002), and identified as a potential source of systematic error in longitudinal studies (Tuokko et al. 2003). In the context of aging and dementia, any systematic definition of reserve must account for this passive effect, which in practical terms means demonstrating differential rates of neurological or cognitive change over time. A more active form of cognitive reserve contends that individuals who have developed a range of deliberate cognitive strategies for solving complex problems are more likely to remain within normal functional limits for longer. This dynamic account predicts that two individuals may begin at the same cognitive starting point, suffer the same progressive burden of disease, but due to increased use of strategic coping mechanisms one may perform better at follow-up testing or experience less day-to-day functional limitations. While certainly an important clinical phenomena that captures part of the ecological nature of how individuals differ, this notion of reserve is surprisingly difficult to measure. Simply asking subjects about their use of deliberate strategies while performing cognitive tasks can produce more questions than answers (Naveh-Benjamin, Brav, and Levi 2007). This active and deliberative form of cognitive reserve therefore suffers from a lack of feasible operationalization. Computational Perspective More recent incarnations of reserve capacity have focused on computational processes such as network redundancy and flexibility (Valenzuela, Breakspear, and Sachdev 2007). In this case, individuals may not only vary on their range of deliberative strategies but also possess differences in the diversity of neural pathways available for execution of these cognitive processes. Having multiple neural pathways for instantiation of the same computational problem (redundancy), or an enhanced ability to reorganize pathways after “network attack” (in computational terms “degeneracy”; see Tononi, Sporns, and Edelman 1999), is theorized to facilitate maintenance of function after neurological insult. While this approach
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
103
benefits from a unification of brain and cognitive reserve via an explicit mechanism, outside of computational simulations operationalization is again problematic (Rubinov et al. 2009). A Behavioral Perspective: Defining Cognitive Lifestyle The alternative pursued in our group has been to simply ask how mentally active and engaged has a person been over his or her lifespan in comparison to the average? Relevant information here includes level and duration of formal education from young adulthood to the present day, the nature and complexity of occupations throughout his or her working life, and the diversity, frequency and cognitive challenge of past and present leisure activities. This has been combined into a validated assessment tool, the Lifetime of Experience Questionnaire (LEQ) (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2007). Higher LEQ scores independently predict not only attenuated cognitive decline over time, but also a reduced rate of hippocampal atrophy (Valenzuela et al. 2008) (see Figure 5.1). This straightforward approach has the main advantage of providing a working operational definition that is clinically relevant. The behavioral perspective inherent in the LEQ does not identify itself with a specific neurological quantity, computational property, or cognitive process. Participation in complex mental activities throughout the lifespan is assumed to lead to changes in a number of interacting mechanisms at different temporal and spatial scales (Valenzuela, Breakspear, and Sachdev 2007). Together these alter an individual’s risk for dementia and cognitive dysfunction. Indeed, we have suggested elsewhere that perhaps there is no one brain or cognitive reserve, but a number of reserves (Valenzuela 2008). For too long researchers have seemed to confuse a single potential reserve mechanism, of which there are certainly a plurality, with the apparent unity of the reserve effect (i.e., clinical protection). The approach of using behavioral anchor points for the assessment of cognitive lifestyle is reliable and clinically predictive, and so it is hoped that more powerful and meaningful mechanistic studies will follow. Cognitive Lifestyle and Dementia Risk Cognitive lifestyle and dementia are linked. Highly consistent connections between complex mental activity and reduced dementia risk have been found across large-scale prospective studies of dementia incidence. Our meta-analysis of the area combined data from 22 international cohort studies and showed that overall individuals with more active cognitive
104
Dementia
Figure 5.1
Scatterplot showing a positive relationship between the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire on the x-axis (a validated measure of cognitive lifestyle) and hippocampal volume on the y-axis. Insets show examples from individuals with high (left) and low (right) LEQ scores along with volumes of their hippocampus.
lifestyles were at 46% reduced risk for incident dementia (CI: 0.49–0.59) (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006). In this systematic review, the effects of education (OR = 0.53), occupational complexity (OR = 0.56), and late-life leisure activities (OR = 0.50) were each individually highly consistent. Similar protective effects of 40–50% risk reduction are also found when specifically isolated to cognitive lifestyle in late life (i.e., after 60 years of
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
105
age), independent of earlier exposures to education or occupational complexity (Scarmeas et al. 2001). This has now been replicated internationally (Wang et al. 2002; Fratiglioni et al. 2000; Wilson et al. 2002). There is furthermore evidence for a dosage effect (Valenzuela et al. 2006). Verghese et al. (2003), for example, found a 50 percent risk reduction for incident dementia over five years in those with a moderate number of cognitive lifestyle activities compared to those with low numbers, while those with the highest degree of participation had a 67 percent reduction in incident dementia. Protective effects in individuals with a more active cognitive lifestyle in later life even after controlling for earlier life experiences gives great hope that interventions implemented at this time can still be effective for helping prevent dementia. Yet despite such convergent epidemiological data, the underlying reasons for the relationship remain unclear. A brief review of possible mediating mechanisms is therefore presented next. MECHANISMS UNDERLYING BENEFITS OF COGNITIVE LIFESTYLE Both the structure and function of the brain can change in response to environmental complexity. Thirty years of research has now been amassed on the effects of environmental enrichment in rodents, a relatively simple intervention that involves moving animals from standard housing to a home environment with additional toys, mazes, wheels, and littermates (for a review see Nithianantharajah and Hannan 2006). Enrichment is therefore a multiplex intervention that increases animals’ cognitive, physical, and social activity. This of course makes precise isolation of the mechanisms’ underlying solely mental activity, in contradistinction to physical exercise or socialization, quite challenging. Yet rodent studies suggest more similarities than differences when comparing mechanisms involved in voluntary running (Cotman, Berchtold, and Christie 2007) to cognitive stimulation (Nithianantharajah and Hannan 2006). Similar principles may also apply in humans, but as yet we lack the tools to probe the brain in vivo at sufficient spatial and temporal resolution. Despite these limitations, neuroimaging studies are beginning to chart the nature of activitydependent brain changes. Overall, human and animal research indicates that mental stimulation induces a complex web of biological mechanisms at different spatial and temporal scales. An active cognitive lifestyle therefore more than likely contributes to a stronger defense against dementia by a number of different means.
106
Dementia
Molecular Mechanisms Long-term potentiation (LTP) and depression (LTD) are important cellular and molecular processes implicated in memory (Malenka and Bear 2004). Both of these depend on activity-dependent changes to excitatory AMPA and NMDA receptors, which in effect change the probability that a postsynaptic neuron will fire in response to presynaptic stimulation. It is therefore significant that as little as five days of enrichment can upregulate AMPA receptors (Naka et al. 2005), and thereafter alter LTP and LTD (Artola et al. 2006). Upstream to these effects in both space and time are molecular changes to gene expression. Remarkably, microarray analysis has shown dozens of gene expression changes, including those implicated in regulating synaptic plasticity, following as little as three hours of environmental stimulation (Rampon et al. 2000). However, arguably the most important molecular changes occur in relation to brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). BDNF is a “master molecule” of sorts, implicated in a wide range of neuroplastic processes including, neural stem cell survival, synaptogenesis, neurogenesis, dendritic arborisation, and synaptic plasticity (Fumagalli, Racagni, and Riva 2006). Enrichment causes profound increases in BDNF production throughout the brain, particularly in the hippocampus (Mohammed et al. 2002; Ickes et al. 2000). There is therefore increasing interest in BDNF as an “enviromimetic” (McOmish and Hannan 2007), although much further research is needed to understand the pathways involved in its regulation and differential effects.
Disease Modification A number of studies of transgenic Alzheimer mice have now investigated the effects of environmental enrichment, with mixed findings. One study found a 50% reduction in amyloid burden subsequent to five months of enrichment, with a suggestion this was due to increased plaque breakdown (Lazarov et al. 2005). Another study found increased plaque load, but paradoxically, improved behavioral outcomes (Janowsky et al. 2005). A third group also noted cognitive improvements, along with evidence for both amyloid-dependent and -ndependent mechanisms (Arendash et al. 2004; Costa et al. 2006). Interestingly, for the optimal triple-pronged effect of decreased AD burden, increased synaptic density, and improved memory performance, all three aspects of enrichment were needed, that is, cognitive, social, and physical activity (Cracchiolo et al. 2007). Whether
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
107
this disease modification is relevant to humans is of course difficult to determine given the wide gulf between transgenic models and humans. Clinical studies using amyloid imaging are therefore eagerly anticipated. Cellular Mechanisms Synaptogenesis is arguably the most robust neuroplastic change, with enrichment leading to increases in synaptic density in the order of 150– 300% (Levi et al. 2003). Changes in synaptic density of this sort are highly correlated to memory function in the rat (Frick and Fernandez 2003). Moreover, this finding is relevant to human brain aging and dementia, since post mortem synaptophysin levels are strongly correlated to cognitive and clinical status before death: Two independent laboratories have found correlations between 0.7–0.8 (Terry et al. 1991; Scheff and Price 2003). One of the key mechanisms by which an active cognitive lifestyle leads to reduced dementia risk may be through upregulation of synaptogenesis in important memory-dependent areas of the brain. Experience-dependent changes in neurogenesis (Kempermann 2006) and angiogenesis (Black et al. 1990) also occur, which in combination may explain why enrichment seems to lead to increased gross brain volume (Altman et al. 1968). However, the functional significance of neurogenesis remains highly controversial—correlations between neurogenesis and spatial memory performance in older animals have for example been contradictory (Kempermann 2006; Drapeau et al. 2003; Bizon and Gallagher 2005). Whether cognitive lifestyle modulates dementia risk through neurogenesis is not clear. Cortical Network Mechanisms Glucose-labeled PET studies can estimate the brain’s overall rate of metabolic consumption as well as its regional variation. Using this approach, repeated cognitive exercise was found to lead to increased efficiency in the shape of a 25–30% reduction in global resting metabolism (Haier et al. 1988). On the other hand, cognitive exercise also results in selective and temporary increases in hemodynamic responsivity in those same brain areas engaged by the tasks (Olesen, Westerberg, and Klingberg 2004; Moore, Cohen, and Ranganath 2006). Cortical compensatory processes are also important and refer to an enhanced ability to adapt against progressive disease in one part of the brain, through functional reorganization in another part of the brain. Studies have, for example, shown that elders with preserved memory
108
Dementia
ability—behaviorally equivalent to that of younger individuals—engage bilateral prefrontal brain areas in comparison to older memory-deficient peers, who continue to only activate a unilateral brain network like younger individuals (Cabeza 1997; Grady et al. 2003; Rosen et al. 2002; Scarmeas et al. 2003). Successful brain aging may therefore not only involve the continued deployment of the same neural processing pathways, but also the recruitment of new brain networks better suited to the aged brain. More recent neuroimaging studies have focused on characterizing changes in direct response to mental and physical training. In general these have found evidence for expansion of cortical grey matter after several weeks of training (Draganski et al. 2004; Boyke et al. 2008; Colcombe et al. 2006), as well as improvements in cerebral blood flow (Mozolic, Hayaska, and Laurienti 2010; Colcombe et al. 2004). Training may therefore at least partially counteract age-related and disease-related atrophy in different brain regions; however, more research is required since these findings appear to be highly dependent on analytical approach (Thomas et al. 2009). Memory training can also lead to specific increments in phosphocreatine concentration in the hippocampus as revealed by magnetic resonance spectroscopy (Valenzuela et al. 2003), of interest since AD leads to phosphocreatine depletion (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001) and dietary supplementation is neuroprotective in animal models (Brustovetsky, Brustovetsky, and Dubinsky 2001). While more research is required, initial neuroimaging reports suggest that mental and physical training can have positive effects on the brain. How Does This Delay or Prevention Dementia? Complex mental activity is evidently a strong stimulator of the brain’s myriad neuroplastic mechanisms. By contrast, the degenerative conditions that underlie dementia—including age-related degeneration, AD, and cerebrovascular disease—all combine to severely reduce neuroplasticity (Mesulam 1999). In a simplistic sense, an active cognitive lifestyle may therefore help protect against dementia by counteracting negative disease-related effects on neuroplasticity. More precise models and detailed human data are required to better understand these important therapeutic and preventative mechanisms. Theoretical considerations also reveal a more fundamental link between cognitive lifestyle and dementia. For several decades we have considered dementia to simply represent the endpoint of a gradual buildup of pathology and associated neuronal loss. This view assumes a unidirectional relationship, implies a high level of clinicopathologic determinism, and is
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
109
the basis for the search for better disease “biomarkers” based on amyloid imaging, hippocampal structural modeling, and so forth. However, as reviewed above, clinical symptoms do no arise in almost a third of cases with supra-threshold pathological AD at post mortem. These individuals have benefited in life from some as-yet-undefined countervailing factors and, for reasons outlined above, individual differences in neuroplasticity may be implicated. For example, the great variability witnessed in cortical compensation and reorganization suggests that dementia may be better conceptualized as a “compensatory failure” (Valenzuela, Breakspear, and Sachdev 2007). This view suggests that disease burden is not the only salient factor when attempting to make sensible predictions and judgments about dementia, but that an assessment of an individual’s neuroplastic capacities is also needed (see Figure 5.2). Dementia onset therefore becomes a dynamic tension between a progressive disease and the brain’s limited but ever-surprising ability to adapt, react, and regenerate. An important consequence of this paradigm is that interventions and practice that can enrich our neuroplastic capacities also help to subvert the clinical effects of neurodegenerative disease. In the next section we will therefore move from theory to practice and review the evidence base for interventions designed to augment cognitive lifestyle. CAN ENRICHMENT OF COGNITIVE LIFESTYLE HELP PREVENT DEMENTIA? Despite the strong epidemiological links between cognitive lifestyle and reduced dementia risk, and a wealth of potential explanatory mechanisms, important questions remain over the arrow of causality: does preceding mental activity reduce or delay expression of future dementia, or is preclinical dementia causing a reduction in participation in activities prior to formal diagnosis? In order to disentangle this complex “chicken or egg” problem, data from clinical trials of cognitive training are most important. When reviewing a potentially vast literature, a specific type of cognitive lifestyle activity has been chosen for pragmatic reasons: cognitive training. Given the sometimes unclear usage of the term and possible overlap with other cognitive interventions such as cognitive remediation, cognitive rehabilitation, and cognitive stimulation, we have striven to use an explicit definition. Cognitive training is any intervention aimed at improving, maintaining, or restoring mental function through the repeated and structured practice of tasks that pose an inherent problem or mental challenge and that target specific cognitive domains (Gates and Valenzuela 2010). This definition does not include training in strategies to compensate
110
Dementia
Figure 5.2
A framework for conceptualizing dementia as a dynamic balance between disease burden and neuroplastic capacities. Above, an individual with a given disease burden expresses cognitive symptoms in the context of relatively low neuroplastic capacities. Below, the same disease burden leads to no clinical symptoms due to greater neuroplastic capacities. The aim of interventions based on boosting cognitive lifestyle is to shift individuals at risk for dementia to the lower scenario.
for deficits, traditionally a rehabilitative or remedial approach (Sitzer, Twamley, and Jeste 2006). Theoretical Issues Repetitive cognitive training undoubtedly improves performance on the trained task— there is indeed more than 20 years of cognitive psychology
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
111
research on this topic (Rebok, Carlson, and Langbaum 2007). To determine whether cognitive training could potentially help reduce or delay the incidence of dementia, two major issues need to be addressed. Generalization or Transfer of Effect Does the cognitive training intervention only lead to improvement in the trained task, or does it also transfer to nontrained tasks? We have proposed a hierarchy of generalization of increasing clinical relevance (Gates and Valenzuela 2010) (see Figure 5.3): 1. Transfer to nontrained tasks in same cognitive domain 2. Transfer to nontrained tasks in other cognitive domains 3. Transfer to global measures of general cognitive ability (e.g, Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive, tests for general intellectual ability, etc.) 4. Transfer to measures of general function (e.g., Instrumental Activities of Daily Living, Quality of Life, Mood, etc.) Improvement on the same tasks as covered in training are therefore clinically trivial, while generalization of training to include better overall cognitive function, day-to-day abilities, and quality of life should be considered benchmark tests when evaluating clinical efficacy. Persistence or Durability of Effect Does the effect of cognitive training intervention last beyond the immediate posttraining period, or is continual cognitive training required? Longitudinal follow-up of cognitive training efficacy is required to answer this question. In the following brief review, a summary of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) in the healthy aging and MCI areas is provided. RCTs in Healthy Aging We have recently published a systematic review of RCTs of cognitive training in healthy older individuals in which longitudinal follow-up was a critical design feature (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2009). A total of seven trial outcomes suggested that a discrete program of cognitive training in the order of 2–3 months can have long-lasting and persistent protective effects on cognition. The overall weighted mean difference was strong in magnitude, estimated at 1.07 (CI: 0.32–1.83) and the nonweighted average relative effect size was Cohen’s d = 0.5.
112
Dementia
Figure 5.3
Hierarchy of generalization whereby change on the trained task is clinically trivial, while improvements in day-to-day function and subjective measures of well-being are the most clinically valuable.
The ACTIVE study is the largest trial in the area (Ball et al. 2002) and examined the effects of 10 sessions of cognitive training on 2832 healthy older individuals. Participants completed three different intervention groups: memory training, reasoning training, and processing speed training. Two years later, each intervention improved cognitive ability only in the targeted area, an effect of limited clinical value. Follow up at five years, however, found that reasoning training protected against functional decline compared to any of the other interventions or the control wait-andsee condition, albeit with very a modest effect size (Willis et al. 2006). This is therefore the first large clinical trial to demonstrate potentially clinically relevant transfer effects. There is also a high degree of community and commercial interest in computer-based cognitive training. One group has conducted a RCT with such a product (Mahncke et al. 2006). The attraction of computerized cognitive training is that training can be standardized and allows a gradient in task difficulty to be automatically incorporated as individuals’ skill
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
113
levels progress. Neuropsychological tests immediately after the end of the training period found verbal memory performance improved by up to 25% of a standard deviation, and testing three months later showed that short term memory performance remained enhanced. The combination of both cognitive and physical exercise is also of great interest. This has yet to be tested in a rigorous RCT. The Sim-A study investigated the effects of cognitive, physical, and combined training in healthy older individuals over a five-year period (Oswald et al. 2006). Thirty paper-and-pencil cognitive training sessions produced a significant effect over both the 12-month and five-year follow-up periods. Moreover, this effect seemed to transfer to a measure of general cognition. The group that did both cognitive and physical training experienced a larger effect size than the simple addition of those who completed just one type of training, suggestive of a potential synergistic action. Other smaller studies with samples of less than 100 individuals have found positive trends but have lacked power (Derwinger, Stigsdotter Neely, and Backman 2005; Scogin and Bienias 1988). The overall effect size and consistency across longitudinal trials of cognitive training in the healthy elderly is therefore promising, yet many questions remain. There has been a wide variety of primary outcome measures across the trials, and details of the applied cognitive exercises also varied. Quality of trial design and reporting has in general been low. However, it is encouraging that those studies with longer-term follow-up showed no evidence of less potent effects. A durable long-term effect from cognitive training may therefore be realistic. Significantly, two of the more recent clinical studies have also shown that their training protocols generalize to domains beyond the narrow focus of the trained tasks (Willis et al. 2006; Oswald et al. 2006). RCTs in Mild Cognitive Impairment Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) may be an optimal stage at which to intervene for the purpose of prevention and delay of progression to bona fide dementia. A nonsystematic review of cognitive training in MCI has suggested potential efficacy for cognitive outcomes (Belleville 2008). At least three RCTs have been reported which adhere to our definition of cognitive training and are summarized below. A small study (n=8) of community-based individuals with MCI tested a multifaceted memory enhancement training with a no-treatment control (Rapp, Brenes, and Marsh 2002). Training comprised of six two-hour meetings held weekly and also involved education, relaxation skills, and homework
114
Dementia
practice. Both cognitive training and rehabilitative strategies were therefore combined. Initially, there were no group differences; however, at the six month follow-up the treatment group had superior delayed list recall than the controls, suggestive of durable effects. No relative improvements in other cognitive domains or global cognitive function were evident. Olazaran et al. (2004) studied a mixed sample of 12 individuals with MCI plus 72 people with AD, randomly assigned to a psychosocial control group or a cognitive motor intervention that included cognitive training. Results across diagnostic groups were combined, so it is impossible to isolate the effect size in the MCI group. Overall, Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive (ADAS-Cog) scores remained stable in the treatment group while it declined in controls. There was also a significantly positive effect of depression scores. The most rigorous study to date has been by Rozzini et al. (2007) who conducted a RCT with individuals diagnosed solely with MCI (n=59). Participants were allocated into one of three groups: treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors (ChEI), ChEI plus cognitive training, or no treatment control. Cognitive training was based on a computerized software package and targeted multiple cognitive functions with increasing complexity. Participants completed 60 one-hour sessions of training over a period of nine months. Three months after the end of training, episodic memory and abstract reasoning were significantly increased in the combined ChEI+CT group, with a moderate relative effect size of 0.7 (in comparison to ChEI alone). This study therefore suggests an enduring effect of training in two areas of cognitive function above and beyond medical treatment. Transfer to general functioning was assessed with a mood scale and both treatment groups demonstrated reduced levels of depressive symptoms. The combined treatment group was also notable for a significant reduction in behavioral disturbance. This trial therefore provides clinically relevant evidence that cognitive training may be useful in MCI, an effect that is durable for at least three months and which seems to transfer to general daily function. Research Challenges for the Field The last 10 years has seen intense medical, community, and commercial interest in trying to harness the power of neuroplasticity for the prevention of age-related cognitive dysfunction. Perhaps the greatest influence has been a sociological trend away from pharmacological agents and toward behavioral and lifestyle modification and positivistic health attitudes. Yet this enthusiasm should not obscure our demand for rigorous scientific
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
115
evidence when attempting to translate preclinical findings to individual and community interventions. The greatest challenge for the field of cognitive training and dementia prevention is in the domain of clinical trials. No trial has, for example, definitively shown that cognitive training reduces the incidence of dementia, as opposed to the rate of cognitive decline. Higher-caliber RCTs are therefore required, with close attention to implementation of active control groups, longitudinal evaluation, choice of cognitive training protocol and outcome measures, and recruitment of relevant samples. While the optimal dose, nature, and frequency of cognitive training in MCI is unclear, a trend that does emerge is that training across multiple cognitive domains leads to better long-term cognitive outcomes (Gunther et al. 2003; Rozzini et al. 2007). A basic recommendation for maximum efficacy is therefore to trial cognitive training protocols that exercise a broad range of cognitive domains using the drill-and-practice approach. This information will then need to inform wider community programs, and many more questions arise. For example, is starting a new cognitively demanding hobby as good as so many hours of computer-based cognitive training (Carlson et al. 2009)? If so, are all activities equally effective or only some? How often and what intensity of engagement is required? Is group participation better than individual practice at home? Generic issues of scalability, accessibility, economy and accountability will also need to be addressed. Clinical Recommendations In the meantime, the general public needs to be well informed about the links between cognitive lifestyle and reduced dementia risk. Given the negligible potential for harm, it is sensible to encourage all individuals to increase their levels of complex, enjoyable, and engaging cognitive activity for optimal brain health, particularly after retirement. Activities that combine cognitive, social, and physical exercise are likely to be the most powerful, and popular examples include learning to dance (Verghese et al. 2003), tai chi, learning a language and then traveling with it, among many others. Individuals should be encouraged to use their own preferences for participating in a new activity that combines these three key ingredients. There is of course a clinical obligation to also realistically manage expectations, for no intervention can guarantee the absolute prevention of dementia. An active cognitive lifestyle may help prevent dementia and minimize risk, but as yet there is no strategy that can fully eliminate this risk. An active cognitive lifestyle should therefore be part of a holistic
116
Dementia
risk-reduction strategy that includes blood pressure control, minimization of other cardiac risk factors, and a healthy diet. CONCLUSIONS An active cognitive lifestyle involves a lifespan interest and engagement in cognitively complex pursuits, including formal and informal education, a preference for cognitively demanding work, and cognitively loaded leisure activities. There is now consistent epidemiological evidence showing that an active cognitive lifestyle is a protective factor against dementia. The neurobiological basis for this is complex and potentially involves stimulation of several interacting neuroplastic processes. Yet most important, these mechanisms can be exploited even well into later life, such that interventions like cognitive training that boost cognitive lifestyle, appear to slow the rate of cognitive decline. There are therefore good grounds to expect that interventions based around an enhanced cognitive lifestyle may contribute to the primary prevention of dementia, but much more research is required. In the meantime, we should encourage individuals to stay mentally active, particularly after retirement, for the promotion of brain health.
REFERENCES Altman, J., R. Wallace, W. Anderson, and G. Das. 1968. Behaviourally induced changes in length of cerebrum in rat. Developmental Psychobiology 1: 112–117. Arendash, G., et al. 2004. Environmental enrichment improves cognition in aged Alzheimer ’s transgenic mice despite stable β-amyloid deposition. Neuroreport 15: 1751–1754. Artola, A., J. von Frijtag, P. Fermont, W. Gispen, L. Schrama, A. Kamal, et al. 2006. Long-lasting modulation of the induction of LTD and LTP in rat hippocampal CA1 by behavioural stress and environmental enrichment. European Journal of Neuroscience 23: 261–272. Ball, K., D. Berch, K. Helmers, J. Jobe, M. Leveck, M. Marsiske, et al. 2002. Effect of cognitive training interventions with older adults—a randomised control trial. JAMA 288: 2271–2281. Belleville, S. 2008. Cognitive training for persons with mild cognitive impairment 3. International Psychogeriatrics 20: 57–66. Bizon, J., and M. Gallagher. 2005. More is less: Neurogenesis and age-related cognitive decline in Long-Evans rats. Science, Aging, Knowledge and Environment 2005 (7): re2. Black, J., K. Isaacs, B. Anderson, A. Alcantara, and W. Greenough. 1990. Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
117
cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 87: 5568–5572. Borenstein, G. A., J. A. Mortimer, J. D. Bowen, W. C. McCormick, S. M. McCurry, G. D. Schellenberg, et al. 2001. Head circumference and incident Alzheimer ’s disease: Modification by apolipoprotein E. Neurology 57: 1453–1460. Boyke, J., J. Driemeyer, C. Gaser, C. Buchel, and A. May. 2008. Training-induced brain structure changes in the elderly. Journal of Neuroscience 28: 7031–7035. Brustovetsky, N., T. Brustovetsky, and J. Dubinsky. 2001. On the mechanisms of neuroprotection by creatine and phosphocreatine. Journal of Neurochemistry 76: 425–434. Cabeza, R. 1997. Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience 17: 391–400. Carlson, M., K. Erickson, A. Kramer, M. Voss, N. Bolea, M. Mielke, et al. 2009. Evidence for neurocognitive plasticity in at-risk older adults: The Experience Corps Program. Journal of Gerontology A: Biological Science Medical Science 64A: 1275–1282. Colcombe, S., E. Eriksson, P. Scalf, J. Kim, R. Prakash, E. McAuley, et al. 2006. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences 61A: 1166–1170. Colcombe, S., A. Kramer, K. Erickson, P. Scalf, E. McAuley, N. Cohen, et al. 2004. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101: 3316–3321. Costa, D., J. Cracchiolo, A. Bachstetter, T. Hughes, K. Bales, S. Paul, et al. 2006. Enrichment improves cognition in AD mice by amyloid-related and unrelated mechanisms. Neurobiology of Aging 28: 831–844. Cotman, C. W., N. Berchtold, and L. Christie. 2007. Exercise builds brain health: Key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends in Neuroscience 30: 465–472. Cracchiolo, J., T. Mori, S. Nazian, J. Tan, H. Potter, and G. Arendash. 2007. Enhanced cognitive activity—over and above social or physical activity—is required to protect Alzheimer ’s mice against cognitive impairment, reduce Aβ deposition, and increase synaptic immunostaining. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 88: 277–294. Derwinger, A., A. Stigsdotter Neely, and L. Backman. 2005. Design your own memory strategies! Self-generated strategy training versus mnemonic training in old age: An 8-month follow-up. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 15: 37–54. Desmond, D. W., J. Moroney, M. Paik, M. Sano, E. Mohr, S. Aboumatar, et al. 2000. Frequency and clinical determinants of dementia after ischemic stroke. Neurology 54: 1124–1131. Draganski, B., C. Gaser, V. Busch, G. Schuierer, U. Bogdahn, and A. May. 2004. Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 427: 311–312.
118
Dementia
Drapeau, E., W. Mayo, C. Aurousseau, M. Le Moal, P. Piazza, and D. Abrous. 2003. Spatial memory performances of aged rats in the water maze predict levels of hippocampal neurogenesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100: 14385–14390. Fratiglioni, L., H. X. Wang, K. Ericsson, M. Maytan, and B. Winblad. 2000. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: A community-based longitudinal study. Lancet 355: 1315–1319. Frick, K. and S. Fernandez. 2003. Enrichment enhances spatial memory and increases synaptophysin levels in aged female mice. Neurobiology of Aging 24: 615–626. Fumagalli, F., G. Racagni, and M. Riva. 2006. The expanding role of BDNF: A therapeutic target for Alzheimer ’s disease. Pharmacogenomics Journal 6: 8–15. Gates, N. and M. Valenzuela. 2010. Cognitive exercise and its role in cognitive function in the elderly. Current Psychiatry Reports 12: 20–27. Gould, S. J. 1991. The Mismeasure of Man. New York: Norton. Grady, C., A. McIntosh, S. Beig, M. Keightley, H. Burian, and S. Black. 2003. Evidence from functional neuroimaging of a compensatory prefrontal network in Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience 23: 986–993. Grafman, J., A. Salazar, H. Weingartner, S. Vance, and D. Amin. 1986. The relationship of brain-tissue loss volume and lesion location to cognitive deficit. Journal of Neuroscience 6: 301–307. Gunther, V., P. Schafer, B. Holzner, and G. Kemmler. 2003. Long-term improvements in cognitive performance through computer-assisted cognitive training: A pilot study in a residential home for older people. Aging and Mental Health 7: 200–206. Haier, R., B. Siegel, K. Nuechterlein, E. Hazlett, J. Wu, J. Paek, et al. 1988. Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography. Intelligence 12: 199–217. Ickes, B., T. Pham, L. Sanders, D. Albeck, A. Mohammed, and A. Granholm. 2000. Long-term environmental enrichment leads to regional increases in neurotrophin levels in rat brain. Experimental Neurology 164: 45–52. Janowsky, J., et al. 2005. Environmental enrichment mitigates cognitive deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience 25: 5217–5224. Jorm, A. E., H. Creasey, A. Broe, M. Sulway, S. Kos, and O. Dent. 1997. The advantage of being broad-minded: brain diameter and neuropsychological test performance in elderly war veterans. Personality and Individual Differences 23: 371–377. Katzman, R., R. Terry, R. DeTeresa, T. Brown, P. Davies, P. Fuld, et al. 1988. Clinical, pathological and neurochemical changes in dementia: A subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Annals of Neurology 23: 138–144. Kempermann, G. 2006. Adult Neurogenesis. New York: Oxford University Press.
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
119
Lazarov, O., J. Robinson, Y. Tang, I. Hairston, Z. Korade-Mirnics, V. Lee, et al. 2005. Environmental enrichment reduces Aß levels and amyloid deposition in transgenic mice. Cell 120: 701–713. Levi, O., A. Jongen-Relo, J. Feldon, A. Roses, and D. Michaelson. 2003. ApoE4 impairs hippocampal plasticity isoform-specifically and blocks the environmental stimulation of synaptogensis and memory. Neurobiology of Disease 13: 273–282. Mahncke, H., B. Connor, J. Appelman, O. Ahsanuddin, J. Hardy, R. Wood, et al. 2006. Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticitybased training program: A randomised, controlled study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103: 12523–12528. Malenka, R., and M. Bear. 2004. LTP and LTD: An embarrassment of riches. Neuron 44: 5–21. McOmish, C., and A. Hannan. 2007. Enviromimetics: exploring gene environment interactions to identify therapeutic targets for brain disorders. Expert Opinion on Therapeutic Targets 11: 899–913. Mesulam, M. 1999. Neuroplasticity failure in Alzheimer ’s disease: Bridging the gap between plaques and tangles. Neuron 24: 521–529. Mohammed, A. H., S. Zhu, S. Darmopil, J. Hjerling-Leffler, P. Ernfors, B. Winblad, et al. 2002. Environmental enrichment and the brain. Progress in Brain Research 138: 109–133. Moore, C., M. Cohen, and C. Ranganath. 2006. Neural mechanisms of expert skills in visual working memory. Journal of Neuroscience 26: 11187–11196. Mortimer, J. 1988. Do psychosocial risk factors contribute to Alzheimer ’s disease? In Etiology of dementia of Alzheimer ’s type, ed. A. Henderson and J. Henderson, 39–52. Chichester: Wiley and Sons. Mortimer, J. A. 1997. Brain reserve and the clinical expression of Alzheimer ’s disease. Geriatrics 52 (Suppl 2): S50–S53. Mozolic, J., S. Hayaska, and P. Laurienti. 2010. A cognitive training intervention increases resting cerebral blood flow in healthy older adults. Frontiers in Neuroscience Human Neuroscience 4: 16. Naka, F., N. Narita, N. Okado, and M. Narita. 2005. Modification of AMPA receptor properties following environmental enrichment. Brain and Development 27: 275–278. Naveh-Benjamin, M., T. Brav, and O. Levi. 2007. The associative memory deficits of older adults: The role of strategy utilization. Psychology and Aging 22: 202–208. Neuropathology Group. 2001. Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre, community-based population in England and Wales. Neuropathology Group of the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study MRC CFAS. Lancet 375: 169–175. Nithianantharajah, J., and A. Hannan. 2006. Enriched environments, experiencedependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 697–709.
120
Dementia
Olazaran, J., R. Muniz, B. Reisberg, J. Pena-Casanova, T. Del Ser, et al. 2004. Benefits of cognitive-motor intervention in MCI and mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Neurology 63: 2348–2353. Olesen, P., H. Westerberg, and T. Klingberg. 2004. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience 7: 75–79. Oswald, W., T. Gunzelmann, R. Rupprecht, and B. Hagen. 2006. Differential effects of single versus combined cognitive and physical training with older adults: The SimA study in a 5-year perspective. European Journal of Ageing 3: 179–192. Rampon, C., C. H. Jiang, H. Dong, Y. P. Tang, D. J. Lockhart, P. G. Schultz, et al. 2000. Effects of environmental enrichment on gene expression in the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97: 12880–12884. Rapp, S., G. Brenes, and A. Marsh. 2002. Memory enhancement training for older adults with mild cognitive impairment: A preliminary study. Aging and Mental Health, 5–11. Rebok, G., M. Carlson, and J. Langbaum. 2007. Training and maintaining memory abilities in healthy older adults: traditional and novel approaches. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 62B: 53–61. Rosen, A., M. Prull, R. O’Hara, E. Race, J. Desmond, G. Glover, et al. 2002. Variable effects of aging on frontal lobe contributions to memory. NeuroReport 13: 2425–2428. Roth, M. 1986. The association of clinical and neurological findings and its bearing on the classification and aetiology of Alzheimer ’s disease. British Medical Bulletin 42: 42–50. Rozzini, L., D. Costardi, V. Chilovi, S. Franzoni, M. Trabucchi, and A. Padovani. 2007. Efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in patients with mild cognitive impairment treated with cholinesterase inhibitors. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22 (4): 356–360. Rubinov, M., A. McIntosh, M. Valenzuela, and M. Breakspear. 2009. Simulation of neuronal death and network recovery in a computational model of distributed cortical activity. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 210–217. Satz, P. 1993. Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology 7: 273–295. Scarmeas, N., G. Levy, M. Tang, J. Manly, and Y. Stern. 2001. Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 57: 2236–2242. Scarmeas, N., E. Zarahn, K. Anderson, C. Habeck, J. Hilton, J. Flynn, et al. 2003. Association of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology 60: 359–365. Scheff, S. and D. A. Price. 2003. Synaptic pathology in Alzheimer ’s disease: A review of ultrastructural studies. Neurobiology of Aging 24: 1029–1046.
Cultivating a Cognitive Lifestyle
121
Schofield, P., G. Logroscino, H. Andrews, S. Albert, and Y. Stern. 1997. An association between head circumference and Alzheimer ’s disease in a populationbased study of aging and dementia. Neurology 49: 30–37. Scogin, F., and J. Bienias. 1988. A three-year follow-up of older adult participants in a memory-skills training program. Psychology and Aging 3: 334–337. Sitzer, D., E. Twamley, and D. Jeste. 2006. Cognitive training in Alzheimer ’s disease: A meta-analysis of the literature. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 114: 75–90. Stern, Y. 2002. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 8: 448–460. Terry, R. D., E. Masliah, D. P. Salmon, N. Butters, R. DeTeresa, R. Hill, et al. 1991. Physical basis of cognitive alterations in Alzheimer ’s disease: Synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Annals of Neurology 30: 572–580. Thomas, A., S. Marrett, Z. Saad, D. Ruff, A. Martin, and P. Bandettini. 2009. Functional but not structural changes associated with learning: An exploration of longitudinal Voxel-based morphometry VBM. NeuroImage 48: 117–125. Tomlinson, B., G. Blessed, and M. Roth. 1970. Observations on the brains of demented old people. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 11: 205–242. Tononi, G., O. Sporns, and G. Edelman. 1999. Measures of degeneracy and redundancy in biological networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 96: 3257–3262. Tuokko, H., D. Garrett, I. McDowell, N. Silverberg, and B. Kristjansson. 2003. Cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults: reserve or ascertainment bias? Aging and Mental Health 7: 259–270. Valenzuela, M., M. Breakspear, and P. Sachdev. 2007. Complex mental activity and the ageing brain: Molecular, cellular and cortical network mechanisms. Brain Research Reviews 56: 198–213. Valenzuela, M., and P. Sachdev. 2001. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in AD. Neurology 56: 592–598. Valenzuela, M., and P. Sachdev. 2009. Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of dementia? A systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow up. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 179–187. Valenzuela, M., P. Sachdev, T. Rundeck, and D. Bennett. 2006. Cognitive leisure activities, but not watching TV, for future brain benefits. Neurology 67: 729. Valenzuela, M., P. Sachdev, W. Wen, X. Chen, and H. Brodaty. 2008. Lifespan mental activity predicts diminished rate of hippocampal atrophy. PLoS One 3: e2598. Valenzuela, M. J. 2008. Brain reserve and the prevention of dementia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 21: 296–302. Valenzuela, M. J., M. Jones, W. Wen, C. Rae, S. Graham, R. Shnier, et al. 2003. Memory training alters hippocampal neurochemistry in healthy elderly. NeuroReport 14: 1333–1337.
122
Dementia
Valenzuela, M. J., and P. Sachdev. 2006. Brain reserve and dementia: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine 36: 441–454. Valenzuela, M. J., and P. Sachdev. 2007. Assessment of complex mental activity across the lifespan: Development of the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire. Psychological Medicine 37: 1015–1025. Verghese, J., R. Lipton, M. Katz, C. Hall, C. Derby, G. Kuslansky, et al. 2003. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine 348: 2508–2516. Wang, H., A. Karp, B. Winblad, and L. Fratiglioni. 2002. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: A longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Epidemiology 12: 1081–1087. Willis, S., S. Tennstedt, M. Marsiske, K. Ball, J. Elias, et al. 2006. Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA 296: 2805–2814. Wilson, R. S., C. F. Mendes De Leon, L. L. Barnes, J. A. Schneider, J. L. Bienias, D. A. Evans, et al. 2002. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA 287: 742–748.
Chapter 6
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia Art Walaszek
THE PRINCIPLES OF MEDICAL ETHICS Three ethical principles are fundamental to medical care in general and the care of individuals with dementia in particular: autonomy, welfare, and social justice (ABIM Foundation 2002). The principle of autonomy dictates that individuals must be allowed to make decisions about their own medical care and about their overall welfare, and to act independently and without coercion; in turn, clinicians must respect these decisions and thereby support their patients’ autonomy. Maintaining autonomy is a challenge for individuals with dementia: cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and functional impairments interfere with one’s ability to comprehend, to reason, to recall, and to have a coherent sense of self. As will be discussed in detail below, clinicians are often called upon to assess the capacity of older adults to make medical decisions, to live independently, and to manage their affairs. Clinicians are obligated to provide treatment that serves the best interests of their patients—thus the principle of patient welfare (or beneficence). In dementia care, a number of issues arise in this area: diagnosing dementia early and using biomarkers appropriately; ensuring that individuals who do not have the capacity to make decisions receive appropriate care; prescribing antipsychotic medications for behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia; protecting vulnerable older adults from physical, emotional, and financial exploitation; and ensuring comfort at the end of life.
124
Dementia
The principle of social justice calls for a fair distribution of healthcare resources and an elimination of discrimination in health care (ABIM Foundation 2002). The aging population and ever-escalating costs of health care will test this principle as society debates the equitable use of scarce resources. The debatable cost-effectiveness of cognitive enhancers and the appropriate role of palliative approaches in advanced dementia are also issues of social justice. ETHICAL CHALLENGES IN THE DIAGNOSIS OF DEMENTIA The diagnosis of dementia remains essentially a clinical one, using established criteria that are reasonably accurate when compared with the gold standard, pathological examination at autopsy. Early concerns about the utility of diagnosing an irreversible, terminal condition such as Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) have waned as effective treatments have emerged (Walaszek 2009). In fact, the ethical principle of truth-telling dictates that patients must be informed of their diagnosis. Early diagnosis allows an individual to prepare advance directives and designate a power of attorney, to consider participating in research, to participate in support groups, and to decide whether or not to take a cognitive enhancer (Post 2000). Great interest now exists in biological markers that would either result in more definitive diagnosis and appropriate treatment, or that could identify individuals at risk of dementia so that preventive measures can be developed. Nevertheless, there has been understandable concern that testing for such markers in young, asymptomatic individuals, in the absence of effective preventive measures, may result in psychological distress, stigma, discrimination, and difficulty with employability. This is in particular true for genetic polymorphisms associated with AD: the ε4 allele of apolipoprotein E and mutations in the PS1, PS2, and APP genes. A 1997 position statement argued that apolipoprotein E testing should have a limited role in diagnosis and no place in screening asymptomatic individuals; PS1, PS2 and APP testing may be useful in evaluating individuals with early-onset dementia (Post et al. 1997). Several recent studies have further refined this issue. The REVEAL (Risk Evaluation and Education for Alzheimer ’s Disease) study examined the effect of genetic disclosure on adult children of patients who had autopsy-confirmed AD. The subjects randomly assigned to learn their apolipoprotein E status, including those who carried the ε4 allele, did not become more depressed or anxious than those who did not learn their status; also, subjects who learned their status became more engaged in
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
125
activities (such as physical exercise) that may lower the risk of developing AD (Roberts et al. 2005). All subjects had extensive genetic counseling, suggesting that genetic testing in asymptomatic relatives of patients who have AD may be ethical if proper supports are in place. Consistent with this finding, the Alzheimer ’s Association argues that genetic testing should only be done when coupled with comprehensive pre- and posttest genetic counseling (Alzheimer ’s Association 2008). Because genetic risk varies by ethnicity, genetic counseling must be tailored to each individual’s ethnic background (Christensen et al. 2008). An extension of the REVEAL study to subjects tested for the PS1 or PS2 mutation (which confers a vastly greater risk of developing AD than the ε4 allele) also found that subjects who learned their genetic status experienced no greater psychological distress than those who did not (Cassidy et al. 2008). Longitudinal follow-up of the REVEAL study (six weeks, six months and one year after disclosure or nondisclosure of genetic status) confirmed that disclosure did not result in excessive distress, though a baseline high level of emotional distress was predictive after greater distress after disclosure (Green et al. 2009). Concern remains about disclosure of genetic test results. The Alzheimer ’s Association asserts that anonymous testing should be available, that is, the test result should not enter a patient’s medical record (Alzheimer ’s Association 2008). In the United States, the Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act (GINA) of 2008 prohibits discrimination in health coverage and employment based on genetic information, which may help allay some of these concerns (National Human Genome Research Institute 2008). In summary, asymptomatic individuals receiving a positive result of genetic testing for AD may not be subject to as many psychological and other consequences as initially thought and may in fact alter their behavior in ways that could lower their risk of developing AD. Nevertheless, until effective preventive measures are developed, caution should be exercised when testing AD biomarkers in asymptomatic individuals, in particular as new biomarkers (e.g., functional neuroimaging with positron emission tomography) become available. THE ASSESSMENT OF CAPACITY IN INDIVIDUALS WITH DEMENTIA By definition, dementia results in a decline in cognition and functioning, which in turn diminishes one’s capacity to make personal choices and to care for oneself. Clinicians are often called upon to assess the capacity
126
Dementia
of an individual with dementia to make medical decisions. Legal systems become involved to determine if an individual is incompetent, that is, unable to make decisions about one’s welfare. Such situations involve balancing the ethical principles of autonomy and patient welfare. An individual’s ability to provide informed consent requires that information relevant to the decision is available, that the individual is free to make a choice without coercion or manipulation (voluntarism), and that the individual has the ability to make a decision (decisional capacity). Dementia can impair both voluntarism and decisional capacity. Voluntarism may be affected by illness-related considerations (e.g., executive dysfunction, apathy, inattention, memory loss, poor impulse control), psychological issues (e.g., depression, loneliness, impulsivity, anxiety, paranoia), cultural values (e.g., discomfort questioning authority), and external pressures (e.g., pressure from or coercion by caregivers) (Roberts 2002). An individual with apathy due to dementia may appear to consent to an intervention without actually having the capacity to do so (Grimes et al. 2000), thus it behooves a clinician to consider the possibility of incapacity not only when an individual refuses but also when one assents. Concerns about voluntarism are especially relevant when an individual with dementia must rely on a caregiver for assistance and when one is a resident of a long-term care facility. Having decisional capacity requires an individual to be able to express a preference, understand relevant information about a situation and the choice to be made, manipulate the relevant information and thereby reason about the situation, and appreciate how the situation is personally relevant (Roberts and Dyer 2004, 54–56). A “sliding scale” approach is widely accepted; that is, the higher the risk of accepting or refusing an intervention, the higher the threshold required for consent or refusal (Drane 1984). Although a diagnosis of dementia does not in and of itself mean that an individual has lost decisional capacity, dementia is a powerful contributor to incapacity. For example, a prospective study of subjects with mild AD found them to have, when compared to healthy older adults, impairments in the capacity domains of understanding, reasoning and appreciation that worsened over the course of two years. At baseline, 70% of AD subjects were deemed capable of reasoning, but at two-year follow-up, only 30% were; interestingly, no AD at baseline were thought to be fully capable of understanding, presumably due to deficits in short-term memory and executive function (Huthwaite et al. 2006). Another prospective study comparing 53 subjects with dementia to 53 older adult controls without dementia showed that 9.4% of subjects had incapacity at baseline, but only
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
127
nine months later, 26.4% had incapacity, primarily due to declining ability to reason (Moye et al. 2006). Performance on neuropsychological testing has been shown to account for a significant amount of the variance in the decisional capacity among subjects with dementia, including 77.8% of the variance in understanding (Gurrera et al. 2006). Though the specifics of assessing capacity and addressing incapacity vary among jurisdictions (and clinicians should be aware of local laws and regulations), a general framework has been proposed. A clinician asks an individual with dementia a series of questions to assess her or his understanding, appreciation, reasoning and ability to express a choice (Karlawish 2008): 1. After disclosing relevant information (e.g., the risks and benefits of a treatment), the clinician asks the individual to repeat the information in her or his own words. 2. The clinician asks about the individual’s beliefs about the diagnosis of dementia (i.e, whether s/he believes s/he has dementia) and about possible benefits to her or him of treatment. 3. The clinician ascertains if the individual can compare options and can infer how a choice will affect her or him, testing for logic and consistency. 4. The individual must be able to communicate a consistent decision. Multiple instruments are available to standardize and/or augment the assessment of decisional capacity. In a review of 15 capacity assessment instruments, Dunn et al. (2006) argued that the MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T) is the best studied and has the broadest application to various clinical situations. The MacCAT-T is based on the gold standard MacArthur capacity model. Training is required to ensure inter-rater reliability. The assessment begins with a discussion of the clinical situation, including the diagnosis, the proposed treatment, and its risks, benefits and alternatives. The interviewer asks multiple questions to assess the components of decisional capacity: understanding, reasoning and appreciation. The patient makes a choice and explains her or his reasoning. Finally, the interviewer comes to a conclusion about whether or not the patient has capacity to make the decision (Grisso, Appelbaum, and Hill-Fotouhi 1997). It has been suggested that the MacCAT-T and similar competency assessment tools are best used as adjuncts to the clinical assessment described above. Studies assessing the utility of bedside cognitive tests have yielded mixed results. Kim, Karlawish, and Caine (2002), in their review of this
128
Dementia
literature, argue that a Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Folstein et al. 1975) score of greater than 24 is predictive of capacity, less than 18 predictive of incapacity, with intermediate scores requiring further assessment to determine capacity. A subsequent study of 37 subjects with mild-tomoderate AD found the specificity for incapacity to be 92.9% for an MMSE cutoff of 19, and the sensitivity for incapacity to be 91.3% for a cutoff of 26 (Kim and Caine 2002). However, no bedside cognitive test alone can accurately predict capacity; rather, a clinician may incorporate such testing as part of a comprehensive capacity assessment. Whereas the preceding discussion has focused on the assessment of capacity to make medical decisions, there are a number of other capacities that may be affected by dementia.
Everyday Decisionmaking and the Ability to Live Independently At some point in the course of their illness, individuals with dementia will require assistance with their basic and instrumental activities of daily living, and they may eventually lose the ability to live independently. Individuals with AD perform poorly on standardized measures of everyday problem-solving (Willis et al. 1998). Executive dysfunction in particular has been associated with worsening functional status. Structured assessments of executive function such as the Executive Interview and of functional abilities such as the Kohlman Evaluation of Living Skills (KELS) may help identify individuals needing higher levels of care. The KELS may in particular be useful in suspected cases of self-neglect (Royall, Chiodo, and Polk 2005; Pickens et al. 2007).
Finances Difficulty managing finances is an early disruption in AD in the instrumental activities of daily living. Patients with dementia and their caregivers frequently misestimate the patients’ financial abilities, suggesting that an objective evaluation tool may be useful (Okonkwo et al. 2008). For example, the Financial Capacity Instrument has been used to determine that individuals with mild AD had high rates of impaired financial capacity (47–87%), and those with moderate AD were almost uniformly incapacitated (90–100%) (Marson et al. 2000). Concerns that arise in this setting include the risk of financial exploitation of the elder, and the need to identify a proxy to manage finances (either by invoking a durable power of attorney or by appointing a guardian).
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
129
Driving Another early functional impairment in AD is the ability to drive, which raises the possibility of harm of self and others. The American Academy of Neurology recommends that individuals with mild cognitive impairment or mild AD should be monitored closely and should be considered for a formal evaluation of driving skills. Those with moderate or severe AD should not drive (Dubinsky, Stein, and Lyons 2000). Clinicians should familiarize themselves with local laws regarding their obligation to report potentially dangerous driving. Sexual Relations Many older adults with cognitive impairment remain sexually active. Of particular ethical interest are situations wherein one partner in a relationship has developed dementia and the other is cognitively intact. Unfortunately, the capacity of individuals with dementia to consent to sexual relations has been poorly studied. One model suggests that having any deficits in awareness of the relationship, in the capacity to avoid exploitation, or in the awareness of potential risks of sex indicates that the individual does not have the capacity to consent to a sexual relationship (Lichtenberg and Strzepek 1990). Voting A standard for assessing the capacity to vote has only recently been developed (Karlawish et al. 2004). Though most individuals with mild dementia retain the capacity to vote, those with severe dementia typically do not have that capacity (Appelbaum, Bonnie, and Karlawish 2005). In the United States, surrogate decision-makers are not allowed to cast votes for those who do not have capacity (Karlawish et al. 2004). On the other hand, individuals with capacity who reside in long-term care facilities may be at risk of being disenfranchised because of procedural problems (Karlawish, Bonnie, et al. 2008). Testamentary Capacity One of the rationales for early diagnosis in dementia is that the individual is more likely to have the capacity to make decisions about her or his future, including creating an advance directive, identifying powers of attorney, and executing a will. The International Psychogeriatric
130
Dementia
Association has developed guidelines for the assessment of testamentary capacity (Shulman et al. 2009). ADDRESSING INCAPACITY A finding of incapacity should lead to (a) the identification and addressing of any reversible causes of incapacity; (b) attempts to improve capacity by means of cognitive and educational strategies; and/or (c) surrogate decision-making. Although most causes of dementia are irreversible, there may be other contributions to incapacity, including depression, anxiety, delirium, and polypharmacy (Walaszek 2009). Given that cognitive enhancers may improve cognition and functioning in a subset of individuals with dementia, it is possible that treatment with a cognitive enhancer may improve capacity—although this hypothesis has yet to be tested, and it presupposes that an individual has capacity to consent to treatment with a cognitive enhancer in the first place. A variety of strategies have been developed to improve capacity in older adults to make medical decisions, including simplifying information, providing it in the form of a story book or video, disclosing information in parts, using health educators, and quizzing subjects (Sugarman, McCrory, and Hubal 1998). As described below, an “enhanced written consent procedure” has been developed to improve the capacity of individuals with dementia to participate in research (Mittal et al. 2007). Roberts and Dyer (2004, 70) recommend a stepwise approach for consent in individuals with deficits in decisional capacity: seek consent to simply begin treatment; then, as the individual’s symptoms (and presumably capacity) improve, discuss more substantive or challenging choices. When capacity cannot be restored, a surrogate or proxy decisionmaker must step in. The identity of the decisionmaker depends on whether or not the incapacitated individual had previously designated a surrogate, namely, a power of attorney. Often individuals have not designated a power of attorney; in such cases, most jurisdictions have laws that describe the sequence of which family members (e.g., spouse, adult children, siblings, and so on) become surrogate decisionmakers. The “best interest” and “substituted judgment” standards govern the behavior of surrogate decisionmakers. In the former, the surrogate makes a decision based on what the surrogate perceives is in the best interest of the individual with dementia. In the latter, the surrogate makes a decision based on the choice the individual would have made, if the individual had been able to do so; in some cases, the individual may have created
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
131
an advance directive to express future preferences and thereby guide this decision-making. A combined approach has been recommended, wherein the substituted judgment standard is applied when it is clear what the individual would have decided; otherwise, the best interest standard is used (Gutheil and Appelbaum 2000, 232–233). A capacity assessment yields a dichotomous outcome: an individual is deemed able or not able to make a medical decision. In reality, however, individuals with dementia often collaborate with family members and other caregivers in decisionmaking. This has been referred to as a “shared decisionmaking” model, wherein the individual can make a decision with assistance or the responsibility for decisionmaking is shared among the parties; unfortunately, legal procedures are generally not available to support this pragmatic approach (Kapp 2002). When an individual has been found incapable of making everyday decisions, managing her or his affairs, and living independently, a court will declare her or him incompetent and appoint a guardian. Clinicians should be aware of local laws governing guardianship proceedings, especially because they may be called upon to testify regarding the individual’s mental state and decisional capacity. Moye et al. (2007) have reviewed the process of evaluating an individual for guardianship. ETHICAL CHALLENGES IN THE USE OF COGNITIVE ENHANCERS Diagnosing dementia, especially early in the course of the illness, offers individuals the opportunity to decide on treatment with cognitive enhancers. Although currently available cognitive enhancers (donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine, and memantine) do not reverse cognitive losses or modify the course of dementia, a subset of patients may benefit from a clinically significant delay in cognitive decline (Qaseem et al. 2008). However, a number of ethical concerns have been raised about the use of these agents. First, an individual with dementia may have diminished capacity to consent to treatment with a cognitive enhancer. A study of 48 subjects with mild or moderate AD found that only 40% had the capacity to consent to cognitive enhancing treatment (Karlawish et al. 2005). Anosognosia, or lack of recognition that one has dementia, interferes with capacity as well. Even if an individual initially consents, it is possible that, due to cognitive decline, s/he will no longer be able to consent to continued treatment. Thus, clinicians may need to assess to capacity of an individual with dementia to provide informed consent, as described above.
132
Dementia
Post et al. (2001), citing qualitative data from focus groups of patients, caregivers, and clinicians, raised the possibility of instilling false hope among patients and caregivers regarding the outcome of treatment with cognitive enhancers. However, a more recent study employing semistructured interviews of 12 caregivers of patients who had used cognitive enhancers failed to replicate this finding. Interestingly, the authors noted that “some hope is necessary to continue to live through the situation” (Huizing et al. 2006). If cognitive enhancers prolong morbidity or extend life, then treatment will increase suffering rather than decrease suffering (Post and Whitehouse 1998). However, there is no evidence that the use of cholinesterase inhibitors alone or in combination with memantine is associated with a change in time to death. This study also replicated earlier findings that the use of cognitive enhancers is associated with delayed time to nursing home placement (Lopez et al. 2009). The cost-effectiveness of cognitive enhancers is uncertain, raising concerns about social justice and the appropriate use of limited resources. The cost of donepezil may be over US$120,000 per quality-adjusted year of life, and the savings resulting from delayed institutionalization may not offset the cost of medication (Loveman et al. 2006). The National Institute for Health and Clinic Excellence (NICE) in the United Kingdom has responded to these concerns by limiting the use of cholinesterase inhibitors to patients with moderately severe AD (MMSE score between 10 and 20) and recommending against the use of memantine (NICE 2007). A review of the accessibility of cognitive enhancers in 23 of 26 European Union nations revealed widely varying criteria for use, with upper limits of MMSE ranging between 20 and 30 and lower limits of MMSE between 10 and 13 for cholinesterase inhibitors; for memantine, the respective ranges were 11–26 and 0–13. There were also marked differences in policies regarding who could prescribe cognitive enhancers (general practitioners versus specialists) and reimbursement for cognitive enhancers (Oude Voshaar, Burns, and Olde Rikkert 2006). It is unlikely that such variability is solely due to different interpretations of the research literature regarding cost-effectiveness; rather, local political and financial considerations probably contribute to the variability. The combined use of a cholinesterase inhibitor and memantine may further increase costs, though a clinical trial is currently underway to determine the cost-effectiveness of combination therapy (Jones et al. 2009). Finally, it is not clear when to discontinue treatment with cognitive enhancers. In addition to evidence of modest benefit in severe dementia for cholinesterase inhibitors (Winblad et al. 2009) and memantine (Thomas and Grossberg 2009), there are reports of declines in cognition,
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
133
functioning, or behavior with discontinuation of cognitive enhancers (e.g., Daiello et al. 2009). Blass et al. (2008) found that 30% of nursing home residents with advanced dementia were prescribed cognitive enhancers, though the prevalence decreased to 10% by the time of death. A consensus panel of geriatricians at the University of Chicago concluded that the use of cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine was “never appropriate” in the palliative care of advanced dementia (Holmes et al. 2008), yet a study of 10,065 individuals with advanced dementia admitted to U.S. hospices found that 21% were prescribed a cognitive enhancer (Weschules, Maxwell, and Shega 2008). Clinicians, patients, and proxy decisionmakers should closely collaborate in the on-going monitoring of the risks and benefits of continued treatment with cognitive enhancers. Further research is required to determine at what point to discontinue treatment with cognitive enhancers. ETHICAL CHALLENGES IN THE TREATMENT OF BEHAVIORAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL SYMPTOMS OF DEMENTIA Although dementia is defined in terms of cognitive impairment and functional decline, emotional and behavioral symptoms are also very common. In fact, at least 80% of individuals with dementia will experience behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) such as depression, anxiety, psychosis, and agitation (Lyketsos et al. 2002). These symptoms may lead to poor patient quality of life, increased caregiver burden, concerns about patient safety, and higher risk of institutionalization. Atypical antipsychotics, the pharmacological agents most likely to be effective in addressing these symptoms, are associated with significant morbidity and mortality (Schneider, Dagerman, and Insel 2006). A meta-analysis of trials of atypical antipsychotics in dementia found a mortality rate of 3.5% versus 2.3% for placebo, with an odds ratio of 1.54 (95% confidence interval, 1.06–2.23) (Schneider, Dagerman, and Insel 2005). In April 2005 the U.S. FDA added a black-box warning advising against the use of antipsychotics in older adults with dementia. Clinical treatment guidelines (e.g., Lyketsos et al. 2006) recommend a sequential strategy to addressing BPSD that includes: • first identifying and addressing possible medical causes (delirium, pain, dehydration, urinary tract infection, and so on); • then using evidence-based nonpharmacologic interventions (behavioral management, cognitive stimulation, socialization); • then considering pharmacological interventions if BPSD cause significant distress or are potentially dangerous.
134
Dementia
Additional safeguards exist in nursing homes by means of U.S. federal regulations limiting the initiation of psychotropic medications to specific indications, requiring regular review of medications by pharmacists, and requiring trials of drug discontinuation (Kapp 2009, 478); see below for details. Nevertheless, pending the development of more effective treatments, antipsychotic medications will likely remain the mainstay of managing severe BPSD. Clinicians thus face the ethical dilemma of recommending a treatment that may hasten death. A further complication is that the patients themselves rarely have the capacity to consent to treatment with antipsychotics, and so a surrogate decisionmaker must be involved. Thus, a clinician considering prescribing an antipsychotic medication for BPSD must very carefully review the risks, benefits, and alternatives of such treatment with patients and their surrogates, emphasizing that these medications are not appropriate long-term solutions (Lyketsos et al. 2006). As a result of behavioral disturbances that are imminently dangerous to self or others, an individual with dementia may require a higher level of supervision and care, for example, a psychiatric hospitalization. Local regulations regarding involuntary psychiatric treatment vary tremendously, and the clinician must be familiar with the standards and laws in her or his own jurisdiction. In general, criteria for involuntary commitment include that the patient must, as a result of being mentally ill, pose an imminent danger to self, pose an imminent danger to others, or be unable to care for self; some jurisdictions allow for involuntary hospitalization if, in the absence of treatment, severe deterioration in the patient’s condition is likely (Gutheil and Appelbaum 2000, 52–53). Voluntary psychiatric hospitalization is complicated by the high rates of incapacity to make medical decisions among individuals with dementia. For example, a study of 379 older adults admitted to general medical units and then assessed for capacity found that 59% lacked medical decisionmaking capacity, with dementia and delirium being major contributors to incapacity (Mujic et al. 2009). Maxmin et al. (2009) interviewed 99 older adult psychiatric inpatients, only 52.5% of whom had capacity for admission and 38.4% for treatment decisions; again, dementia was associated with incapacity. It has been suggested that a lower threshold for capacity for admission is acceptable as long as patients agree to “enter the hospital and some in-hospital review process [is] available to pass [judgment] on the appropriateness of their decision” (Gutheil and Appelbaum 2000, 48). Because medical causes of BSPD, including delirium, are common, it is critical that a patient admitted to the hospital receive an appropriate
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
135
medical evaluation, either by admission to a medical unit initially or by medical consultation on a psychiatric unit. ELDER ABUSE AND SELF-NEGLECT Individuals with dementia are vulnerable to victimization by others or, more commonly, to self-neglect. Types of elder abuse include physical abuse, emotional or psychological abuse, sexual abuse, material abuse (i.e., financial exploitation), neglect, abandonment, and self-neglect. Rates of elder abuse range from 4.5 to 14.6 per 1,000, with an estimated 1–2 million cases each year in the United States (Jogerst et al. 2003). A recent survey of American older adults indicated that 1 in 10 had experienced elder abuse in the last year (Acierno et al. 2010). Risk factors for elder abuse include a diagnosis of dementia, presence of BPSD, low social supports, need for help with activities of daily living, female gender, low socioeconomic status, African American ethnic background, declining physical function, caregiver burden and caregiver depression/anxiety (Cooper et al. 2010; Acierno et al. 2010). Family members are implicated in 90% of elder abuse cases. After self-neglect, financial exploitation is the most common type of elder abuse (see Tueth 2000 for a review of this topic). All 50 states have laws to protect older adults from abuse and neglect, with four states (Colorado, New York, North Dakota, South Dakota) allowing for voluntary reporting and all other states requiring mandatory reporting when healthcare professionals suspect elder abuse (Walaszek 2009). Surveys of healthcare professionals indicate that 33.7–39.9% had detected elder abuse in the last year, but only half had reported suspected abuse (Cooper et al. 2009). Face-to-face training of health care professionals about the management of suspected abuse appears to increase knowledge and increase reporting rates (Cooper, Selwood, and Livingston 2009). Clinicians should familiarize themselves with local laws regarding the reporting of elder abuse. Those working with patients with dementia should maintain a high index of suspicion of self-neglect. Self-neglect is the most common form of elder abuse and can be difficult to detect because individuals with dementia may not be able to provide accurate reports of their functioning; caregivers and family members are typically not available to report the self-neglect. Self-neglect has been associated with a marked increase in risk of mortality (Dong et al. 2009). Concern about self-neglect should lead to a clinician formally assessing the individual’s capacity to live independently, as described above. Relevant local authorities tasked with addressing elder abuse may need to be contacted.
136
Dementia
Clinicians face an ethical quandary when they suspect elder abuse, but the elder does not wish to have the abuse reported; this represents a tension between the ethical principles of beneficence and autonomy. In such situations, clinicians will need to carefully evaluate the reasons why the patient does not want abuse reported (e.g., fear of retaliation), determine if the patient believes s/he is abused, and assess the patient’s capacity to refuse further intervention. Of course, if there is evidence that an individual is in immediate danger, local authorities must be contacted. ETHICAL ISSUES RELEVANT TO LONG-TERM CARE FACILITIES Among the most challenging transitions for an individual with dementia is the move from one’s home into a long-term care facility. Ethically, institutionalization is a response to the changing balance between the principles of autonomy and beneficence. That is, as an individual with dementia becomes progressively less able to care for herself or himself, some autonomy may be lost in order to support her or his well-being. The individual with dementia may not have the capacity to make this decision or may actively refuse admission to a long-term care facility, thereby necessitating the involvement of surrogate decisionmakers or legal processes such as guardianship. The assessment of capacity to live independently is discussed above. Clinicians and their patients appear to disagree frequently on the appropriateness of institutionalization (Bartels et al. 2003) and there are significant cross-cultural differences in attitudes toward long-term care (McCormick et al. 2002). Individuals in long-term care face substantial limitations on making personal choices, affecting their autonomy and voluntarism (Kapp 1998). Thus, when an individual with dementia is no longer able to live independently, careful consideration should be made of her or his beliefs and values when determining an appropriate next step. For example, alternatives to institutionalization may include increased services within one’s home and increased support for family members serving as caregivers. Though only 4% of individuals with dementia live in a long-term care facility at any one time (Matthews and Dening 2002), approximately 90% will eventually move to a long-term care facility, compared to 50% of nondemented older adults (Smith, Kokmen, and O’Brien 2000). The majority of nursing home residents (56% in one estimate) have dementia (Matthews and Dening 2002), and the rates of incapacity to make medical decisions are high (Moye and Marson 2007).
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
137
Extensive U.S. federal regulations (referred to collectively as OBRA 1987) govern the behavior of nursing homes (Kapp 2009, 466). Nursing homes must comply with Medicaid regulations (CMS, or Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services), with the Americans with Disabilities Act and the Rehabilitation Act, with standards of accrediting bodies (e.g., the Joint Commission), and with state regulations. These measures are intended to insure the appropriate treatment of nursing home residents with neuropsychiatric issues, including dementia and BPSD. Screening must take place prior to admission to a nursing home to determine if a patient has mental-health needs and if that facility can offer appropriate services for those needs (Kapp 2009, 468, 469). Residential care and assisted living facilities have emerged as common alternatives to nursing homes; rates of dementia and BPSD are similar in these settings to nursing homes, but with less oversight and with regulation that varies from state to state (Gruber-Baldini et al. 2004). The use of physical restraints has increasingly been recognized as often “unnecessary, improper, and even abusive.” The CMS have encouraged the aggressive enforcement of federal regulations limiting the use of “any physical restraints imposed or psychoactive drug administered for purposes of discipline or convenience, and not required to treat the resident’s medical symptoms” (Kapp 2009, 476, 477). In order to respect the autonomy and preserve the dignity of nursing home residents, facilities should address BPSD by employing nonpharmacologic interventions (as described above), improving the environment (e.g., more activities for residents to prevent boredom), and/or making administrative or staff changes. A randomized controlled trial in four Norwegian nursing homes of a two-day staff training followed by six monthly meetings was associated with decreased use of restraints at 6-month follow-up (though not at 12 months), and decreased BPSD among nursing home residents at both 6 months and 12 months (Testad et al. 2010). This suggests that training, regular monitoring and on-going guidance are necessary to promote and maintain changes in the use of restraints. A goal of OBRA 1987 regulations has been to prevent the use of psychotropic medications as “chemical restraints” or for environmental control, though it is recognized that there are appropriate uses of psychotropic medications. OBRA 1987 has resulted in decreased use of antipsychotic medications and increased use of antidepressant medications, the former through strict guidelines about the use of antipsychotics and the latter through increased detection of depression (Lantz, Giambanco, and Buchalter 1996). As described above, antipsychotic medications may
138
Dementia
have a role in the treatment of severe BPSD, but this decision should be weighed carefully with proxy decisionmakers. OBRA 1987 requires that antipsychotic medications be given only if they are “necessary to treat a specific condition as diagnosed and documented in the clinical record.” CMS considers a “dementing illness with associated behavioral symptoms” a valid indication for antipsychotic medications, but the symptoms must be dangerous to self or others or must result in severe distress or decline in functioning. The resident’s record must include documentation of the decisionmaking process, including diagnostic evaluation of BPSD, alternative nonpharmacologic interventions, and risk-benefit analysis. Furthermore, efforts must be made to discontinue antipsychotic medications (unless clinically contraindicated) by means of gradual dose reductions and behavioral interventions (CMS 2009). Clinicians prescribing psychotropic medications in long-term care facilities should familiarize themselves with relevant regulations. RESEARCH INVOLVING INDIVIDUALS WITH DEMENTIA Clinical research is essential for developing effective preventive, diagnostic, and treatment strategies in dementia care. However, by virtue of their cognitive impairment, individuals with dementia may have diminished capacity to consent to participate in research. In this section, we discuss assessing the capacity of a potential research subject to provide informed consent, methods of increasing capacity to participate in research, the use of surrogate decisionmakers to provide informed consent, and advance directives for participation in research. Dunn and Misra (2009) have reviewed the literature on ethical issues in research involving older adults with neuropsychiatric disorders, including dementia. They note that a research setting is inherently different from a clinical setting, as “participation is voluntary and the risk-to-benefit ratio is typically skewed toward more direct risk with less direct benefit.” Individuals with AD have diminished capacity to consent to clinical research compared with older adults with schizophrenia or diabetes mellitus (Palmer et al. 2005). Capacity varies significantly among those with AD, though severity of cognitive impairment is a strong predictor of capacity and individuals with AD eventually lose capacity (Dunn and Misra 2009). The MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Clinical Research (MacCAT-CR) (Appelbaum and Grisso 2001) is a widely accepted and well-validated instrument to measure capacity to participate in research (Grisso and Appelbaum 1995). This tool is analogous to the MacCAT-T
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
139
described above. The MacCAT-CR is adapted for discussion of the specific research study; over the course of 15–20 minutes, information is gathered about the potential subject’s understanding, appreciation, reasoning, and ability to express a choice. Though some element of subjectivity remains in the assessment of capacity using the MacCAT-CR, this systematic and structured approach may allow for specific impairments in decisional capacity to be identified and perhaps addressed (Dunn and Misra 2009). Methods have been developed to enhance an individual’s capacity to participate in research. Mittal and colleagues (2007) studied the effectiveness of a multimedia presentation describing a hypothetical clinical trial versus that of an “enhanced written consent procedure” in 35 subjects with AD or mild cognitive impairment. As assessed by the MacCAT-CR, subjects in both study groups demonstrated an improvement in their understanding of the trial. Further research is required in larger samples, in more diverse populations, and involving different types of hypothetical studies. Surrogate decisionmakers may, under certain circumstances, provide consent for participation in research. According to an Alzheimer ’s Association position statement (1997), a proxy may provide consent in the following circumstances: • if the research entails minimal risk to the individual with dementia; • if the research entails greater than minimal risk, but there is also “a reasonable potential for benefit to the individual”; • if the research entails greater than minimal risk and there is no reasonable potential for benefit, but the individual had executed an advance directive for research (see below)—in which case, the role of the surrogate is to “monitor the individual’s involvement in the research.” It should be noted, however, that standards vary significantly among organizations, regulatory bodies, and legal statutes, so individual institutional review boards have reached different conclusions regarding the appropriateness of surrogate consent. Surveys of older Americans seem to indicate strong public support for surrogate consent for dementia research. Kim et al. (2009) surveyed a subset (N=1,515) of subjects from the Health and Retirement Study, a nationally representative study of persons 51 and older. Subjects were randomized to review one of four plausible AD research scenarios: a study involving lumbar puncture (LP), a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of a new drug, a vaccine study, or a gene transfer neurosurgical study. When
140
Dementia
subjects were asked to consider if they would allow a close family member to provide consent for them to participate in these hypothetical studies, 70.8% responded affirmatively to the LP study, 79.7% to the RCT, 57.4% to the vaccine study, and 68.7% to the gene transfer study. When subjects were asked if our society should allow families to consent for individuals who cannot consent themselves, the affirmative responses were, respectively, 72.0%, 82.5%, 70.5% and 67.5%. Predictors of assenting to proxy consent included a personal willingness to participate in research, female gender, being married (versus not married), and excellent self-related health; ethnic background was not a predictor, and support for proxy consent was strong among African Americans and Hispanics. Other studies (e.g., Karlawish et al. 2009) have similarly demonstrated strong public support for surrogate consent for dementia research. The use of a surrogate begs the question of what method the surrogate should use when deciding whether or not to enroll an individual with dementia in a study. As noted above, there are two standards governing the behavior of surrogate decisionmakers: best interest and substituted judgment (Gutheil and Appelbaum 2000, 232). Studies investigating which standard surrogates follow in dementia research have generally yielded support for the best interest model. Karlawish, Kim, et al. (2008) surveyed the views of research subjects and their “study partners” enrolled in a randomized control trial of simvastatin for the treatment of AD; the results indicated that study partners (some of whom were also surrogate decisionmakers) followed the best-interest standard. Interestingly, study partners were highly involved in decisionmaking whether or not they were formally identified as surrogates, suggesting that a shared decisionmaking model, as described above, may apply in dementia research. It is possible that an individual with intact decisionmaking capacity could execute an advance directive for participation in dementia research. However, a controlled trial of a research advanced directive in 149 subjects with dementia and their proxies did not demonstrate altered study enrollment rates, decision ease or proxy comfort and certainty compared to a control group (Stocking et al. 2007). Furthermore, concerns have been raised about the inability of a research advance directive to address the specific information required to consent to a particular study (Dunn and Misra 2009). END-OF-LIFE CARE IN INDIVIDUALS WITH DEMENTIA The terminal stage of dementia is marked by severe cognitive impairment and a high rate of morbidity and mortality. For example, a prospective
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
141
18-month study of nursing home residents with advanced dementia (mean MMSE score of 5) revealed a mortality rate of 54.8%; medical morbidities included poor eating in 85.5% of residents, a febrile episode in 52.6%, and pneumonia in 41.1%, each of which in turn increased the risk of death. Distressing symptoms were common: 46.0% of residents experienced dyspnea and 39.1% had pain. Advanced dementia has a life expectancy similar to metastatic breast cancer and stage IV congestive heart failure (Mitchell et al. 2009). It is therefore clinically and ethically appropriate to employ a palliative care approach for individuals who have severe dementia, including a focus on promoting patient comfort, avoiding hospitalization and surgery, respecting advance directives, and employing do-not-resuscitate orders (American Academy of Neurology Ethics and Humanities Subcommittee 1996). Post (2000) also presents this issue in terms of social justice: The cost to society of interventions to extend the life of patients who have advanced dementia may not be just or justifiable. Whereas a terminally ill but cognitively intact individual may personally decide to receive care that is focused on comfort, a proxy decisionmaker must be involved in palliative-care decisions for individuals with dementia. Mitchell et al. (2009) found that only 18.0% of proxies had received prognostic information from a physician. Patients whose proxies believed that the patient had less than 6 months to live and understood the clinical complications of dementia were markedly less likely to undergo burdensome interventions (hospitalization, tube-feeding, parenteral therapy) (Mitchell at al. 2009). Thus, surrogate decisionmakers must receive adequate information and support in order to ensure that individuals with advanced dementia receive appropriate palliative care (Hertogh 2006). Interestingly, clinicians themselves may need further education in this regard: In a study of patients with advanced dementia admitted to nursing homes, only 1.1% were perceived to have a life expectancy of less than six months, whereas 71.0% died during that period (Mitchell, Kiely, and Hamel 2004). Among the most challenging steps is the withholding of artificial hydration and nutrition. As noted above, stopping eating is often a precursor of death in individuals with dementia. Feeding tubes probably do not reduce a patient’s suffering and may, in fact, cause suffering (Gillick 2000). An Alzheimer ’s Association position statement argues that it is ethically permissible to withhold nutrition and artificial hydration from individuals who refuse to eat or drink (Alzheimer ’s Association 1988). Roberts and Dyer (2004, 186–191) provide a useful framework for ethical issues at the end of life, including addressing the patient’s physical and psychological pain; ensuring that all decisions are consistent with
142
Dementia
the patient’s values, preferences, and spiritual beliefs; addressing difficult issues with the patient and caregivers; and consulting all relevant parties, including family members, other healthcare providers, attorneys, and spiritual figures. CONCLUSIONS Many clinical decisions in the care of individuals with dementia are inherently ethical decisions. The very nature of dementia is to progressively rob an individual of her or his capacity to remember, to make personal decisions, and to care for oneself—thereby striking at the core of autonomy. Yet, each individual is unique in terms of her or his own beliefs, cultural background, personality, life experiences, relationships, and progression of illness. Thus, clinicians, patients, and their caregivers must work together to carefully review the risks and benefits of options related to the diagnosis and treatment of dementia, management of distressing symptoms, participation in research trials, institutionalization, and endof-life issues. Because individuals with dementia are vulnerable to abuse and exploitation, clinicians must be especially vigilant for any evidence of elder abuse. Given aging world populations, discussions must take place at a societal level regarding the fair and just distribution of medical resources—while not forgetting that it is our moral obligation to address the suffering of individuals with dementia. REFERENCES ABIM Foundation. 2002. Medical professionalism in the new millennium: A physician charter. Annals of Internal Medicine 136: 243–246. Acierno, R., M. A. Hernandez, A. B. Amstadter, H. S. Resnick, K. Steve, W. Muzzy, and D. G. Kilpatrick. 2010. Prevalence and correlates of emotional, physical, sexual, and financial abuse and potential neglect in the United States: The National Elder Mistreatment Study. American Journal of Public Health 100: 292–297. Alzheimer ’s Association. 1988. Treatment of patients with advanced dementia. http://www.alz.org/national/documents/statements_advancedementia. pdf (accessed April 26, 2010). Alzheimer ’s Association. 1997. Ethical issues in dementia research (with special emphasis on “informed consent”). http://www.alz.org/national/documents/ statements_ethicalissues.pdf (accessed April 24, 2010). Alzheimer ’s Association. 2008. Genetic testing. http://www.alz.org/national/ documents/statements_genetictesting.pdf (accessed April 24, 2010).
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
143
American Academy of Neurology Ethics and Humanities Subcommittee. 1996. Ethical issues in the management of the demented patient. Neurology 46: 1180–1183. Appelbaum, P. S., R. J. Bonnie, and J. H. Karlawish. 2005. The capacity to vote of persons with Alzheimer ’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry 162: 2094–2100. Appelbaum, P. S., and T. Grisso. 2001. MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for clinical research. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press. Bartels, S. J., K. M. Miles, A. R. Dums, and K. J. Levine. 2003. Are nursing homes appropriate for older adults with severe mental illness? Conflicting consumer and clinician views and implications for the Olmstead decision. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 51: 1571–1579. Blass, D. M., B. S. Black, H. Phillips, T. Finucane, A. Baker, D. Loreck, and P. V. Rabins. 2008. Medication use in nursing home residents with advanced dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23: 490–496. Cassidy, M. R., J. S. Roberts, T. D. Bird, E. J. Steinbart, L. A. Cupples, C. A. Chen, E. Linnenbringer, and R. C. Green. 2008. Comparing test-specific distress of susceptibility versus deterministic genetic testing for Alzheimer ’s disease. Alzheimer ’s and Dementia 4: 406–413. Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. 2009. State operation manual. Appendix PP: Guidance to surveys for long-term care facilities. http:// www.cms.gov/manuals/Downloads/som107ap_pp_guidelines_ltcf.pdf (accessed April 27, 2010). Christensen, K. D., J. S. Roberts, C. D. Royal, G. A. Fasaye, T. Obisesan, L. A. Cupples, P. J. Whitehouse, et al. 2008. Incorporating ethnicity into genetic risk assessment for Alzheimer disease: The REVEAL study experience. Genetics in Medicine 10: 207–214. Cooper, C., A. Selwood, M. Blanchard, Z. Walker, R. Blizard, and G. Livingston. 2010. The determinants of family carers’ abusive behaviour to people with dementia: Results of the CARD study. Journal of Affective Disorders 121: 136–142. Cooper, C., A. Selwood, and G. Livingston. 2009. Knowledge, detection, and reporting of abuse by health and social care professionals: A systematic review. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 826–838 Daiello, L. A., B. R. Ott, K. L. Lapane, S. E. Reinert, J. T. Machan, and D. D. Dore. 2009. Effect of discontinuing cholinesterase inhibitor therapy on behavioral and mood symptoms in nursing home patients with dementia. American Journal of Geriatric Pharmacotherapy 7: 74–83. Dong, X., M. Simon, C. Mendes de Leon, T. Fulmer, T. Beck, L. Hebert, C. Dyer, G. Paveza, and D. Evans. 2009. Elder self-neglect and abuse and mortality risk in a community-dwelling population. JAMA 302: 517–526. Drane, J. F. 1984. Measuring decision-making capacity in cognitively impaired individuals. JAMA 252: 925–927.
144
Dementia
Dubinsky, R. M., A. C. Stein, and K. Lyons. 2000. Practice parameter: risk of driving and Alzheimer ’s disease (an evidence-based review): Report of the quality standards subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 54: 2205–2211. Dunn, L. B., and S. Misra. 2009. Research ethics issues in geriatric psychiatry. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 32: 395–411. Dunn, L. B., M. A. Nowrangi, B. W. Palmer, D. V. Jeste, and E. R. Saks. 2006. Assessing decisional capacity for clinical research or treatment: A review of instruments. American Journal of Psychiatry 163: 1323–1334. Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh. 1975. “Mini-Mental State”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12: 189–198. Gillick, M. R. 2000. Rethinking the role of tube feeding in patients with advanced dementia. New England Journal of Medicine 342: 206–210. Green, R. C., J. S. Roberts, L. A. Cupples, N. R. Relkin, P. J. Whitehouse, T. Brown, S. L. Eckert, et al. 2009. Disclosure of APOE genotype for risk of Alzheimer ’s disease. New England Journal of Medicine 361: 245–254. Grimes, A. L., L. B. McCullough, M. E. Kunik, V. Molinari, and R. H. Workman Jr. 2000. Informed consent and neuroanatomic correlates of intentionality and voluntariness among psychiatric patients. Psychiatric Services 51: 1561–1567. Grisso, T, and P. S. Appelbaum. 1995. The MacArthur treatment competence study. III: Abilities of patients to consent to psychiatric and medical treatments. Law and Human Behavior 19: 149–174. Grisso, T., P. S. Appelbaum, and C. Hill-Fotouhi. 1997. The MacCAT-T: A clinical tool to assess patients’ capacities to make treatment decisions. Psychiatric Services 48: 1415–1419. Gruber-Baldini, A. L., M. Boustani, P. D. Sloane, and S. Zimmerman. 2004. Behavioral symptoms in residential care/assisted living facilities: Prevalence, risk factors, and medication management. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 52: 1610–1617. Gurrera, R. J., J. Moye, M. J. Karel, A. R. Azar, and J. C. Armesto. 2006. Cognitive performance predicts treatment decisional abilities in mild to moderate dementia. Neurology 66: 1367–1372. Gutheil, T. G., and P. S. Appelbaum. 2000. Clinical Handbook of Psychiatry and the Law, 3rd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Hertogh, C. M. 2006. Advance care planning and the relevance of a palliative care approach in dementia. Age and Ageing 35: 553–555. Holmes, H. M., G. A. Sachs, J. W. Shega, G. W. Hougham, H. D. Cox, and W. Dale. 2008. Integrating palliative medicine into the care of persons with advanced dementia: Identifying appropriate medication use. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56: 1306–1311. Huizing, A. R., R. L. Berghmans, G. A. Widdershoven, and F. R. Verhey. 2006. Do caregivers’ experiences correspond with the concerns raised in the
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
145
literature? Ethical issues relating to anti-dementia drugs. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21: 869–875. Huthwaite, J. S., R. C. Martin, H. R. Griffith, B. Anderson, L. E. Harrell, and D. C. Marson. 2006. Declining medical decision-making capacity in mild AD: A two-year longitudinal study. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 24: 453–463. Jogerst, G. J., J. M. Daly, M. F. Brinig, J. D. Dawson, G. A. Schmuch, and J. G. Ingram. 2003. Domestic elder abuse and the law. American Journal of Public Health 12: 2131–2136. Jones, R., B. Sheehan, P. Phillips, E. Juszczak, J. Adams, A. Baldwin, C. Ballard, et al. 2009. DOMINO-AD protocol: Donepezil and memantine in moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease—a multicentre RCT. Trials 10: 57. Kapp, M. B. 1998. “A place like that”: Advance directives and nursing home admissions. Psychology, Public Policy and Law 4: 805–828. Kapp, M. B. 2002. Decisional capacity in theory and practice: legal process versus “bumbling through.” Aging and Mental Health 6: 413–417. Kapp, M. B. 2009. Ethical and medicolegal issues. In Psychiatry in long-term care, 2nd ed., ed. W. E. Reichman and P. R. Katz. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Karlawish, J. 2008. Measuring decision-making capacity in cognitively impaired individuals. Neurosignals 16: 91–98. Karlawish, J. H., R. J. Bonnie, P. S. Appelbaum, R. A. Kane, C. G. Lyketsos, P. S. Karlan, B. D. James, C. Sabatino, T. Lawrence, and D. Knopman. 2008. Identifying the barriers and challenges to voting by residents in nursing homes and assisted living settings. Journal of Aging and Social Policy 20: 65–79. Karlawish, J. H., R. J. Bonnie, P. S. Appelbaum, C. Lyketsos, B. James, D. Knopman, C. Patusky, R. A. Kane, and P. S. Karlan. 2004. Addressing the ethical, legal, and social issues raised by voting by persons with dementia. JAMA 292: 1345–1350. Karlawish, J. H., D. J. Casarett, B. D. James, S. X. Xie, and S. Y. Kim. 2005. The ability of persons with Alzheimer disease (AD) to make a decision about taking an AD treatment. Neurology 64: 1514–1519. Karlawish, J., S. Y. Kim, D. Knopman, C. H. van Dyck, B. D. James, and D. Marson. 2008. The views of Alzheimer disease patients and their study partners on proxy consent for clinical trial enrollment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16: 240–247. Karlawish, J., J. Rubright, D. Casarett, M. Cary, T. Ten Have, and P. Sankar. 2009. Older adults’ attitudes toward enrollment of non-competent subjects participating in Alzheimer ’s research. American Journal of Psychiatry 166: 131–134. Kim, S. Y., and E. D. Caine. 2002. Utility and limits of the Mini Mental State Examination in evaluating consent capacity in Alzheimer ’s disease. Psychiatric Services 53: 1322–1324. Kim, S. Y., J. H. Karlawish, and E. D. Caine. 2002. Current state of research on decision-making competence of cognitively impaired elderly persons. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10: 151–165.
146
Dementia
Kim, S. Y., H. M. Kim, K. M. Langa, J. H. Karlawish, D. S. Knopman, and P. S. Appelbaum. 2009. Surrogate consent for dementia research: a national survey of older Americans. Neurology 72: 149–155. Lantz, M. S., V. Giambanco, and E. N. Buchalter. 1996. A ten-year review of the effect of OBRA-87 on psychotropic prescribing practices in an academic nursing home. Psychiatric Services 47: 951–955. Lichtenberg, P. A., and D. M. Strzepek. 1990. Assessments of institutionalized dementia patients’ competencies to participate in intimate relationships. Gerontologist 30: 117–120. Lopez, O. L., J. T. Becker, A. S. Wahed, J. Saxton, R. A. Sweet, D. A. Wolk, W. Klunk, and S. T. Dekosky. 2009. Long-term effects of the concomitant use of memantine with cholinesterase inhibition in Alzheimer disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 80: 600–607. Loveman, E., C. Green, J. Kirby, A. Takeda, J. Picot, E. Payne, and A. Clegg. 2006. The clinical and cost-effectiveness of donepezil, rivastigmine, galantamine and memantine for Alzheimer ’s disease. Health Technology Assessment 10: 1–151. Lyketsos, C. G., O. Lopez, B. Jones, A. L. Fitzpatrick, J. Breitner, and S. DeKosky. 2002. Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: results from the cardiovascular health study. JAMA 288: 1475–1483. Lyketsos, C. G., C. C. Colenda, C. Beck, K. Blank, M. P. Doraiswamy, D. A. Kalunian, K. Yaffe, and Task Force of American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006. Position statement of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry regarding principles of care for patients with dementia resulting from Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14: 561–572. Marson, D. C., S. M. Sawrie, S. Snyder, B. McInturff, T. Stalvey, A. Boothe, T. Aldridge, A. Chatterjee and L. E. Harrell. 2000. Assessing financial capacity in patients with Alzheimer disease: A conceptual model and prototype instrument. Archives of Neurology 57: 877–884. Matthews, F. E., and T. Dening for the UK Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study. 2002. Prevalence of dementia in institutional care. Lancet 360: 225–226. Maxmin, K., C. Cooper, L. Potter, and G. Livingston. 2009. Mental capacity to consent to treatment and admission decisions in older adult psychiatric inpatients. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24: 1367–1375. McCormick, W. C., C. Y. Ohata, J. Uomoto, H. M. Young, A. B. Graves, W. Kukull, L. Teri, et al. 2002. Similarities and differences in attitudes toward long-term care between Japanese Americans and Caucasian Americans. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 50: 1149–1155. Mitchell, S. L, D. K. Kiely, and M. B. Hamel. 2004. Dying with advanced dementia in the nursing home. Archives of Internal Medicine 164: 321–326. Mitchell, S. L., J. M. Teno, D. K. Kiely, M. L. Shaffer, R. N. Jones, H. G. Prigerson, L. Volicer, J. L. Givens, and M. B. Hamel. 2009. The clinical course of advanced dementia. NEJM 361: 1529–1538.
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
147
Mittal, D., B. W. Palmer, L. B. Dunn, R. Landes, C. Ghormley, C. Beck, S. Golshan, D. Blevins, and D. V. Jeste. 2007. Comparison of two enhanced consent procedures for patients with mild Alzheimer disease or mild cognitive impairment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 15: 163–167. Moye, J., S. W. Butz, D. C. Marson, E. Wood, and ABA-APA. 2007. Capacity assessment of older adults working group: A conceptual model and assessment template for capacity evaluation in adult guardianship. Gerontologist 47: 591–603. Moye, J., M. J. Karel, R. J. Gurrera, and A. R. Azar. 2006. Neuropsychological predictors of decision-making capacity over 9 months in mild-to-moderate dementia. Journal of General Internal Medicine 21: 78–83. Moye, J., and D. C. Marson. 2007. Assessment of decision-making capacity in older adults: An emerging area of practice and research. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 62: P3–P11. Mujic, F., M. Von Heising, R. J. Stewart, and M. J. Prince. 2009. Mental capacity assessments among general hospital inpatients referred to a specialist liaison psychiatry service for older people. International Psychogeriatrics 21: 729–737. National Human Genome Research Institute (NIH). 2008. Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act (GINA) of 2008. http://www.genome.gov/24519851 (accessed April 25, 2010). National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE). 2007. Donepezil, galantamine, rivastigmine (review) and memantine for the treatment of Alzheimer ’s disease (amended). http://www.nice.org.uk/TA111 (accessed April 27, 2010). Okonkwo, O. C., V. G. Wadley, H. R. Griffith, K. Belue, S. Lanza, E. Y. Zamrini, L. E. Harrell, et al. 2008. Awareness of deficits in financial abilities in patients with mild cognitive impairment: Going beyond self-informant discrepancy. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16: 650–659. Oude Voshaar, R. C., A. Burns, and M. G. M. Olde Rikkert. 2006. Alarming arbitrariness in EU prescription and reimbursement criteria for anti-dementia drugs. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21: 29–31. Palmer, B. W., L. B. Dunn, P. S. Appelbaum, S. Mudaliar, L. Thal, R. Henry, S. Golshan, and D. V. Jeste. 2005. Assessment of capacity to consent to research among older persons with schizophrenia, Alzheimer disease, or diabetes mellitus: Comparison of a 3-item questionnaire with a comprehensive standardized capacity instrument. Archives of General Psychiatry 62: 726–733. Pickens, S., A. D. Naik, J. Burnett, P. A. Kelly, M. Gleason, and C. B. Dyer. 2007. The utility of the Kohlman Evaluation of Living Skills test is associated with substantiated cases of elder self-neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practitioners 19: 137–142. Post, S. G. 2000. Key issues in the ethics of dementia care. Neurology Clinics 18: 1011–1022.
148
Dementia
Post, S. G., J. C. Stuckey, P. J. Whitehouse, S. Ollerton, C. Durkin, D. Robbins, and S. J. Fallcreek. 2001. A focus group on cognition-enhancing medications in Alzheimer disease: Disparities between professionals and consumers. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 15: 80–88. Post, S. G., and P. J. Whitehouse. 1998. Emerging antidementia drugs: A preliminary ethical view. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 46: 784–787. Post, S. G., P. J. Whitehouse, R. H. Binstock, T. D. Bird, S. K. Eckert, L. A. Farrer, L. M. Fleck, et al. 1997. The clinical introduction of genetic testing for Alzheimer disease: an ethical perspective. JAMA 277 (1997): 832–836. Qaseem, A., V. Snow, J. T. Cross Jr., M. A. Forciea, R. Hopkins Jr., P. Shekelle, A. Adelman, et al. 2008. Current pharmacologic treatment of dementia: A clinical practice guideline from the American College of Physicians and the American Academy of Family Physicians. Annals of Internal Medicine 148: 370–378. Roberts, L. W. 2002. Informed consent and the capacity for voluntarism. American Journal of Psychiatry 159: 705–712. Roberts, J. S., L. A. Cupples, N. R. Relkin, P. J. Whitehouse, R. C. Green, and REVEAL (Risk Evaluation and Education for Alzheimer ’s Disease) Study Group. 2005. Genetic risk assessment for adult children of people with Alzheimer ’s disease: The Risk Evaluation and Education for Alzheimer ’s Disease (REVEAL) study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 18: 250–255. Roberts, L. W., and A. R. Dyer. 2004. Concise Guide to Ethics in Mental Health Care. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. Royall, D. R., L. K. Chiodo, and M. J. Polk. 2005. An empiric approach to level of care determinations: The importance of executive measures. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 60: 1056–1064. Schneider, L. S., K. Dagerman, and P. S. Insel. 2005. Risk of death with atypical antipsychotic drug treatment for dementia: Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. JAMA 294: 1934–1943. Schneider, L. S., K. Dagerman, and P. S. Insel. 2006. Efficacy and adverse effects of atypical antipsychotics for dementia: Meta-analysis of randomized, placebo-controlled trials. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14: 191–210. Shulman, K. I., C. Peisah, R. Jacoby, J. Heinik, and S. Finkel. 2009. Contemporaneous assessment of testamentary capacity. International Psychogeriatrics 21: 433–439. Smith, G. E., E. Kokmen, and P. C. O’Brien. 2000. Risk factors for nursing home placement in a population-based dementia cohort. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 48: 519–525. Stocking, C. B., G. W. Hougham, D. D. Danner, M. B. Patterson, P. J. Whitehouse, and G. A. Sachs. 2007. Empirical assessment of a research advance directive for persons with dementia and their proxies. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 55: 1609–1612.
Ethical Issues in the Care of Individuals with Dementia
149
Sugarman, J., D. C. McCrory, and R. C. Hubal. 1998. Getting meaningful informed consent from older adults: A structured literature review of empirical research. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 46: 517–524. Testad, I., C. Ballard, K. Brønnick, and D. Aarsland. 2010. The effect of staff training on agitation and use of restraint in nursing home residents with dementia: A single-blind, randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 71: 80–86. Thomas, S. J., and G. T. Grossberg. 2009. Memantine: A review of studies into its safety and efficacy in treating Alzheimer ’s disease and other dementias. Clinical Interventions in Aging 4: 367–377. Tueth, M. J. 2000. Exposing financial exploitation of impaired elderly person. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 8: 104–111. Walaszek, A. 2009. Clinical ethics issues in geriatric psychiatry. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 32: 343–359. Weschules, D. J., T. L. Maxwell, and J. W. Shega. 2008. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitor and N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor antagonist use among hospice enrollees with a primary diagnosis of dementia. Journal of Palliative Medicine 11: 738–745. Willis, S. L., R. Allen-Burge, M. M. Dolan, R. M. Bertrand, J. Yesavage, and J. L. Taylor. 1998. Everyday problem solving among individuals with Alzheimer ’s disease. Gerontologist 38: 569–577. Winblad, B., S. E. Black, A. Homma, E. M. Schwam, M. Moline, Y. Xu, C. A. Perdomo, J. Swartz, and K. Albert. 2009. Donepezil treatment in severe Alzheimer ’s disease: A pooled analysis of three clinical trials. Current Medical Research and Opinion 25: 2577–2587.
Chapter 7
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological and Functional Assessment: Contributions to Early Detection of Dementia Mônica Sanches Yassuda, Mariana Kneese Flaks, and Fernanda Speggiorin Pereira
As the elderly population increases, dementia and depression have become the most prevalent neuropsychiatric disorders among aged individuals (Ferri et al. 2005). Considering the fact that dementia is a neurodegenerative disease with progressive neuronal loss, it becomes a medical and social problem in a large and growing scale. The final diagnosis of most dementia syndromes depends on neuropathologic examination. However, a thorough clinical examination including anamnesis, psychiatric evaluation, neuropsychological assessment, and physical and neurological examination, combined with biochemical tests and neuroimaging, could enhance the accuracy of early and differential diagnosis. Technological innovations that use structural and functional neuroimaging techniques as well as molecular biology and molecular genetics procedures have provided new tools that can facilitate the early diagnosis of dementias, especially of Alzheimer ’s disease (Ho et al. 2010; Hampel et al. 2008; Shaw et al. 2007). The expected advances in pharmacological treatment, seeking to modify pathogenic processes, increase the need to identify the disease in its early stages, before dysfunctional and severe cognitive deficits are established
152
Dementia
(Bischkopf, Busse, and Angermeyer 2002). As a result, differential diagnosis carries therapeutic and prognostic implications. Despite the important scientific progress related to imaging and biological markers, the diagnosis of dementia syndromes remains a clinical process, supplemented by relevant investigations such as cognitive testing (Portet et al. 2006). Due to the difficulties in identifying the early signs of dementia only by means of clinical assessment and routine medical examination, cognitive assessment is a key tool that can improve diagnostic accuracy. In this context, cognitive screening tools, which are used at the beginning of the diagnostic process, and the neuropsychological instruments, which are used in more extensive investigations, are required. Neuropsychological assessment can often point toward patterns of cognitive alteration that are typical of dementia. The tests are sensitive to brain dysfunction and identify damaged areas that are not yet evident in imaging and in an electroencephalogram. Neuropsychological testing may define the location and lateralization of brain dysfunction related to behavioral impairment by quantifying changes in sensory or motor functions and by examining more complex brain functions such as language, visual-spatial awareness, verbal and nonverbal memory (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). Neuropsychological assessment is important when the clinical pattern is ambiguous or complex, making it possible to reliably identify different types of dementia early in the course of the disease, and to distinguish dementia from normal aging or from other illnesses. Additionally, it provides guidance to physicians and to family members with regards to deficit compensation strategies for a particular patient. It can also provide information concerning conduct and therapeutic options during the disease (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). Furthermore, consecutive cognitive testing presents objective data as to whether changes in the clinical profile are occurring as expected for a given diagnosis. In what follows, we review a range of validated neuropsychological assessment tools that can facilitate accurate diagnosis of an early dementing process in aged individuals. We provide guidance on interpretive strengths and weakness of each of these tools. We emphasize our experience within the Brazilian context in order to provide a more global picture of the contributions of neuropsychological markers for the dementias, in addition to the standard North American account of this emerging area of study and practice. According to criteria from the National Institute for Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer ’s Disease and Related Disorders
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
153
Association (NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group—Dubois et al. 2007), before recommending extensive and often costly neuropsychological assessment, screening tools should be used to verify the need for this procedure in case of possible dementia. The purpose of the screening tool is different from that of the neuropsychological instrument. The screening process indicates the likelihood of dementia. If results from the screening are positive, the assessment may ratify or reject the diagnostic hypothesis. COGNITIVE SCREENING TOOLS Shulman and Feinstein (2003) state that the ideal screening test should be: 1. Brief 2. Well accepted by patients without giving rise to discomfort or defensive reactions 3. Easy to apply and to review 4. Relatively free of confounding elements such as schooling, culture and language 5. Have good psychometric properties such as reliability and validity regardless of the tester and in test-retest situations, sensitivity, specificity, and high positive and negative predictability 6. Cover a representative range of intellectual functions On the other hand, screening tests should be used and interpreted with caution, considering that there is no flawless cognitive assessment tool. Special attention should be paid to the likelihood of a high percentage of false-negatives, when the screening is conducted in early stage dementia, or in individuals with high intellectual levels or with many years of schooling (Katzman 1993; Stern et al. 1994; Cummings et al. 1998). Falsepositive results may occur among healthy individuals with low schooling. Few years of schooling and illiteracy are associated with a greater prevalence of dementia (De Ronchi et al. 1998; Herrera et al. 2002; Bottino et al. 2008), yet many individuals who cannot read score within ranges typical of dementia even though their functions are completely preserved. This emphasizes the importance of tests that are less vulnerable to educational experience or for normative values corrected for education. Special attention should be paid to developing countries (Ferri et al. 2005; Yassuda et al. 2009), where underschooling is frequently observed among elders. Some cognitive screening tools specifically designed for suspected dementia are described below. Diagnostic accuracy data is also reviewed.
154
Dementia
Mini Mental State Examination The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975) is a widely used and studied screening tool to detect changes in cognition in geriatric patients in many countries. It is part of an array of neuropsychological batteries such as the CAMDEX-R (Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly) (Roth et al. 1986) and the CERAD battery (The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer ’s Disease) (Morris et al. 1989). The MMSE provides objective assessment of cognition through questions clustered into seven categories: 1. 2. 3. 4.
Orientation to time Orientation to place Memorization of three words (immediate memory) Attention and calculation (subtract the number 7 from 100 five consecutive times) 5. Recall of the previous three words (delayed memory) 6. Language (objects naming, repetition of a sentence, execute a verbal and a written command, write a sentence) 7. Visual construction (copy of a drawing)
The scores range from 0 to 30, and the overall score decreases as cognitive impairment increases. It takes little more than five minutes, a pencil, and a sheet of paper to administer. In developing countries, where a wide range of educational profiles can be found, schooling must be taken into account before administering the test. In addition, when testing minorities in developed countries who may not have received more than a high school education, caution must be used when interpreting MMSE performance. Therefore, the cutoff scores for dementia syndromes may need to be adjusted according to the educational level. Studies from different countries have been seeking to establish differential cutoff points for specific educational strata, for high and low educational levels, to improve accuracy in identifying possible cases of dementia (Ostrosky-Solis, Lopez-Arango, and Ardila 2000; Rosselli et al. 2000; Espino et al. 2001; de Silva and Gunatilake 2002; Brucki et al. 2003; Xu et al. 2003; Reyes-Beaman et al. 2004; Simpao et al. 2005; Crane et al. 2006; Laks et al. 2007; O’Bryant et al. 2008, Kohn et al. 2008). In addition, cross-cultural comparisons of the MMSE in different countries provide information regarding the cut-off scores in each population (Gibbons et al. 2002; Jones 2006; Dodge et al. 2009).
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
155
Clock Drawing Test Spreen and Strauss (1998) wrote a thorough review on use of the Clock Drawing Test (CDT), indicating it has been widely used as part of short mental tests in neurological investigations. Moreover, its use is often recommended as a screening test in suspected dementia cases. The first systematic use of the CDT was reported by Goodglass and Wingfield (1993), and the test was included as part of the Boston Aphasia Battery. Since then, different application and correction protocols for the CDT have been developed (Shulman 2000). Due to this fact, no specific CDT norms will be cited in this chapter. Unlike most assessment tools for dementia, which emphasize memory and attention in verbal domains, the CDT is based on visuo-spatial skills, dependent upon motor execution skills (Sunderland et al. 1989). The test assesses, in addition to visuo-spatial functions, the executive ability needed to recreate the memory of a clock face from a verbal command and translate it into a graphic image (Spreen and Strauss 1998). This constructive praxis involves not only visuo-perceptual analysis but also motor execution, attention, language comprehension and understanding of numbers (Mendez, Ala, and Underwood 1992). The CDT can be carried out in a variety of ways regarding directions and scoring, based on the concepts devised by different authors. The needed tools to administer the CDT are just a sheet of paper and a pencil. The CDT is considered a suitable tool to identify individuals with possible dementia, yet its accuracy may be reduced among individuals with limited education (Shulman 2000; Storey et al. 2002; Nishiwaki et al. 2004; Parker and Philp 2004; Fuzikawa et al. 2007; Atalaia-Silva and Lourenço 2008; Aprahamian et al. 2010). Syndrom Kurztest—Short Cognitive Performance Test The Syndrom Kurztest (SKT) (Erzigkeit 1992) is a screening tool to asses the magnitude of attentional deficits, taking into account information processing speed and memory deficits. It comprises nine subtests; six are attention subtests and three are memory subtests. The attention tests measure simple attention, processing speed, concentration, inhibitory control, and working memory. Memory is assessed in its visual aspect involving immediate and delayed recall and recognition. The required time to administer this test is estimated at 10 minutes, and scoring at three minutes. SKT overall score ranges from 0 to 27 points, and the higher the score the more severe the cognitive deficit. The total score can be subdivided into attention and memory subscores,
156
Dementia
describing the contribution of each function to the final score. Special material is required, such as a board with numbers, a stimulus booklet, and a chronometer. The test presents five parallel versions that were developed to avoid learning effect in case of retesting. Its normative values take into consideration age and intelligence level (Erzigkeit 2001). The SKT is most frequently used to detect mild cognitive impairment and mild to moderate stages of dementia (Erzigkeit 1992; Ihl et al. 1992; Overall and Schaltenbrand 1992; Kim, Nibbelink, and Overall 1993; Lehfeld and Erzigkeit 1997; Weyer et al. 1997; Flaks et al. 2006, 2009). The instrument loses its capacity for precise staging in cases of severe cognitive deficits when the understanding of instructions is markedly impaired (Erzigkeit 2001). Cross-cultural stability was found between several research centers (Lehfeld et al. 1997). However, the test is influenced by educational bias when applied in subjects with no or low educational level (Ostrosky-Solís et al. 1999; Flaks et al. 2009). Frontal Assessment Battery The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) (Dubois et al. 2000) is a more recent instrument designed to assess frontal lobe functions. It is a brief bedside cognitive and behavioral battery to screen for executive dysfunctions, more specifically, to assess functions related to the dorsolateral and medial frontal cortex (Guedj et al. 2008). The FAB consists of six subtests that explore: 1. Conceptualization (abstraction taking into account similarities between two concepts) 2. Verbal fluency (mental flexibility) 3. Motor programming 4. Sensitivity to interference (tendency to distraction based on conflicting instructions) 5. Inhibitory control 6. Prehension behavior (environmental autonomy) Scores range from 0 to 18, with higher scores indicating better test performance. It takes about 10 minutes to administer and only a chronometer is requested for test application. Some research centers indicate that the FAB is a test capable of distinguishing frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer ’s disease, as a measure of executive function (Slachevsky and Dubois 2004; Lipton et al. 2005; Castiglioni et al. 2006; Oguro et al. 2006; Kugo et al. 2007; Nakaaki et al.
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
157
2007). A recent research study regarding the performance of healthy aged individuals suggested the FAB may be influenced by schooling (Beato et al. 2007). Verbal Fluency The purpose of verbal fluency tests (VFT) is to assess the spontaneous production of words beginning with a given letter (phonemic association) or within one category (semantic association) for a limited 60-second time period. These tests assess executive and language functions, and could also evaluate semantic memory (Spreen and Strauss 1998). A study conducted in healthy elderly indicated that a good performance in verbal fluency is related to the ability to quickly organize information and formulate effective recall strategies (Bolla et al. 2006). The needed tools to administer the verbal fluency tests are just a sheet of paper, a pencil, and a chronometer. Lam et al. (2006), in a study of VFT, with semantic restriction, described the test as capable of discriminating different stages of cognitive impairment, and Libon et al. (2009) emphasized this characteristic in frontotemporal lobar degeneration patients. Investigators found that special attention must be paid when the test is administered to subjects with low educational level, because results are clearly affected by schooling (Caramelli et al. 2007). Special attention should be given to the category used. Fruit category proved to be the best VFT as it may be less biased by education, being a more appropriate test across different educational groups (Rosselli et al. 2009). Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination—Revised Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination–Revised (ACE-R) (Mioshi et al. 2006) is a recent tool designed to detect early stage dementia, which may be especially useful to distinguish AD from frontotemporal dementia. ACE-R assesses five cognitive domains, namely: 1. Orientation (time and space), attention, and concentration (subtract the number 7 from 100 five consecutive times and spell the word “world” backward) 2. Verbal memory (immediate and delayed recall of three words), episodic memory (remember a name and a address), and semantic memory (historical information) 3. Verbal fluency
158
Dementia
4. Language (comprehension of an oral command, write a sentence, repeat words and phrase, name objects, and reading) 5. Visuo-spatial skills (copy drawings) and perceptual abilities The total score ranges from 0 to 100, in that higher scores denote better performance. It takes approximately 10 to 15 minutes to administer. The needed tools to administer the ACE-R are just a sheet of paper, a pencil, and a chronometer. Research studies suggest that the ACE-R is a valid dementia screening test that is sensitive to early cognitive dysfunction (Galton et al. 2005; Mioshi et al. 2006; Larner 2007). Different cut-off points are recommended for different educational levels (García-Caballero et al. 2006; Carvalho, Barbosa, and Caramelli 2010). Other Brief Screening Tests More recently two other screening tests have been proposed to detect early cognitive impairment. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment was designed to assist health professionals to detect mild cognitive impairment (Nasreddine et al. 2005), and several studies have suggested its validity to identify cognitive deficits associated with several neurologic conditions (for a full list of references about this instrument please refer to www.mocatest.org). The Test Your Memory (TYM) instrument has been designed as a cognitive screening test that can be self administered, in the waiting room, in about five minutes. Results have suggested it was accurate in detecting 93% of Alzheimer ’s disease patients compared to 52% when the MMSE was used. This screening test may also help to identify other types of dementia and mild cognitive impairment (Brown et al. 2009). NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT OF MAIN COGNITIVE FUNCTIONS IN THE DEMENTIAS Memory and Learning Assessment Memory comprises a multitude of subsystems and it is possible to observe an uneven age-related decline among them. Therefore, memory assessment requires the examination of different subsystems such as episodic memory, working memory, and prospective memory, among others. The assessment protocol should include tasks based on visual and auditory stimuli, which require immediate and delayed recall. The assessment of the learning curve in consecutive trials and the magnitude of information
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
159
loss in delayed recalls are key aspects of the neuropsychological assessment of episodic memory when one is trying to differentiate age-related decline from early dementia. Memory assessment involves the use of tools that require encoding new information, that is, the formation of new memory traces, with the primary aim of examining the integrity of the medial-temporal region, which includes the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, directly involved in memory processes. Instruments that include multiple learning trials are of utmost importance, as they enable the assessment of the learning curve. Tests such as Word List from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) (Wechsler, “Memory Scale” 1997) and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Ivnik et al. 1990) provide five repetitions of the original word list. In the Selective Reminding Test (SRT) (Masur et al. 1990), which involves memorizing a list of 12 words, and in the Fuld Object-Memory Evaluation (FOME) (Fuld et al. 1990), which requires memorizing 10 objects placed in a bag, the examiner repeats only the nonrecalled items in each consecutive recall trial (for references and a detailed description of these instruments see Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). Cognitively unimpaired older adults should improve performance in each trial, and in the delayed recall trial, after 30 minutes, they should remember most of the information recalled in the last trial (around 80%). Dementia patients, on the other hand, do not improve performance with re-exposure to the stimulus list and exhibit significant loss of information in delayed recall trials. The Visual Reproduction and Logical Memory sub-tests, also from the WSM-III battery (Wechsler, “Memory Scale” 1997), are excellent options for assessing visual and auditory episodic memory respectively. In the former, the patient memorizes geometric figures, and in the latter he or she memorizes two stories. Both involve one immediate and one delayed recall, making it possible to examine short- and long-term memory. The Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test (RBMT) (Wilson, Cockburn, and Baddeley 1985) is also a helpful tool in memory assessment, since it encompasses ecological tasks that are seldom explored in other batteries. It informs the examiner about possible difficulties subjects might encounter in their everyday lives, such as being oriented to time and place, recalling a name associated to a person’s face, recalling a route the examiner walks in the room leaving an envelope at a certain spot, remembering to reclaim an object that was lent to someone, or asking a question at the sound of an alarm bell. In a recently published study, low performance in the RBMT screening score was found to be a significant predictor of conversion to Alzheimer ’s disease among normal controls and patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (Forlenza et al. 2010).
160
Dementia
For working memory assessment, Digit Span Backward and the LetterNumber Sequencing from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAISIII) battery (Wechsler, “Adult Intelligence” 1997) are useful in addition to a qualitative examination of the older individual’s performance in the Arithmetic subtest from the same battery. Results from memory tests are extremely important when exploring differential diagnosis in dementias. Alzheimer ’s disease is usually associated with a flat learning curve and significant loss in the delayed recall in episodic memory trials (Collie and Maruff 2000). Alzheimer ’s disease patients also frequently recall items that were not presented during learning trials. Dementias related to cerebrovascular diseases can often emerge with no mnestic dysfunction as executive dysfunction may be its most prominent marker (Gainotti et al. 2008). Memory preservation can also be observed in dementias in which the first affected areas are not the temporal-medial but the frontal areas, as can be observed in frontotemporal lobar degeneration associated with semantic dementia or in frontotemporal dementia (Rabinovici and Miller 2010). Attention Assessment Paying attention requires detecting changes in the outside world and simultaneously inhibiting interference from other competing stimuli (Posner and Raichle 1994). Attention is a multidimensional skill, and its components are intrinsically connected with other skills such as memory and executive functions; therefore, it is difficult to assess attention separately. The following tests are simple to apply and can be used to assess different aspects of attention processes: Digit Span Forward and Digit Symbol from the WAIS-III battery (Wechsler, “Adult Intelligence” 1997); Trail A (Ashendorf et al. 2008); Stroop in its several formats; and letter, number, or symbol cancellation tests (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). The Mental Control subtest of the WMS-III battery can also be used (Wechsler, “Memory Scale” 1997). Attentional deficits are common among aged patients with cerebrovascular diseases. Research studies suggest that a considerable number of subcortical white matter lesions are linked to poorer performance in visual and auditory attention tests and in executive function tests (Van Dijk et al. 2004), as well as to psychomotor slowing (Gainotti et al. 2008). Dementia with Lewy bodies, in turn, is characterized by significant cognitive fluctuation, especially in attention tests (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). Attention tasks such as inhibitory and mental control (ability to focus) in the face of competing and concomitant stimuli, which are directly related
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
161
to executive functions regarding storage and simultaneous processing of information, are described below. Executive Function Assessment The term executive function denominates a set of skills that are needed for complex behaviors. The executive system is a hypothetical cognitive principle involving planning tasks, organizational tasks, mental flexibility, abstract thinking, avoidance of inappropriate actions, and inhibition of irrelevant information processing. The executive system is also supposed to be in charge of adjusting one’s behavior to solve day-to-day situations such as initiative, management of choices, consequence assessment, decisionmaking, action implementation and control, and course correction and adjustment when needed. The examination of executive functions aims at assessing prefrontal cortex integrity. Several neuropsychological tests may be used to assess different aspects of cognition associated with executive functions. Some of the most frequently used are: (1) verbal fluency during a limited 60-second time frame, restricted semantically, for instance, by a category such as animal and fruit naming (Caramelli et al. 2007; Radanovic et al. 2009), or restricted phonemically, by using words starting with F, A, or S (Controlled Oral Word Association, COWA) (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004) to assess information processing speed; (2) Clock Drawing Test, which assesses visuo-spatial functions as well as planning and self-regulation during execution, as previously described; (3) Trail B, as it demands coordination of two competing information systems (Ashendorf et al. 2008); and (4) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004), possibly the most traditional executive function test, when the patient is required to combine 48 cards with one of four model cards that vary according to criteria such as color, shape and number of symbols to assess mental flexibility and abstraction capacity (Modified Card Sorting Test, MCST; Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). In order to assess complex decision making skills, the Iowa Gambling Task may be used as it has already been adjusted and validated for use with older populations (Bechara et al. 1994). The task entails choosing cards from four decks. As the individual verifies gains and losses associated with each card deck, she or he infers which decks are more advantageous and which are not. The subject’s ability to evaluate choices to maximize gains, minimizing losses, is thus surveyed. In recent years, an executive control assessment interview, EXIT-25 (Executive Interview, Royall, Mahurin, and Gray 1992) was developed,
162
Dementia
grouping traditional tests associated with frontal functions. The EXIT-25 is an important battery because performance in it is strongly correlated with activities of daily living (Royall et al. 2007; Pereira et al. 2008). In addition, decline in this scale significantly predicts dependence and need for care (Mann et al. 1992). The Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004) is also worth mentioning. It simulates daily challenges involving executive functions, such as removing a cork from inside a bottle using a few available tools, working out a route within a zoo, temporal judgment, and managing time for task completion. The advantage of this battery is its resemblance with challenges encountered in the subjects’ everyday lives. Language Assessment Language functions tend to be preserved in healthy aged individuals. Vocabulary remains stable and can even expand with aging, slightly declining after the age of 70. Nevertheless, some language changes have been recorded in healthy elderly individuals. Difficulty in finding words or the “tip of the tongue” experience can be noted more often. From a qualitative standpoint, healthy older adults tend to: (1) use a greater number of words to describe something that could be expressed with only one; (2) describe the function of the object rather than name it; (3) less accurately identify objects due to sensory deficits; (4) make associative semantic errors, when something related to the object is mentioned instead of the object itself (Woodruff-Pak 1997). These changes could suggest difficulty or slowing in semantic access. Complex sentence comprehension and formulation, as well as speech organization and accuracy, could also moderately decline in healthy aged individuals. The Vocabulary subtest from the WAIS-III battery (Wechsler, “Adult Intelligence” 1997) is among the tasks that are frequently used for language assessment among older adults, as it enables the examiner to verify whether the patients’ word knowledge is compatible with his or her level of schooling. Qualitatively, the examiner may also notice if the definitions provided are accurate, and if access to these definitions is fast and easy. Another frequently used test is the Boston Naming Test (Steinberg et al. 2005), when the patient is asked to name 60 pictures, increasingly difficult, and the examiner may need to judge whether semantic and/or phonological clues are helpful. Vocabulary and naming trials are usually complemented by verbal fluency tasks, also used to measure executive function. An additional common practice is to ask the subject to complete
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
163
and interpret proverbs. This task not only examines language comprehension but also assesses semantic memory and abstract thinking. In case of suspected aphasia, the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) (Goodglass, Kaplan, and Barresi 2000) is commonly used. The traditional “cookie theft picture” of this battery can be used separately to examine the quality of oral and narrative speech. Language assessment may be relevant in differential diagnosis of dementias. In the course of healthy aging, language abilities are expected to be preserved, with just mild changes typical of healthy aging (Woodruff-Pak 1997). Significant decline in this function may suggest that lesions in language-related regions (Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas) might be present, or that neurodegenerative disorders that manifest with losses in this function, such as semantic dementia or nonfluent progressive aphasia, might be present (Rabinovici and Miller 2010). Visuo-Spatial Ability Assessment Visuo-spatial abilities are needed to perform tasks such as copying figures, assembling objects, interpreting maps, dimensioning spatial relationships among objects. They also involve spatial orientation to perform complex actions, along with several other abilities (Woodruff-Pak 1997). Among the most widely used trials to assess visuo-spatial ability in neuropsychological practice are the following: the above-mentioned Boston Naming Test, which in addition to naming capability also examines visual perception; the Hooper Visual Organization Test; the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure copying; the Necker Cube copying; and the Clock Drawing Test (for references and detailed description of these tools see Lezak, Howieson, and Loring 2004). The latter three instruments involve visuoconstructive abilities, as they require copying or drawing of complex figures. There are motor demands in addition to visual integration and organization. In addition, the Block Design subtest from the WAIS-III battery (Wechsler, “Adult Intelligence” 1997) is also regularly used to assess visual-perceptual and visuo-constructive abilities. In this subtest, the patient is asked to use blocks to form a three-dimensional representation of a bidimensional picture in a card. Visuo-spatial ability loss can be observed in patients with dementias with different etiologies, usually in advanced stages. Patients with focal lesions in the parietal and occipital lobes tend to show significant impairment in visuo-spatial tasks. It must be pointed out that patients suffering from dementia with Lewy bodies display considerable visuo-spatial deficits even at early stages of the disease (Lezak, Howieson, and Loring
164
Dementia
2004). Nevertheless, when assessing visuo-spatial abilities, the educational and occupational background of the older individual must be taken into account. In cases when the individual reports to have always performed poorly in visuo-constructive tasks, a below-average result should not be given much weight. On the other hand, for patients who used to sew, who used to develop carpentry projects, or with a history of complex craftsmanship, poor results in these tasks should be highlighted. Neuropsychological tests are usually strongly influenced by educational experience. Therefore, in developing countries, where older adults’ education profile tends to be diverse, and when assessing minority seniors in developed nations, the assessment protocol should be planned carefully. In a recent study (Yassuda et al. 2009) involving older adults with heterogeneous educational backgrounds, the RBMT, the FOME, and verbal fluency with animal category were not significantly influenced by education and therefore they should be used in such cases. Functional Assessment in the Context of the Dementias One vital aspect in the neuropsychological assessment of older individuals concerns gathering proxy measures of performance in daily living tasks. In the context of aging, assessment of functionality is of utmost importance because it draws the line between normal and pathological cognitive aging. The concept of functional status receives a variety of definitions and it is assessed in many different ways by health professionals. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), it is a key factor for the definition of physical and mental health. Due to the wide array of concepts and terminologies used, the WHO published in 2001 the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health, also known as ICF. According to this classification, functional status is considered a broad concept that encompasses a number of body functions and structures, as well as activity and participation in a socio-environmental context (Buñuales, Diego, and Moreno 2002; WHO 2001). Functional capacity concerns the ability to maintain the physical and mental skills needed for independent living, valuing autonomy and selfdetermination (Gordilho et al. 2000). Functional capacity is an index of how a given activity is carried out in everyday life, what people do in their environment, thus including involvement in real-life situations. It is a key aspect in the concept of independence, which implies the ability to function effectively without the help of others in activities of daily living. Functional disability can be denoted by the presence of difficulties
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
165
in performing certain activities of daily living, or even by the complete inability to perform them unaided (Rosa et al. 2003). Autonomy is related to the practice of self-governing and includes the following elements: individual freedom, privacy, free will, and harmony regarding one’s own feelings and needs. The greater the independence, the greater the likelihood of having autonomy, though under partial dependency conditions the individual can keep his or her autonomy, depending on the social arrangements he or she manages to make. Functional decline is the result of a complex interaction among several elements, and cognitive performance is one of the most important of them. The tools and batteries used to assess impairments in dementias focus primarily on cognitive performance. Usually there is less interest in apprehending how cognitive changes interfere with the person’s functioning. However, functional assessment provides information about daily performance, which is vital for health professionals who constantly have to counsel family member regarding patients’ ability to live independently. Functional assessment is also relevant as functional impairment distinguishes mild cognitive impairment (MCI) from dementia syndromes (Petersen et al. 2001). Current studies have investigated whether MCI patients present functional deficits and which kind of functional loss is typical of older adults with MCI (Farias et al. 2006). Some researchers claim that a modest decline in functioning should be part of the set of criteria for MCI diagnosis (Perneczky et al. 2006). It is well established in the literature that functional decline in the dementias follows a gradient in which basic activities of daily living (BADL) are affected after more complex activities have already deteriorated. Among MCI patients, the focus of functional assessment is on complex instrumental activities of daily living (IADL). Following this line of research, international papers point out that decline in four IADLs as strong predictors of dementia: ability to use the telephone, use of means of transportation, management of one’s own medications, and ability to handle finances (Barberger-Gateau et al. 1999), the latter usually being the first to be impaired (Griffith et al. 2007). In a Brazilian sample of older adults with heterogeneous cognitive profiles assessed with a direct measure of functional status (Direct Assessment of Functional Status, DAFS-BR), MCI patients had significantly lower scores in two domains, dealing with finances and shopping skills, compared to normal controls (Pereira et al. 2010). Despite the clinical relevance of studying the relationship between cognitive and functional performance, few research studies have actually examined this relationship (Royall et al. 2007). It is not yet clear, for
166
Dementia
instance, to what extent functional performance fluctuation can be directly attributed to cognition, or whether both are dependent upon noncognitive variables, such as socioeconomic, cultural, or personal aspects (Mor et al. 1989). Rosa et al. (2003) found that the attributes that have the strongest association with functional decline in a sample of Brazilian older adults were illiteracy, retirement, being a pensioner, being a housewife, being over 65, having a multigenerational family structure, being hospitalized in the last six months, not having the habit of visiting friends or relatives, having visual disabilities and a history of stroke, and having a pessimistic view with regards to one’s own health when compared to peers. Executive functioning has been highlighted as the key factor regarding functional performance among older individuals with cognitive decline. It must be pointed out that the degree of the correlation between executive functions and functionality is greater than that observed between executive functions and memory. Executive dysfunction directly affects IADLs, whereas working memory may be preserved in early dementia, starting to be compromised only in more advanced stages (Royall et al. 2005; Pereira et al. 2008). In the context of dementias, functional assessment may involve direct observation (performance trials) and the use of scales or questionnaires filled in by informants or the patient. Up to the present moment, no consensus has been reached regarding the best method to assess an older individual’s functional performance (Royall et al. 2007). In clinical practice, functioning is assessed by reports of a family member or caregiver concerning the difficulties the patient has encountered in carrying out daily living activities. Even though this approach is often adopted, abundant empirical evidence suggests that information provided by third parties is possibly biased, for example, by mood, caregiver personality and burden, resulting in over- or underestimation of functional deficits (Onor et al. 2006; Tierney et al. 1996; Glass 1998; Loewenstein et al. 2001). Research suggests that objective functional assessments (based on observed performance) are more accurate in identifying functional limitations, in addition to making it possible to devise compensatory strategies (Mangone et al. 1993; Farias et al. 2006; Onor et al. 2006; Pereira et al. 2010). The most frequently used tools to assess functional status are the Barthel Index (Mahoney and Barthel 1965), Activities of Daily Living Scale outlined by Katz et al. (1963), IADL Scales by Lawton and Brody (1969) and by Pfeffer et al. (1982), Brazilian OARS Multidimensional Functional Assessment Questionnaire (BOMFAQ) (Blay, Ramos, and Mary 1988), and Functional Independency Measurement (MIF) (Riberto et al. 2001).
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
167
Among other scales, objective functional assessment of aged individuals can be carried out with the use of the Direct Assessment of Functional Status–Revised (DAFS-R) (Loewenstein and Bates 2006). This assessment relies on direct observation of the individual while he or she undertakes activities that replicate IADLs—for instance, orientation to time, communication, ability to handle finances, aptitude to take care of shopping needs, and BADLs, for instance, dressing ability, personal hygiene, and nutrition. The DAFS-R is used in seven countries around the world (Loewenstein, Amigo, and Duara 1989). A recent paper (Pereira et al. 2008) found a strong correlation between executive control (EXIT-25, Royall et al. 1992) and performance in the DAFS-BR (the Brazilian version of DAFS-R). And as stated earlier, this instrument may help identify MCI cases (Pereira et al. 2010). In conclusion, brief cognitive screening, neuropsychological assessment, and assessment of functional status may significantly contribute to the early diagnosis of cognitive impairment. Early diagnosis may facilitate the implementation of pharmacological and psychosocial interventions that might contribute to the stabilization of cognitive losses. Cognitive testing may also assist in evaluating the effectiveness of treatment protocols.
REFERENCES Aprahamian, I., J. E. Martinelli, A. L. Neri, and M. S. Yassuda. 2010. The accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test compared to that of standard screening tests for Alzheimer ’s disease: Results from a study of Brazilian elderly with heterogeneous educational backgrounds International Psychogeriatrics 22 (1): 64–71. Ashendorf, L., A. L. Jefferson, M. K. O’Connor, C. Chaisson, R. Green, and R. Stern. 2008. Trail Making Test errors in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 23 (2): 129–137. Atalaia-Silva, K. C., and R. A. Lourenço. 2008. Tradução, adaptação e validação de construto do Teste do Relógio aplicado entre idosos no Brasil [Adaptation and validation of the Clock Drawing Test among Brazilian elderly]. Revista Saúde Pública 42 (5): 930–937. Barberger-Gateau, P., C. Fabrigoule, I. Rouch, et al. 1999. Neuropsychological correlates of self-reported performance in instrumental activities of daily living and prediction of dementia. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 54: 293–303. Beato, R. G., R. Nitrini, A. P. Formigoni, and P. Caramelli. 2007. Brazilian version of the frontal assessment battery (FAB): Preliminary data on administration to healthy elderly. Dementia and Neuropsychologia 1: 59–65.
168
Dementia
Bechara, A., A. R. Damasio, H. Damasio, and S. W. Anderson. 1994. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50 (1–3): 7–15. Bischkopf, J., A. Busse, and M. C. Angermeyer. 2002. Mild Cognitive Impairment—A review of prevalence, incidence and outcome according to current approaches. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 106: 403–414. Blay, S. L., L. R. Ramos, and J. J. Mary. 1988. Validity of a Brazilian version of the Older Americans Resources and Services (OARS) mental-health screening questionnaire. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 36: 687–692. Bolla, K. I., K. N. Lindgren, C. Bonaccorsy, and M. L. Bleecker. 2006. Predictors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. Journal of Clinical Psychology 46 (5): 623–628. Bottino, C. M. C., D. Azevedo Jr., M. F. Tatsch, S. R. Hototian, M. A. Moscoso, J. C. Folquitto, A. Z. Scalco, M. C. Bazzarella, M. A. Lopes, and J. Litvoc. 2008. Estimate of dementia prevalence in a community sample from São Paulo, Brazil. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 26: 291–299. Brown, J., K. Dawson, L. A. Brown, and P. Clatworthy. 2009. Self-administered cognitive screening test (TYM) for detection of Alzheimer ’s disease: Cross sectional study. British Medical Journal 338: b2030. Brucki, S. M. D., R. Nitrini, P. Caramelli, P. H. F. Bertolucci, and I. H. Okamoto. 2003. Sugestões para o uso do Mini-Exame do Estado Mental no Brasil [Suggestions for the Mini-Mental State Examination in Brazil]. Arquivos de Neuropsiquiatria 61: 777–781. Buñuales, M. T. J., P. G. Diego, and J. M. M. Moreno. 2002. La Classificación Internacional del Funcionamento de la Discapacidad y de la Salud (CIF). Revista Española de Salud Pública 6: 271–279. Caramelli, P., M. T. Carthery-Goulart, C. S. Porto, H. Charchat-Fichman, and R. Nitrini. 2007. Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer ’s disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 21 (1): 65–67. Carvalho, V. A., M. T. Barbosa, and P. Caramelli. 2010. Brazilian version of the Addenbrooke Cognitive Examination-revised in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 23: 8–13. Castiglioni, S., O. Pelati, M. Zuffi, F. Somalvico, L. Marino, T. Tentorio, and M. Franceschi. 2006. The frontal assessment battery does not differentiate frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer ’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 22 (2): 125–131. Collie, A., and P. Maruff. 2000. The neuropsychology of preclinical Alzheimer ’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24: 365–374. Crane, P. K., L. E. Gibbons, L. Jolley, G. van Belle, R. Selleri, E. Dalmonte, and D. De Ronchi. 2006. Differential item functioning related to education and age in the Italian version of the Mini-Mental State Examination. International Psychogeriatrics 18: 505–515.
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
169
Cummings, J. L., H. V. Vinters, G. M. Cole, and Z. S. Khachaturian. 1998. Alzheimer ’s disease: Etiologies, pathophysiology, cognitive reserve and treatment opportunities. Neurology 51 (Suppl 1): 2–7. De Ronchi, D., L. Fratiglioni, P. Rucci, A. Paternicò, S. Graziani, and E. Dalmonte. 1998. The effect of education on dementia occurrence in an Italian population with mild to high socioeconomic status. Neurology 50: 1231–1238. de Silva, H. A., and S. B. Gunatilake. 2002. Mini-Mental State Examination in Sinhalese: A sensitive test to screen for dementia in Sri Lanka. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 134–139. Dodge, H., K. Meguro, H. Ishii, S. Yamaguchi, J. Saxton, and M. Ganguli. 2009. Cross-cultural comparisons of the Mini-Mental State Examination between Japanese and U.S. cohorts. InternationalPsychogeriatrics 21 (1): 113–122. Dubois, B., H. Feldman, C. Jacova, S. DeKosky, P. Barberger-Gateau, J. Cummings, A. Delacourte, D. Galasko, S. Gauthier, and G. Jicha. 2007. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: Revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. The Lancet Neurology 6 (8): 734–746. Dubois, B., A. Slachevsky, I. Litvan, and B. Pillon. 2000. The FAB: A Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 55: 1621–1626. Erzigkeit, H. 1992. SKT: A short cognitive performance test for assessing memory and attention. SKT Manual. Erlangen, Germany: Beltz Test. Erzigkeit, H. 2001. SKT: A short cognitive performance test for assessing deficits of memory and attention. User ’s manual. 23rd ed. Erlangen, Germany: Geromed GmbH. Espino, D. V., M. J. Lichtenstein, R. F. Palmer, and H. P. Hazuda. 2001. Ethnic differences in Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores: Where you live makes a difference. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 49 (5): 538–548. Farias, S. T., D. Mungas, B. R. Reed, D. Harvey, D. Cahn-Weiner, and C. Decarli. 2006. MCI is associated with deficits in everyday functioning. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 20 (4): 217–223. Ferri, C., M. Prince, C. Brayne, H. Brodaty, L. Fratiglioni, M. Ganguli, K. Hall, K. Hasegawa, H. Hendrie, and Y. Huang. 2005. Global prevalence of dementia: A Delphi consensus study. The Lancet 366 (9503): 2112–2117. Flaks, M. K., M. S. Yassuda, A. C. B. Regina, C. G. Cid, C. H. P. Camargo, W. F. Gattaz, and O. V. Forlenza. 2006. The Short Cognitive Performance Test (SKT): A preliminary study of its psychometric properties in Brazil. International Psychogeriatrics 18 (1): 121–133. Flaks, M. K., O. V. Forlenza, F. S. Pereira, L. F. Viola, and M. S. Yassuda. 2009. Short cognitive performance test: Diagnostic accuracy and education bias in older Brazilian adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 24 (3): 301–306. Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh. 1975. Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients of the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12: 189–198. Forlenza, O. V., B. S. Diniz, L. L. Talib, M. Radanovic, M. S. Yassuda, E. B. Ojopi, and W. F. Gattaz. 2010. Clinical and biological predictors of Alzheimer ’s
170
Dementia
disease in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, April 16 (Epub ahead of print). Fuld, P., D. M. Masur, A. Blau, C. Howard, and M. Aronson. 1990. Object memory evaluation for prospective detection of dementia in normal functioning elderly: Predictive and normative data. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 12 (4): 520–528. Fuzikawa, C., M. F. Lima-Costa, E. Uchôa, and K. Shulman. 2007. Correlation and agreement between the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Clock Drawing Test in older adults with low levels of schooling: The Bambuí Health Aging Study (BHAS). International Psychogeriatrics 19 (4): 657–667. Gainotti, G., M. Ferraciolli, M. G. Vita, and C. Marra. 2008. Patterns of neuropsychological impairment in MCI patients with small subcortical infarcts or hippocampal atrophy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 14 (4): 611–619. Galton, C. J., S. Erzinçlioglu, B. J. Sahakian, N. Antoun, and J. R. Hodges. 2005. A comparison of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE), conventional neuropsychological assessment, and simple MRI-based medial temporal lobe evaluation in the early diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease. Cognitive Behavioral Neurology 18 (3): 144–150. Garcia-Caballero, A., I. Garcia-Lado, J. Gonzalez-Hermida, et al. 2006. Validation of the Spanish version of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination in a rural community in Spain. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21 (3): 239–245. Gibbons, L. E., G. van Belle, M. Yang, C. Gill, C. Brayne, F. A. Huppert, E. Paykel, and E. Larson. 2002. Cross-cultural comparison of the Mini-Mental State Examination in United Kingdom and United States participants with Alzheimer ’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 723–728. Glass, T. A. 1998. Conjugating the tenses of function: Discordance among hypothetical, experimental, and enacted function in older adults. Gerontologist 38: 101–112. Goodglass, H., E. Kaplan, and B. Barresi. 2000. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE-3). 3rd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott. Goodglass, H., and A. Wingfield. 1993. Selective preservation of a lexical category in aphasia: Dissociations in comprehension of body parts and geographical place names following focal brain lesion. Memory 1 (4): 313–328. Gordilho, A., A. Sergio, J. Silvestre, L. R. Ramos, M. P. A. Freire, N. Espindola, et al. 2000. Desafios a serem enfrentados no terceiro milênio pelo setor saúde na atençaˉo integral ao idoso. Rio de Janeiro: UnATI/UERI. Griffith, H. R., K. Belue, A. Sicola, S. Krzywanski, et al. 2007. Impaired financial abilities in mild cognitive impairment: A direct assessment approach. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias 22 (3): 21–217. Guedj, E., G. Allali, C. Goetz, I. Le Ber, M. Volteau, L. Lacomblez, P. Vera, et al. 2008. Frontal Assessment Battery is a marker of dorsolateral and medial
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
171
frontal functions: A SPECT study in frontotemporal dementia. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 273 (1–2): 84–87. Hampel, H., K. Bürger, S. Teipel, A. Bokde, H. Zetterberg, and K. Blennow. 2008. Core candidate neurochemical and imaging biomarkers of Alzheimer ’s disease. Alzheimer and Dementia 4 (1): 38–48. Herrera, E., P. Caramelli, A. S. Silveira, and R. Nitrini. 2002. Epidemiologic survey of dementia in a community-dwelling Brazilian population. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 16: 103–108. Ho, L., H. Fiveacoat, J. Wang, and G. M. Pasinetti. 2010. Alzheimer ’s disease biomarker discovery in symptomatic and asymptomatic patients: Experimental approaches and future clinical applications. Experimental Gerontology 45 (1): 15–22. Ihl, R., L. Frölich, T. Dierks, E. M. Martin, and K. Maurer. 1992. Differential validity of psychometric tests in dementia of Alzheimer type. Psychiatric Research 44: 93–106. Ivnik, R. J., J. F. Malec, E. G. Tangalos, R. C. Petersen, E. Kokmen, and L. T. Kurland. 1990. The auditory-verbal learning test (RAVLT): Norms for ages 55 years and older. Psychological Assessment 2: 304–312. Jones, R. N. 2006. Identification of measurement differences between English and Spanish language versions of the Mini-Mental State Examination: detecting differential item functioning using MIMIC modeling. Medical Care 44: S124–133. Katz, S., A. B. Ford, R. W. Moskowitz, N. B. A. Jackson, and M. W. Jaffe. 1963. Studies of illness in the aged. Journal of the American Medical Society 185 (12): 914–921. Katzman, R. 1993. Education and prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 43: 13–20. Kim, Y. G., D. W. Nibbelink, and J. E. Overall. 1993. Factor structure and scoring of the SKT Test Battery. Journal of Clinical Psychology 49 (1): 61–71. Kohn, R., B. Vicente, P. Rioseco, S. Saldivia, and S. Torres. 2008. The Mini-Mental State Examination: Age and education distribution for a Latin American population. Aging and Mental Health 12: 66–71. Kugo, A., S. Terada, T. Ata, Y. Ido, Y. Kado, T. Ishihara, M. Hikiji, Y. Fujisawa, K. Sasaki, and S. Kuroda. 2007. Japanese version of the Frontal Assessment Battery for dementia. Psychiatry Research 153 (1): 69–75. Laks, J., E. M. R. Batista, A. L. B. Contino, and E. Engelhardt. 2007. Mini-Mental State Examination norms in a community-dwelling sample of elderly with low schooling in Brazil. Cadernos de Saúde Pública (FIOCRUZ) 23: 315–319. Lam, L. C., P. Ho, V. W. Lui, and C. W. Tam. 2006. Reduced semantic fluency as an additional screening tool for subjects with questionable dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 22 (2): 159–164. Larner, A. J. 2007. Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-Revised (ACE-R) in day-to-day clinical practice. Age and Ageing 36 (6): 685–686.
172
Dementia
Lawton, M. P., and E. M. Brody. 1969. Assessment of older people: Self maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 9 (3): 179–186. Lehfeld, H., and H. Erzigkeit. 1997. The SKT: A short cognitive performance test for assessing deficits of memory and attention. International Psychogeriatrics 9 (Suppl 1): 115–121. Lehfeld, H., G. Rudinger, C. Rietz, C. Heinrich, V. Wied, L. Fornazzari, J. Pittas, I. Hindmarch, and H. Erzigkeit. 1997. Evidence of the cross-cultural stability of the factor structure of the SKT Short Test for assessing deficits of memory and attention. International Psychogeriatrics 9 (2): 139–153. Lezak, M. D., D. B. Howieson, and D. W. Loring. 2004. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. Libon, D. J., C. McMillan, D. Gunawardena,, C. Powers, L. Massimo, A. Khan, B. Morgan, et al. 2009. Neurocognitive contributions to verbal fluency deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 73 (7): 535–542. Lipton, A. M., K. A. Ohman, K. B. Womack, L. S. Hynan, E. T. Ninman, and L. H. Lacritz. 2005. Subscores of the FAB differentiate frontotemporal lobar degeneration from AD. Neurology 65: 726–731. Loewenstein, D. A., E. Amigo, and R. Duara. 1989. A new scale for the assessment of functional status in Alzheimer ’s disease and related disorders. Journal of Gerontology 4: 114–121. Loewenstein, D. A., S. Argüelles, M. Bravo, R. Q. Freeman, T. Argüelles, A. Acevedo, and C. Eisdorfer. 2001. Caregivers’ judgments of the functional abilities of the Alzheimer ’s disease patient: A comparison of proxy reports and objective measures. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 56: 78–84. Loewenstein, D. A., and C. B. Bates. 2006. The Direct Assessment of Functional Status Revised (DAFS-R). Manual for Administration and Scoring. Miami: Neuropsychological Laboratories and the Wien Center for Alzheimer ’s Disease and Memory Disorders, Mount Sinai Medical Center. Mahoney, F. I., and D. W. Barthel. 1965. Functional evaluation: The Barthel Index. Maryland State Medical Journal 14: 61–65. Mangone, C. A., R. M. Sanguinetti, P. D. Baumann, R. C. Gonzalez, S. Pereyra, F. G. Bozzola, P. B. Gorelick, and R. E. Sica. 1993. Influence of feelings of burden on the caregiver ’s perception of the patient’s functional status. Dementia 4 (5): 287–293. Mann, U. M., E. Mohr, M. Gearing, and T. N. Chase. 1992. Heterogeneity in Alzheimer ’s disease: Progression rate segregated by distinct neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic profiles. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55: 956–959. Masur, D. M., P. A. Fuld, A. D. Blau, H. Crystal, and M. K. Aronson. 1990. Predicting development of dementia in the elderly with the Selective Reminding Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 12 (4): 529–538. Mendez, M. F., T. Ala, and K. L. Underwood. 1992. Development of scoring criteria for the Clock Drawing Task in Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 40: 1095–1099.
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
173
Mioshi, E., K. Dawson, J. Mitchell, R. Arnold, and J. R. Hodges. 2006. The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21 (11): 1078–1085. Mor, V., J. Murphy, S. Masterson-Allen, C. Willey, A. Razmpour, M. E. Jacksin, et al. 1989. Risk of functional decline among well elders. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 42: 895–904. Morris, J. C., A. Heyman, R. C. Mohs, et al. 1989. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer ’s Disease (CERAD). Part I: Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 39: 1159–1165. Nakaaki, S., Y. Murata, J. Sato, Y. Shinagawa, T. Matsui, H. Tatsumi, and T. A. Furukawa. 2007. Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Frontal Assessment Battery in patients with the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 61 (1): 78–83. Nasreddine, Z. S., N. A. Phillips, V. Bédirian, S. Charbonneau, V. Whitehead, I. Collin, J. L. Cummings, and H. Chertkow. 2005. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 695–699. Nishiwaki, Y., E. Breeze, L. Smeeth, C. J. Bulpitt, R. Peters, and A. E. Fletcher. 2004. Validity of the Clock-Drawing Test as a screening tool for cognitive impairment in the elderly. American Journal of Epidemiology 160 (8): 797–807. O’Bryant, S. E., J. D. Humphreys, G. E. Smith, R. J. Ivnik, N. R. Graff-Radford, R. C. Petersen, and J. A. Lucas. 2008. Detecting dementia with the MiniMental State Examination (MMSE) in highly educated individuals. Archives of Neurology 65 (7): 963–967. Oguro, H., S. Yamaguchi, S. Abe, Y. Ishida, H. Bokura, and S. Kobayashi. 2006. Differentiating Alzheimer ’s disease from subcortical vascular dementia with the FAB test. Journal of Neurology 253 (11): 1490–1494. Onor, M. L., M. Trevisiol, C. Negro, and E. Aguglia. 2006. Different perception of cognitive impairment, behavioral disturbances, and functional disabilities between persons with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer ’s disease and their caregivers. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias 21 (5): 333–338. Ostrosky-Solís, F., G. Dávila, X. Ortiz, et al. 1999. Determination of normative criteria and validation of the SKT for use in a Spanish-speaking populations. International Psychogeriatrics 11: 171–180. Ostrosky-Solis, F., G. Lopez-Arango, and A. Ardila. 2000. Sensitivity and specificity of the Mini-mental State Examination in a Spanish-speaking population. Applied Neuropsychology 7: 25–31. Overall, J. E., and R. Schaltenbrand. 1992. The SKT neuropsychological test battery. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 5: 220–227. Parker, C., and I. Philp. 2004. Screening for cognitive impairment among older people in black and minority ethnic groups. Age and Ageing 33 (5): 447–452. Pereira, F. S., M. S. Yassuda, A. M. Oliveira, and O. V. Forlenza. 2008. Executive dysfunction correlates with impaired functional status in older adults with
174
Dementia
varying degrees of cognitive impairment. International Psychogeriatrics 20 (6): 1104–1115. Pereira, F. S., M. S. Yassuda, A. M. Oliveira, B. S. Diniz, M. Radanovic, L. L. Talib, W. F. Gattaz, and O. V. Forlenza. 2010. Profiles of functional deficits in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: benefits from objective measurement. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 16 (2): 297–305. Perneczky, R., C. Pohl, C. Sorg, et al. 2006. Impairment of activities of daily living requiring memory or complex reasoning as part of de MCI syndrome. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 35: 240–245. Petersen, R. C., R. Doody, A. Kurz, R. C. Mohs, J. C. Morris, and P. V. Rabins. 2001. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology 58 (12): 1985–1992. Pfeffer, R. I., T. T. Kurosaki, C. H. Harrah, J. M. Chance, and S. Filos. 1982. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. Journal of Gerontology 37 (3): 323–329. Portet, F., P. J. Ousset, P. J. Visser, G. B. Frisoni, F. Nobili, Ph. Scheltens, B. Vellas, and J. Touchon. 2006. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: A critical review of the concept and new diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer ’s Disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 77: 714–718. Posner, M. I., and M. Raichle. 2004. Images of Mind. New York: Scientific American Library. Rabinovici, G. D., and B. L. Miller. 2010. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: Epidemiology, pathophysiology, diagnosis and management. CNS Drugs 24 (5): 375–398. Radanovic, M., B. S. Diniz, R. M. Mirandez, T. M. Novaretti, M. K. Flaks, M. S. Yassuda, and O. V. Forlenza. 2009. Verbal fluency in the detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease among Brazilian Portuguese speakers: The influence of education. International Psychogeriatrics 21 (6): 1081–1087. Reyes-Beaman, S., et al. 2004. Validation of a modified version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in Spanish. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 11: 1–11. Riberto, M., M. H. Myazaki, D. J. Filho, H. Sakamoto, and L. R. Battistella. 2001. Reprotutibilidade da versão brasileira da Medida de Independência Funcional [Reproductibility of the Brazilian version of the Functional Independence Measure FIM]. Acta Fisiatrica 8: 41–52. Rosa, T. E. C., M. H. Benício, M. O. Latorreb, and L. R. Ramos. 2003. Fatores determinantes da capacidade funcional entre idosos [Determinant factors of functional capacity among the elderly]. Revista de Saúde Pública 37 (1): 40–48. Rosselli, D., A. Ardila, G. Pradilla, L. Morillo, L. Bautista, O. Rey, and M. Camacho. 2000. The Mini-Mental State Examination as a selected diagnostic test
Cognitive Screening and Neuropsychological
175
for dementia: A Colombian population study. Revista Neurologia 30 (5): 428–432. Rosselli, M., R. Tappen, C. Williams, J. Salvatierra, and Y. Zoller. 2009. Level of education and category fluency task among Spanish speaking elders: Number of words, clustering, and switching strategies. Neuropsychology Development and Cognition Section B: Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition 16 (6): 721–744. Roth, M., E. Tym, C. Q. Mountjoy, F. A. Huppert, H. Hendrie, S. Verma, and R. Goddard. 1986. CAMDEX: A standard instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry 149: 698–709. Royall, D. R., E. C. Lauterbach, D. Kaufer, P. Malloy, K. L. Coburn, and K. J. Black. 2007. The cognitive correlates of functional status: A review from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 19 (3): 249–265. Royall, D. R., R. K. Mahurin, and K. F. Gray. 1992. Beside assessment of executive impairment: The Executive Interview (EXIT). Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 40: 1221–1226. Royall, D. R., R. Palmer, L. K. Chiodo, and M. J. Polk. 2005. Executive control mediates memory’s association with change in instrumental activities of daily living: The Freedom House Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 1–11. Shaw, L. M., M. Korecka, C. M. Clark, V. M. Y. Lee, and J. Q. Trojanowski. 2007. Biomarkers of neurodegeneration for diagnosis and monitoring therapeutics. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 6: 295–303. Simpao, M. P., D. V. Espino, R. F. Palmer, M. J. Lichtenstein, and H. P. Hazuda. 2005. Association between acculturation and structural assimilation and Mini-Mental State Examination–Assessed cognitive impairment in older Mexican Americans: Findings from the San Antonio Longitudinal Study of Aging. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 1234–1239. Shulman K. 2000. I. Clock-drawing: Is it the ideal cognitive screening test? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 15: 548–561. Shulman, K., and A. Feinstein. 2003. Quick Cognitive Screening for Clinicians. London: Martin Dunitz. Slachevsky, A., and B. Dubois. 2004. Frontal Assessment Battery and differential diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology 61 (7): 1104–1107. Spreen, O., and E. Strauss. 1998. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press. Steinberg, B. A., L. A. Bieliauskas, G. E. Smith, C. Langellotti, and R. J. Ivnik. 2005. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: Age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Boston Naming Test, the MAE Token Test, and the Judgment of Line Orientation Test. Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (3–4): 280–328.
176
Dementia
Stern, Y., B. Gurland, T. K. Tatemichi, M. X. Tang, D. Wilder, and R. Mayeux. 1994. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer ’s disease. JAMA 271: 1004–1010. Storey, J. E., J. T. J. Rowland, D. Basic, and D. A. Conforti. 2002. Accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test for detecting dementia in a multicultural sample of elderly Australian patients. International Psychogeriatrics 14 (3): 259–271. Sunderland, T., J. L. Hill, A. M. Mellow, et al. 1989. Clock drawing in Alzheimer ’s disease: A novel measure of dementia severity. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 37: 725–729. Tierney, M. C., J. P. Szalai, W. G. Snow, and R. H. Fisher. 1996. The prediction of Alzheimer disease: The role of patient and informant perceptions of cognitive deficits. Archives of Neurology 53 (5): 423–427. Van Dijk, E. J., M. M. B. Breteler, R. Schmidt, K. Berger, L. G. Nilsson, M. Oudkerk, et al. 2004. The association between blood pressure, hypertension, and cerebral white matter lesions: Cardiovascular determinants of dementia study. Hypertension 44: 625–630. Xu, G., J. S. Meyer, Y. Huang, F. Du, M. Chowdhury, and M. Quach. 2003. Adapting Mini-Mental State Examination for dementia screening among illiterate or minimally educated elderly Chinese. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 18 (7): 609–616. Wechsler, D. 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III). 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. 1997. Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III). 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Weyer, G., H. Erzigkeit, S. Kanowski, R. Ihl, and D. Hadler. 1997. Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment Scale: Reliability and validity in a multicenter clinical trial. International Psychogeriatrics 9: 123–138. Wilson, B. A., J. Cockburn, and A. Baddeley. 1985. The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test. Gaylord, MI: National Rehabilitation Services. Woodruff-Pak, D. S. 1997. The Neuropsychology of Aging. Malden, MA: Blackwell. World Health Organization. 2001. International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF). Geneva: World Health Organization. Yassuda, M. S., B. S. Diniz, L. F. Viola, F. S. Pereira, P. V. Nunes, and O. V. Forlenza. 2009. Neuropsychological profile of Brazilian older adults with heterogeneous educational backgrounds. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 24: 71–79.
Chapter 8
Does Poor Sleep Quality in Late Life Compromise Cognition and Accelerate Progression of the Degenerative Dementias? Peter Engel
Cycles of sleep and wakefulness begin in utero and persist throughout life. As such, sleep is both a personally familiar experience and a biologically essential activity. At the same time, the fundamental links between sleep, health, and disease remain elusive. Several decades of sleep research now link rapid eye movement (REM) and non–rapid eye movement (nREM) sleep with new learning and consolidation of both motor and episodic memory. While other functions of sleep escape full understanding, sleep appears to be essential for survival through regulation of growth, development, maintenance, and repair of the brain as well as the entire organism (Diekelmann and Born 2010; Garcia-Rill et al. 2008; Vassalli and Dijk 2009). Hence, the connections between sleep, learning, and memory may have implications for brain development and maintenance during early and mid-life, and cognitive decline associated with aging and degenerative brain disease in late life. These potential late life relationships will be the principal focus of this chapter. Over the lifespan crisp transitions between wakefulness and sleep and their tight circadian regulation diminish in intensity and precision. Sleep-stage transitions become more fragmented, sleep efficiency diminishes, and slow-wave sleep declines with age (Redline et al. 2004; Ohayon
178
Dementia
et al. 2004; Bloom et al. 2009; Dijk et al. 2010) (see Figure 8.1). These changes are enhanced in dementing disorders that are characterized by profound impairments of learning and memory. In some cases of dementia in which sleep disturbances are profound, components of wakefulness may infiltrate sleep (Cajochen et al. 2006; Espiritu 2008; Gagnon et al. 2008). In Alzheimer ’s disease (AD), the dementia in which sleep and circadian disturbances are best studied, the major components of REM and nREM sleep appear to be relatively preserved despite progressive fragmentation of sleep architecture and varying degrees of cell loss in sleep-regulating regions of the brainstem, basal forebrain and hypothalamus. In Lewy body dementia (LBD), sleep and arousal mechanisms may be profoundly disturbed and REM sleep may be disrupted by intrusion of components of the waking state. In REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), the profound hypotonia of REM sleep fails to occur and patients appear to act out their dreams, often with violent gestures and vocalizations. In LBD and Parkinson’s disease, a related synucleopathy, RBD may precede the dementing illness by 10 years or more (Mahowald, Schenck, and Bornemann 2007; McKeith et al. 2005; Boeve et al. 2007; Iranzo, Santamaria, and Tolosa 2009; Postuma et al. 2009). In this chapter we will consider two possible associations between decrements in sleep quality, age-related memory decline, and dementia: first, that age and dementia-related loss of sleep integrity directly contribute to cognitive impairment, and second, that age and disease-associated circadian and sleep disturbances enhance brain injury and accelerate disease progression. To address these questions the discussion will concentrate on AD, the most thoroughly studied dementia for which animal models are available, and LBD, given ample data on RBD and cognitive deficits associated with this disorder. To investigate this broad area, identify gaps, and suggest future directions we will begin by examining sleep architecture in relation to aging, AD, and LBD and relate these to neuroanatomical correlates of sleep mechanisms to the extent that they are understood. An exploration of human and animal research that links sleep to learning and memory consolidation will follow. This background will form the basis for an investigation of the hypotheses proposed above. Three points deserve mention early in this discussion. First, this chapter represents an attempt to identify connections between several broad and disparate areas of research in sleep, learning, and dementia. Second, treatment of sleep disorders in dementia both pharmacological and otherwise are well considered elsewhere and will not be addressed here (Espiritu
Figure 8.1
Hypnograms characteristic of young adults and elderly individuals. Decrements in sleep architecture in the elderly are characterized by delayed sleep onset, sleep fragmentation, early morning awakening, and decreased slow wave sleep (stages 3 and 4). (Reprinted with permission from “Sleep Problems in the Elderly,” May 1, 1999, American Family Physician. Copyright ©1999 American Academy of Family Physicians. All rights reserved.)
180
Dementia
2008; Boeve, “Update” 2008; Dauvillers 2007; Deschenes and McCurry 2009). Third, uncertainties related to the neuropathological and clinical features of AD and LBD point to the difficulties in the study of aging and dementia. The neuropathological differences between age-related change, Alzheimer ’s disease and Lewy body dementia are hardly distinct, and clinical variability in disease expression from one individual to another can be substantial. Alzheimer and Lewy body pathology commonly cooccur with the predominant features determining a specific diagnosis (Jellinger 2008; Kazee and Han 1995). These realities begin to frame the inherent limitations associated with any attempt to understand the relationships between sleep, aging, and the degenerative dementias. But such a caveat need not dissuade this exploration. Since the earliest neuropathological change in AD begins in the allocortex and cortex with variable involvement of sleep and arousal systems in brainstem and midbrain, sleep disturbances in AD might be considered an exaggerated form of normal aging. These medial temporal lobe and cortical changes likely account for impairments of learning, memory, and abstract reasoning, and reduced fluency that characterize early stages of this disorder (Dubois et al. 2007; McKhann et al. 1984; American Psychiatric Association 2000; Braak and Braak 1991; Thal et al. 2002; Schneider et al. 2009). LBD, in contrast, may be a “bottom up” dementia in which the earliest pathological changes occur in the brainstem, or systemically in the gut and spread rostrally with early involvement of sleep-regulating nuclei. This sequence may explain the presence of RBD as a common precursor of LBD with later evidence of deficits of attention, executive function, and visuospatial ability often with fluctuating cognition, visual hallucinations, and features of Parkinsonism (McKeith et al. 2005; Phillips et al. 2008; Braak et al. 2003). Memory may be spared early on in LBD, distinguishing this condition from AD. In relation to the two hypotheses under consideration, only the first, a possible relationship between sleep disturbances and cognitive impairment of LBD, will gain scrutiny here. Exploration of the second is precluded by an insufficient understanding of the factors that modulate the pathological processes associated with this disease. SLEEP AND AROUSAL: NEUROBIOLOGY AND NEUROANATOMY Arousal is mediated by the reticular activating system situated in the upper brainstem near the pons-midbrain junction where cholinergic and monaminergic cell groups constitute the central components. The cholinergic pedunculopointine (PPT) and laterodorsal (LDT) tegmental nuclei in the mesopontine tegmentum project to thalmocortical nuclei and
Poor Sleep Quality
181
the reticular nucleus to facilitate thalmocortical sensory transmission. Cholinergic basal forebrain projections to the cortex are implicated in waking and electroencephalographic (EEG) desynchronization. Lesions of the basal forebrain produce coma, underscoring the critical importance of this area in arousal. The monoaminergic arousal system comprised of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus, dopaminergic ventral preaqueductal gray, serotoninergic raphe nuclei, and histaminergic tuberomammillary neurons project to the thalamus, lateral hypothalamus (LH), basal forebrain, and cortex. Orexin-secreting neurons in the LH project reciprocally to the brainstem monaminergic systems. Orexin, a peptide that promotes arousal and appetite increases firing rates in the monoaminergic arousal system while melanin-concentrating hormone neurons from LH inhibit the arousal system and mediate REM sleep homeostasis (Fuller, Gooley, and Saper 2006; Saper, Scammell, and Lu 2005; Schwartz and Roth 2008) (see Figure 8.2). During sleep, the hypothalamic ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO) inhibits arousal circuits, an effect mediated by gamma amino buteric acid (GABA) and galanin. VLPO lesions produce profound insomnia in animals. Loss of VLPO neurons as occurs in aging and AD may contribute to insomnia and sleep fragmentation that can occur in these conditions. VLPO and arousal systems are mutually inhibitory with functions comparable to an electronic “flip-flop” switch imparting a degree of stability to either the waking or sleeping state (Saper and Scammell 2005). Cell loss also occurs in other sleep-modulating hypothalamic and brainstem nuclei in a variety of neurodegenerative diseases but shows no clear relationship with the associated sleep disturbances (Boeve et al. 2007; Benarroch et al. 2009; Jellinger 1988; Mufson, Mash, and Hersh 1988; Ransmayr, Faucheux, and Nowakowski 2000; Saper and German 1987; Schmeichel et al. 2008; Szymusiak, Gvilia, and McGinty 2007; Zhang et al. 2005; Zweig et al. 1989). Transition from nREM to REM sleep is also mediated by a proposed “flip-flop” switch control of which appears to be progressively compromised by aging and degenerative neurological diseases. Mutually inhibitory cholinergic “REM-on” PPT neurons complement serotonergic dorsal raphe and noradrenergic locus coeruleus “REM-off” cells (Hobson 2009; Monti and Monti 2007). Dopaminergic and cholinergic nuclei are active during REM. In the rat REM-on activity in the subcoeruleus region is associated with electromyographic atonia and appears to effect this state through hyperpolarization of spinal cord anterior horn motor neurons during REM sleep. Loss of atonia as occurs in RBD suggests focal brainstem injury to these anterior horn cell inhibitory circuits (Boeve, “Update” 2008).
182
Dementia
Figure 8.2
Simplified schematic of major sleep-regulating regions. Brainstem nuclei for arousal and REM/slow-wave sleep regulation; locus coeruleus (norepinephrine), raphe (serotonin), tuberomammilliary nucleus (histamine), ventral periaqueductal grey (dopamine), pedunculopontine and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei (acetylcholine). Hypothalamus; ventrolateral preoptic nucleus promotes sleep (GABA, galanin, inhibitory transmitters). Orexin from lateral hypothalamus promotes wakefulness. SCN, suprachiasmatic nucleus. (Modified with permission from Macmillan Publishers, Ltd.: Emmanuel Mignot, Shahrad Taheri, and Seiji Nishino, “Sleeping with the hypothalamus: Emerging therapeutic targets for sleep disorders,” Nature Neuroscience 5: 1071–1075. Copyright © 2002.)
In general, all cell groups fire more during wakefulness than nREM sleep whereas REM sleep is a cholinergically hypermodulated and aminergically demodulated state. Hence the cholinergic deficiency associated with both AD and LBD may contribute to the fragmentation of REM sleep periods (Hobson 2009).
Poor Sleep Quality
183
The suprachaismic nucleus of the hypothalamus exerts overall circadian control of sleep and wakefulness. In aging as well as AD and other degenerative dementias substantial cell loss occurs in the suprachaismic nucleus that is associated with reduction in the amplitude of various circadian rhythms and a phase advance, particularly for sleep. Reduction in sleep quality and increased sleep fragmentation are likely related to these changes (Braak and Braak 1992; Goudsmit et al. 1990; Mirmiran et al. 1992). SLEEP ARCHITECTURE, AGING, AND ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE Polysomnographic studies indicate that sleep efficiency and depth diminish with age, sleep becomes fragmented, and nocturnal arousals occur more frequently. These changes are more prominent in AD as are circadian disturbances in sleep-wake activity (Vassalli and Dijk 2009; Redline et al. 2004; Ohayon et al. 2004; Bloom et al. 2009; Dijk et al. 2010; Cajochen et al. 2006; Espiritu 2008; Gagnon et al. 2008; Bliwise 2004). Normal aging and AD are associated with diminished slow-wave sleep (SWS) and a reduced number of sleep spindles during stage 2 sleep (see Figure 8.1 above). Sleep spindles have been implicated in hippocampal-related learning. In addition, both human and animal studies indicate that sleep deprivation, sleep fragmentation, and reduction in REM or nREM sleep can impair motor and episodic learning (Chee and Chuah 2008; Walker 2008; Banks and Dinges 2007; Schabus et al. 2007; Sterpenich et al. 2007). None of these investigations included aged subjects. Do Age-Related Changes in Sleep Produce Memory Impairment? Age and AD-related changes in sleep are not readily comparable to sleep disturbances produced in experimental paradigms but several parallels may be of interest. An extensive literature has defined the learning impairments associated with sleep deprivation and the need for sleep following learning for consolidation and enhancement of procedural and declarative memory. Both REM and nREM sleep enhance the consolidation of motor and episodic memory as well as other forms of learning. Current sleep research suggests that SWS supports system consolidation while REM sleep mediates synaptic consolidation. SWS slow oscillations, sleep spindles, and high-frequency hippocampal ripple oscillations are purported to coordinate reactivation and redistribution of hippocampal dependent memories to the neocortex, a phenomenon that occurs during low cholinergic states.
184
Dementia
During REM, high cholinergic activity may promote synaptic consolidation. Optimum benefits of sleep for memory consolidation appear to occur when SWS is followed by REM sleep, the usual sequence in young humans and animals. Moreover, cholinergic activation strengthens long-term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampal-medial prefrontal cortex pathway. These observations introduce the possibility that the cholinergic deficiency of AD and LBD combined with disruption of sleep architecture and loss of SWS compromise this dynamic of sleep-induced memory consolidation (Diekelmann and Born 2010; Walker 2009; Stickgold and Walker 2007). Electrophysiological studies suggest that sleep spindles characteristic of stage 2 nREM sleep provide trains of depolarizations to cortex and hippocampus that are comparable to spike trains of in-vitro LTP, a synaptic mechanism implicated in learning (Steriade 1999). Such observations may be linked to impaired learning associated with the reduced number of sleep spindles both in aging and Alzheimer ’s disease, the smaller number of fast spindles in AD patients, and a positive correlation between the intensity of fast spindles and post-sleep immediate recall (Rauchs et al. 2008). Two preliminary investigations suggest a relationship between sleep, learning, and aging. In the first, compared to their younger counterparts, middle-aged subjects demonstrated a decrement in declarative memory that correlated with reduced SWS time (Backhaus et al. 2007). The second showed that improved episodic memory tracked with total sleep time but not specific sleep stage in both young and old subjects (Aly and Moscovitch 2010). Hence age-associated loss of sleep integrity that is exaggerated in Alzheimer ’s disease may be a direct contributor to clinically evident cognitive impairments through decrements in sleep-related memory consolidation and learning processes. Concurrently, age-related reduction in sleep quality combined with age-associated disease may accelerate the pathogenic processes of Alzheimer ’s disease. We will specifically explore this possibility by examining potential relationships between aging, agerelated disorders, and the production of amyloid beta (A-beta). Does the Loss of Sleep Integrity Accelerate Alzheimer ’s Disease Pathogenic Processes? A predominant hypothesis of AD pathogenesis posits the toxicity of A-beta a 40-42 amino acid peptide produced by neurons as a fundamental pathogenic process. Substantial experimental evidence shows that A-beta 42 is neurotoxic, particularly in its soluble, oligameric forms and
Poor Sleep Quality
185
that A-beta is either produced in excess or inefficiently catabolized such that the peptide concentrations reach toxic levels. The neurotoxic effects of A-beta oligomers include depression of LTP, enhancement of long-term depression (LTD), inhibition of synaptogenesis, neuronal cell death and apoptosis (Yang, Hsu, and Kuo 2009). The relationship between A-beta and phosphorylated tau paired helical filaments that accumulate in dying Alzheimer disease neurons is less clear (Querfurth and LaFerla 2010). A-beta is released into the synaptic space during depolarization, raising the possibility that it functions as a neuromodulator of LTP and synaptogenesis (Wasling et al. 2009). The assumption of a physiological function for A-beta is suggested by recent reports of enhanced memory retention and acetylcholine production in hippocampus in response to low doses of A-beta, impaired learning in normal mice following inhibition of A-beta expression, and in-vitro inhibition of hippocampal and dentate gyrus LTP in the presence of A-beta antibodies (Morley et al. 2010). In complementary studies, A-beta 42 in picomolar concentrations facilitated hippocampal LTP whereas nanomolar quantities had the opposite effect. Concurrently picomolar levels of A-beta improved reference and fear memory (Puzzo et al. 2008). Other products of amyloid precursor protein have potential physiological functions. Knockout mice, deficient in neprilysin an amyloid degrading endopeptidase, showed increased A-beta concentrations in the brain. Aged NEP knockout animals demonstrated significantly improved learning and memory and improved LTP in hippocampus and amygdala. This improvement may reflect increased levels of A-beta or other neuropeptides usually metabolized by NEP (Walther et al. 2009). Secreted amyloid precursor protein alpha (sAPP), a product of alpha secretase that incorporates a portion of the A-beta peptide facilitates LTP in rat dentate gyrus in vitro. Preferred production of A-beta at the expense of sAPP alpha may be another potential contributor of the memory deficits of AD (Kim and Tsai 2009; Lauren et al. 2009; Taylor et al. 2008; Bissette 2009). A key point is that dysregulation of A-beta production may be the principal mediator of both immediate adverse effects on cognition through impairment of LTP and longer-term injury-inducing effects in brain areas with the greatest synaptic plasticity. In AD the earliest pathological changes occur in the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus, regions that are critical for learning and memory and like the cortical association areas demonstrate considerable synaptic plasticity (Braak and Braak 1991). Disruption of the tightly regulated function of A-beta as a modulator of synaptic plasticity could result in toxic concentrations of this peptide through a variety of mechanisms, some of
186
Dementia
which may be related to disturbances of sleep integrity, others to ageassociated diseases, brain injury by various mechanisms, and the aging process itself. The fact that Alzheimer neuropathology first evolves in brain areas of high synaptic plasticity representing components of the memory retrieval network (posterior cortical regions, including posterior cingulate, retrosplenial, and lateral parietal cortex) where atrophy is prominent in AD implicates the relative vulnerability of the most dynamic parts of the brain to AD-related injury (Buckner et al. 2005, 2009). These observations lead to a second question: might age and dementia-related decrements in sleep integrity disrupt tightly regulated A-beta mechanisms, facilitate A-beta production, increase neurotoxicity, and promote disease progression? Preliminary information provides initial support for this idea. First, both wild type and Amyloid precursor protein Tg2576 transgenic mice demonstrate diurnal variation in A-beta levels as shown by brain in-vivo microdialysis. Comparable changes have been observed in the spinal fluid of human volunteers. Higher levels of A-beta occur during periods of wakefulness, an effect that is likely mediated by orexin. In transgenic mice sleep deprivation, a form of physiologic stress, results in increased A-beta levels and enhanced amyloid plaque deposition. Notably restraint stress acutely increases A-beta concentrations, an effect mediated by corticotrophin releasing factor (Kang et al. 2009). Second, studies of sleep and circadian abnormalities in Tg2576 Alzheimer model mice show changes that mimic those in aged humans and patients with AD (Zhang et al. 2005; Bliwise 2004). These animals demonstrated a blunted increase in electroencephalographic delta power (i.e., a loss of slow-wave sleep EEG frequencies) following sleep deprivation, longer periods of wheel running activity during dark (wake) periods, and a shift in EEG power to higher frequencies during nREM sleep as compared to normal controls (Wisor et al. 2005; Volicer et al. 2001). Whether dysregulation of A-beta production in these animals accounts for these sleep changes or the other way around remains to be seen. If we accept the possibility that excess A-beta is a significant participant in the pathogenesis of AD, that sleep disruption increases A-beta production, and that sleep becomes lighter and more fragmented with age then it may be of value to explore potential relationships between aging, sleep, cognitive decline and dementia. As previously discussed, both learning and memory consolidation occur during REM and SWS (Diekelmann and Born 2010; Walker 2009; Stickgold and Walker 2007). Disruption of normal sleep patterns compromises these functions. Sleep deprivation impairs hippocampal LTP and synaptic plasticity while enhancing LTD (Tadavarty, Kaan, and Sastry 2009; Kopp et al. 2006; Guzman-Marin et al.
Poor Sleep Quality
187
2006; McDermott et al. 2006). Similar disturbances occur following REM sleep deprivation and sleep fragmentation (Ravassard et al. 2009; Ishikawa et al. 2006; Tartar et al. 2006). These neurobiological effects are largely reiterated in studies of the effects of A-beta peptide on hippocampal function. Soluble oligomers of A-beta peptide inhibit LTP and enhance LTD in the CA1 region of rat hippocampus, an effect mediated in part by inhibition of glutamate reuptake, and by disruption of LTP through inhibition of the N-methyl-D-apartate receptor-dependent LTP induction (Li et al. 2009; Yamin 2009). A-beta induces dendritic spine loss, while A-beta protein fragments 25-35 and 31-35 potentiate hippocampal CA1 LTD in vivo (Hsieh et al. 2006; Cheng et al. 2009). To date there is no direct evidence that decrements in sleep quality associated with aging and dementia directly contribute to deficits in learning and memory, depression of LTP, and acceleration of neurodegenerative processes that occur in sleep-deprived animals. Nonetheless, a potential connection may be more likely in late life if age-related change and ageassociated disease enhance brain vulnerability. Of particular interest are age-related changes in the adrenal-hippocampal, pituitary axis, the proinflammatory state of late life, impaired cerebrovascular autoregulation, and age-related disorders such as sleep apnea, periodic leg movements during sleep, and restless leg syndrome.
COMPONENTS OF AGING AND AGE-RELATED DISEASE PROCESSES THAT MAY ENHANCE THE PRODUCTION OF A-BETA Glucocorticoids Basal cortisol levels increase with age, an effect that is enhanced in AD with the diurnal peak in serum cortisol occurring early in the morning. Hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors mediate the effects of cortisol on the hippocampus that include inhibition of LTP in response to acute elevations and atrophy following chronic elevations. The electrochemical response to glucocorticoids may be directly mediated by elevated A-beta levels, resulting from increased production and blunted metabolism of the peptide. Moreover, stress levels of glucocorticoids increase A-beta and tau pathology in a mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease (Catania et al. 2009; Green et al. 2006; Kulstad et al. 2005; Magri et al. 2006; McAuley et al. 2009; Sotiropoulos et al. 2008; Yao et al. 2007). Notably, the elevations of cortisol, catecholamines, and inflammatory markers characteristic of
188
Dementia
aging are induced in younger individuals by sleep deprivation (Galvao Mde et al. 2009; Leproult and Van Cauter 2010; Mullington et al. 2009; Stamatakis and Punjabi 2010). These, in turn may be associated with increased A-beta production (Kang et al. 2009). Immune Function, Sleep and A-beta Production Intrinsic immunity becomes increasingly pro-inflammatory with age, as reflected by a shift in the mix of circulating cytokines from an anti-inflammatory Th-1 to a Th-2 pro-inflammatory pattern. A similar pro-inflammatory cytokine shift occurs in the aging brain where neuro-inflammation is marked by increased numbers of activated and primed microglia that are hyperresponsive to systemic inflammatory signals or a stressor (Goshen and Yirmiya 2009; Dilger and Johnson 2008; Godbout and Johnson 2009; Sparkman and Johnson 2008). Peripheral inflammatory activity may be enhanced by sleep deprivation although the effect of sleep deprivation or fragmentation on the CNS inflammatory response has not been clearly defined (Irwin et al. 2008; Yehuda et al. 2009). Proinflammatory mediators increase brain A-beta production. Such is the case for systemic administration of lipopolysaccharide to APPswe transgenic mice in which A-beta production is increased threefold. Prostaglandin E2 stimulates A-beta production in vitro and possibly in vivo. In-vitro, glial interferon gamma and tumor necrosis factor enhance A-beta production, directly stimulate the beta-site APP cleaving enzyme (BACE 1), and suppress A-beta degradation (Sheng et al. 2003; Hoshino et al. 2009; Yamamoto et al. 2007; Hoshino et al. 2007). While there are many gaps in the data, one might consider the possibility that inefficient, fragmented, slow-wave-deficient sleep of old age contributes to a chronic stress response that results in increased A-beta production, an effect that is exaggerated within systemic and central nervous system environments of inflammatory hyperresponsiveness. Any type of brain injury enhances production of A-beta, raising the possibility that this or other APP-derived peptides have another physiologic function in the acute response to injury. The injury response includes activation of inflammatory mediators. Traumatic brain injury is a well-established risk factor for AD such that dysregulation of the injury response may facilitate later development of a neurodegenerative disease. The relationships between the neurophysiologic and neurotoxic effects of A-beta and dysregulation of A-beta function through age associated changes in sleep, stress and inflammatory responses suggest a link
Poor Sleep Quality
189
between these variables that deserves further investigation (Mattson et al. 1997; Truettner, Suzuki, and Dietrich 2005; Van Den Heuvel, Thornton, and Vink 2007; Uryu et al. 2007). Do Age-Related Conditions Enhance Brain Vulnerability to Degenerative Changes? Sleep apnea, restless leg syndrome (RLS), and periodic leg movements during sleep (PLMS) are common in late life (Tarasiuk et al. 2008; Al Lawati, Patel, and Ayas 2009; Karatas 2007). Episodic hypoventilation during sleep apnea may be associated with oxygen desaturation. RLS and PLMS delay sleep onset or disrupt sleep continuity. The recurrent hypoxia associated with sleep apnea may be related to animal studies of cerebral hypoperfusion and ischemia, both of which have been associated with depression of LTP as well as increased A-beta production (Zhang et al. 2007; Li et al. 2010; Gasparova, Jariabka, and Stolc 2008; Guglielmotto et al. 2009). Cerebrovascular dysfunction that occurs in AD has been related to deposition of A-beta in blood vessels in cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA). CAA occurs to varying degrees in nearly all patients with Alzheimer ’s disease and in 60–75% of normal octogenarians. CAA results in impaired vasodilator as well as vasoconstrictor responses, capillary occlusions, and microbleeds, all of which could potentially accelerate A-beta production in vulnerable areas of the brain (Shin et al. 2007; Thal et al. 2008). Virtually no convincing evidence links age-related elevations in glucocorticoids to accelerated brain aging, the proinflammatory state of aging to neurodegeneration, or age-related reductions in sleep efficiency to decrements in learning and memory. These intriguing possibilities deserve further experimental exploration. REM SLEEP BEHAVIOR DISORDER AND LEWY BODY DEMENTIA In contrast to AD, the neuropathological processes of LBD and the other synucleopathies are first evident in the brainstem and disseminate rostrally, although recent reports cite a considerable number of exceptions to this pattern (Jellinger 2008; Jost 2010; Braak et al. 2004). To date no pathologically confirmed case of AD has been associated with REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD). This is a remarkable contrast to LBD, Parkinson’s disease, and other synnucleopathies in which RBD prevalence may reach 40%. Sleep architecture is little studied in LBD although preliminary information suggests sleep fragmentation and loss of sleep efficiency
190
Dementia
(McKeith et al. 2005; Jellinger 2008; Boeve, “Polysomnographic evidence” 2008). Many components of sleep-regulatory mechanisms are mediated by the brainstem, basal forebrain, and hypothalamus, but the degeneration of specific cholinergic neurons in the PPT and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei as well as dopamine cell loss in the periaqueductal gray in LBD and multiple system atrophy are not clearly associated with RBD (Benarroch et al. 2009; Schmeichel et al. 2008). Excess loss of dopamine cells may be related to daytime sleepiness observed in LBD. Cognitive deficits are identified in RBD, including poorer working memory, attention, visual and verbal memory, and executive function. Semantic memory, language, and visual perception are preserved. These deficits are mild but significant and are probably unrelated to specific RBD episodes because these typically occur infrequently, once or several times a month (Gagnon et al. 2009; Postuma, Gagnon, and Montplaisir 2008; Terzaghi et al. 2008; Massicotte-Marquez et al. 2008; Plazzi et al. 2005). Limited published data suggests that sleep architecture and sleep efficiency are not significantly altered in RBD, although a single study demonstrated EEG slowing in RBD patients comparable to that occurring in Alzheimer ’s disease both in the waking and sleeping states (Wetter et al. 2001; Fantini et al. 2003). Thus relative sleep deprivation does not appear to be a reasonable explanation for RBD cognitive deficits. Alternatively these findings may reflect the early phase of a more generalized degenerative process. The pattern of cognitive deficits in LBD patients that is distinguished by impairments in attention, executive tasks, and particularly in visuospatial and constructional abilities is more severe and somewhat different than that of RBD. Such differences are useful in distinguishing LBD from AD in which deficits of memory and language are prominent, but provide no obvious connection with the mild deficits of RBD (Tiraboschi et al. 2006). The presumed integrity of sleep architecture in RBD suggests that other manifestations of LBD, particularly visual hallucinations and illusions, are not mediated by the intrusion of sleep states, particularly REM dream material into wakefulness. These distortions of visual perception more likely reflect reduction of cholinergic input and hypometabolism of visual association areas related to cholinergic deafferentation from the basal forebrain (Klein et al. 2010). Similarly, links between the sleep disturbances of LDB and its pathological mechanisms remain unidentified. Alpha synuclein is a nuclear and presynaptic protein that in LBD, Parkinson’s disease, and other synucleopathies aggregates in intracellular inclusions known as Lewy bodies. The means by which aggregated alpha synuclein contributes to neuronal
Poor Sleep Quality
191
injury and death is less well defined as compared to the neurotoxicity of A-beta in AD. It does appear that in common with aggregated A-beta, aggregated alpha synuclein triggers microglial activation, neuroinflammation, and neuronal loss. The common co-occurrence of LBD and AD pathology suggests that progression of both diseases may be mediated by similar inflammatory mediators. Virtually no information relates sleep disturbances, physiological stress, glucocorticoids, or other age-related conditions to cognitive decline and disease progression in LBD (Roodveldt, Christodoulou, and Dobson 2008; Mrak and Griffin 2007; Sawada, Imamura, and Nagatsu 2006; Mackenzie 2000). SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The chapter addresses two hypotheses: first, age-related disruption of sleep architecture, depth and efficiency directly contributes to cognitive changes associated with aging and dementia, particularly memory loss; second these sleep disturbances, combined with aging effects and age-associated diseases, accelerate neuropathological processes of the degenerative dementias. Sleep contributes to learning and memory consolidation. Sleep disruption has the opposite effects and neurophysiologic studies suggest mechanisms such as inhibition of LTP. Parallel investigations of A-beta, a probable mediator of AD, indicate that many A-beta toxic effects are comparable to those associated with sleep disruption, sleep deprivation, in turn, increases A-beta production. The toxicity of A-beta may be enhanced by age-related diseases and the pro-inflammatory environment of an aged brain. A-beta likely has physiologic functions such as modulation of synaptogenesis. Dysregulation of such functions with excess A-beta production results in neurotoxicity. The relatively limited data supporting these ideas may be worthy of further experimental investigations. These speculations are not readily extrapolated to LBD but its frequent association with RBD as the earliest symptom indicates initial involvement of brainstem and midbrain sleep and arousal systems. Such involvement may be reflected by fluctuating cognition and variable levels of arousal and awareness associated with this illness. Such a connection has yet to be verified. Lack of evidence linking visual hallucinations of LBD or fluctuations in cognition and arousal to disturbance in sleep-regulating mechanisms is somewhat surprising. These phenomena are frequently observed in other disorders of sleep regulation such as narcolepsy and in normal individuals with sleep paralysis and hypnagogic hallucinations (Wurtman 2006; Cheyne 2005). Further studies of
192
Dementia
RBD and LBD are likely to clarify relationships between the instability and dysregulation of sleep states, arousal mechanisms, cognition, and other manifestations of LBD that are likely to be more varied and complex than currently appreciated (Mahowald and Schenck 1991; Vetrugno et al. 2009). Even when considered in isolation, mechanisms of aging, functions of sleep, and the fundamental causes of the degenerative dementias escape full understanding. In life the three are related and interactive at various levels of biological organization. Considering them together may provoke new questions and testable hypotheses. ACKNOWLEDGMENT: This chapter is supported in part by the GRECC, VA Boston Healthcare System, Geriatric Research, Educational and Clinical Center, 150 South Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA, 02130. REFERENCES Al Lawati, N. M., S. R. Patel, and N. T. Ayas. 2009. Epidemiology, risk factors, and consequences of obstructive sleep apnea and short sleep duration. Prog Cardiovasc Dis 51: 285–293. Aly, M., and M. Moscovitch. 2010. The effects of sleep on episodic memory in older and younger adults. Memory 18: 327–334. American Psychiatric Association, ed. 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (IV-TR). 4th ed. rev. Washington, DC. Backhaus, J., J. Born, R. Hoeckesfeld, S. Fokuhl, F. Hohagen, and K. Junghanns. 2007. Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep. Learn Mem 14: 336–341. Banks, S., and D. F. Dinges. 2007. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med 3: 519–528. Benarroch, E. E., A. M. Schmeichel, B. N. Dugger, P. Sandroni, J. E. Parisi, and P. A. Low. 2009. Dopamine cell loss in the periaqueductal gray in multiple system atrophy and Lewy body dementia. Neurology 73: 106–112. Bissette, G. 2009. Does Alzheimer ’s disease result from attempts at repair or protection after transient stress? J Alzheimers Dis 18: 371–380. Bliwise, D. L. 2004. Sleep disorders in Alzheimer ’s disease and other dementias. Clin Cornerstone 6 (Suppl 1A): S16–28. Bloom, H. G., I. Ahmed, C. A. Alessi, et al. 2009. Evidence-based recommendations for the assessment and management of sleep disorders in older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc 57: 761–789. Boeve, B. F. 2008. Polysomnographic evidence of sleep fragmentation and poor sleep efficiency in dementia with Lewy bodies. Alzheimer ’s and Dementia 4: T435.
Poor Sleep Quality
193
Boeve, B. F. 2008. Update on the diagnosis and management of sleep disturbances in dementia. Sleep Med Clin 3: 347–360. Boeve, B. F., M. H. Silber, C. B. Saper, et al. 2007. Pathophysiology of REM sleep behaviour disorder and relevance to neurodegenerative disease. Brain 130: 2770–2788. Braak, H., and E. Braak. 1991. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 82: 239–259. Braak, H., and E. Braak. 1992. Anatomy of the human hypothalamus (chiasmatic and tuberal region). Prog Brain Res 93: 3–14. Braak, H., K. Del Tredici, U. Rub, R. A. de Vos, E. N. Jansen Steur, and E. Braak. 2003. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 24: 197–211. Braak, H., E. Ghebremedhin, U. Rub, H. Bratzke, and K. Del Tredici. 2004. Stages in the development of Parkinson’s disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Res 318: 121–134. Buckner, R. L., J. Sepulcre, T. Talukdar, et al. 2009. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: Mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 29: 1860–1873. Buckner, R. L., A. Z. Snyder, B. J. Shannon, et al. 2005. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer ’s disease: Evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci 25: 7709–7717. Cajochen, C., M. Munch, V. Knoblauch, K. Blatter, and A. Wirz-Justice. 2006. Agerelated changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep. Chronobiol Int 23: 461–474. Catania, C., I. Sotiropoulos, R. Silva, et al. 2009. The amyloidogenic potential and behavioral correlates of stress. Mol Psychiatry 14: 95–105. Chee, M. W., and L. Y. Chuah. 2008. Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Curr Opin Neurol 21: 417–423. Cheng, L., W. J. Yin, J. F. Zhang, and J. S. Qi. 2009. Amyloid beta-protein fragments 25-35 and 31-35 potentiate long-term depression in hippocampal CA1 region of rats in vivo. Synapse 63: 206–214. Cheyne, J. A. 2005. Sleep paralysis episode frequency and number, types, and structure of associated hallucinations. J Sleep Res 14: 319–324. Dauvilliers, Y. 2007. Insomnia in patients with neurodegenerative conditions. Sleep Med 8 (Suppl 4): S27–34. Deschenes, C. L., and S. M. McCurry. 2009. Current treatments for sleep disturbances in individuals with dementia. Curr Psychiatry Rep 11: 20–26. Diekelmann, S., and J. Born. 2010. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 114–126. Dijk, D. J., J. A. Groeger, N. Stanley, and S. Deacon. 2010. Age-related reduction in daytime sleep propensity and nocturnal slow wave sleep. Sleep 33: 211–223.
194
Dementia
Dilger, R. N., and R. W. Johnson. 2008. Aging, microglial cell priming, and the discordant central inflammatory response to signals from the peripheral immune system. J Leukoc Biol 84: 932–939. Dubois, B., H. H. Feldman, C. Jacova, et al. 2007. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: Revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol 6: 734–746. Espiritu, J. R. 2008. Aging-related sleep changes. Clin Geriatr Med 24: 1–14, v. Fantini, M. L., J. F. Gagnon, D. Petit, et al. 2003. Slowing of electroencephalogram in rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Ann Neurol 53: 774–780. Fuller, P. M., J. J. Gooley, and C. B. Saper. 2006. Neurobiology of the sleep-wake cycle: Sleep architecture, circadian regulation, and regulatory feedback. J Biol Rhythms 21: 482–493. Gagnon, J. F., D. Petit, V. Latreille, and J. Montplaisir. 2008. Neurobiology of sleep disturbances in neurodegenerative disorders. Curr Pharm Des 14: 3430–3445. Gagnon, J. F., M. Vendette, R. B. Postuma, et al. 2009. Mild cognitive impairment in rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder and Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 66: 39–47. Galvao Mde, O., R. Sinigaglia-Coimbra, S. E. Kawakami, S. Tufik, and D. Suchecki. 2009. Paradoxical sleep deprivation activates hypothalamic nuclei that regulate food intake and stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34: 1176–1183. Garcia-Rill, E., A. Charlesworth, D. Heister, M. Ye, and A. Hayar. 2008. The developmental decrease in REM sleep: The role of transmitters and electrical coupling. Sleep 31: 673–690. Gasparova, Z., P. Jariabka, and S. Stolc. 2008. Effect of transient ischemia on longterm potentiation of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal slices. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 29: 702–705. Godbout, J. P., and R. W. Johnson. 2009. Age and neuroinflammation: A lifetime of psychoneuroimmune consequences. Immunol Allergy Clin North Am 29: 321–337. Goshen, I., and R. Yirmiya. 2009. Interleukin-1 (IL-1): A central regulator of stress responses. Front Neuroendocrinol 30: 30–45. Goudsmit, E., M. A. Hofman, E. Fliers, and D. F. Swaab. 1990. The supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei of the human hypothalamus in relation to sex, age and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 11: 529–536. Green, K. N., L. M. Billings, B. Roozendaal, J. L. McGaugh, and F. M. LaFerla. 2006. Glucocorticoids increase amyloid-beta and tau pathology in a mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 26: 9047–9056. Guglielmotto, M., M. Aragno, R. Autelli, et al. 2009. The up-regulation of BACE1 mediated by hypoxia and ischemic injury: Role of oxidative stress and HIF1alpha. J Neurochem 108: 1045–1056. Guzman-Marin, R., Z. Ying, N. Suntsova, et al. 2006. Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. J Physiol 575: 807–819.
Poor Sleep Quality
195
Hobson, J. A. 2009. REM sleep and dreaming: Towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 803–813. Hoshino, T., T. Nakaya, T. Homan, et al. 2007. Involvement of prostaglandin E2 in production of amyloid-beta peptides both in vitro and in vivo. J Biol Chem 282: 32676–32688. Hoshino, T., T. Namba, M. Takehara, et al. 2009. Prostaglandin E2 stimulates the production of amyloid-beta peptides through internalization of the EP4 receptor. J Biol Chem 284: 18493–18502. Hsieh, H., J. Boehm, C. Sato, et al. 2006. AMPAR removal underlies A-betainduced synaptic depression and dendritic spine loss. Neuron 52: 831–843. Iranzo, A., J. Santamaria, and E. Tolosa. 2009. The clinical and pathophysiological relevance of REM sleep behavior disorder in neurodegenerative diseases. Sleep Med Rev 13: 385–401. Irwin, M. R., M. Wang, D. Ribeiro, et al. 2008. Sleep loss activates cellular inflammatory signaling. Biol Psychiatry 64: 538–540. Ishikawa, A., Y. Kanayama, H. Matsumura, H. Tsuchimochi, Y. Ishida, and S. Nakamura. 2006. Selective rapid eye movement sleep deprivation impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 24: 243–248. Jellinger, K. 1988. The pedunculopontine nucleus in Parkinson’s disease, progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51: 540–543. Jellinger, K. A. 2008. A critical reappraisal of current staging of Lewy-related pathology in human brain. Acta Neuropathol 116: 1–16. Jost, W. H. 2010. Gastrointestinal dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease. J Neurol Sci 289: 69–73. Kang, J. E., M. M. Lim, R. J. Bateman, et al. 2009. Amyloid-beta dynamics are regulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle. Science 326: 1005–1007. Karatas, M. 2007. Restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movements during sleep: Diagnosis and treatment. Neurologist 13: 294–301. Kazee, A. M., and L. Y. Han. 1995. Cortical Lewy bodies in Alzheimer ’s disease. Arch Pathol Lab Med 119: 448–453. Kim, D., and L. H. Tsai. 2009. Bridging physiology and pathology in AD. Cell 137: 997–1000. Klein, J. C., C. Eggers, E. Kalbe, et al. 2010. Neurotransmitter changes in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson disease dementia in vivo. Neurology 74: 885–892. Kopp, C., F. Longordo, J. R. Nicholson, and A. Luthi. 2006. Insufficient sleep reversibly alters bidirectional synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor function. J Neurosci 26: 12456–12465. Kulstad, J. J., P. J. McMillan, J. B. Leverenz, et al. 2005. Effects of chronic glucocorticoid administration on insulin-degrading enzyme and amyloid-beta peptide in the aged macaque. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 64: 139–146.
196
Dementia
Lauren, J., D. A. Gimbel, H. B. Nygaard, J. W. Gilbert, and S. M. Strittmatter. 2009. Cellular prion protein mediates impairment of synaptic plasticity by amyloid-beta oligomers. Nature 457: 1128–1132. Leproult, R., and E. Van Cauter. 2010. Role of sleep and sleep loss in hormonal release and metabolism. Endocr Dev 17: 11–21. Li, S., S. Hong, N. E. Shepardson, D. M. Walsh, G. M. Shankar, and D. Selkoe. 2009. Soluble oligomers of amyloid beta protein facilitate hippocampal long-term depression by disrupting neuronal glutamate uptake. Neuron 62: 788–801. Li, S., W. Wang, C. Wang, and Y. Y. Tang. 2010. Possible involvement of NO/NOS signaling in hippocampal amyloid-beta production induced by transient focal cerebral ischemia in aged rats. Neurosci Lett 470: 106–110. Mackenzie, I. R. 2000. Activated microglia in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 55: 132–34. Magri, F., L. Cravello, L. Barili, et al. 2006. Stress and dementia: The role of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal axis. Aging Clin Exp Res 18: 167–170. Mahowald, M. W., and C. H. Schenck. 1991. Status dissociates—A perspective on states of being. Sleep 14: 69–79. Mahowald, M. W., C. H. Schenck, and M. A. Bornemann. 2007. Pathophysiologic mechanisms in REM sleep behavior disorder. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 7: 167–172. Massicotte-Marquez, J., A. Decary, J. F. Gagnon, et al. 2008. Executive dysfunction and memory impairment in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology 70: 1250–1257. Mattson, M. P., S. W. Barger, K. Furukawa, et al. 1997. Cellular signaling roles of TGF beta, TNF alpha and beta APP in brain injury responses and Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 23: 47–61. McAuley, M. T., R. A. Kenny, T. B. Kirkwood, D. J. Wilkinson, J. J. Jones, and V. M. Miller. 2009. A mathematical model of aging-related and cortisol induced hippocampal dysfunction. BMC Neurosci 10: 26. McDermott, C. M., M. N. Hardy, N. G. Bazan, and J. C. Magee. 2006. Sleep deprivation-induced alterations in excitatory synaptic transmission in the CA1 region of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol 570: 553–565. McKeith, I. G., D. W. Dickson, J. Lowe, et al. 2005. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 65: 1863–1872. McKhann, G., D. Drachman, M. Folstein, R. Katzman, D. Price, and E. M. Stadlan. 1984. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: Report of the NINCDSADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer ’s Disease. Neurology 34: 939–944. Mirmiran, M., D. F. Swaab, J. H. Kok, M. A. Hofman, W. Witting, and W. A. Van Gool. 1992. Circadian rhythms and the suprachiasmatic nucleus in perinatal development, aging and Alzheimer ’s disease. Prog Brain Res 93: 151–162; discussion 162–153.
Poor Sleep Quality
197
Monti, J. M., and D. Monti. 2007. The involvement of dopamine in the modulation of sleep and waking. Sleep Med Rev 11: 113–133. Morley, J. E., S. A. Farr, W. A. Banks, S. N. Johnson, K. A. Yamada, and L. Xu. 2010. A physiological role for amyloid-beta protein: Enhancement of learning and memory. J Alzheimers Dis 19: 441–449. Mrak, R. E., and W. S. Griffin. 2007. Common inflammatory mechanisms in Lewy body disease and Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 66: 683–686. Mufson, E. J., D. C. Mash, and L. B. Hersh. 1988. Neurofibrillary tangles in cholinergic pedunculopontine neurons in Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann Neurol 24: 623–629. Mullington, J. M., M. Haack, M. Toth, J. M. Serrador, and H. K. Meier-Ewert. 2009. Cardiovascular, inflammatory, and metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Prog Cardiovasc Dis 51: 294–302. Ohayon, M. M., M. A. Carskadon, C. Guilleminault, and M. V. Vitiello. 2004. Metaanalysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: Developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27: 1255–1273. Phillips, R. J., G. C. Walter, S. L. Wilder, E. A. Baronowsky, and T. L. Powley. 2008. Alpha-synuclein-immunopositive myenteric neurons and vagal preganglionic terminals: Autonomic pathway implicated in Parkinson’s disease? Neuroscience 153: 733–750. Plazzi, G., R. Vetrugno, F. Provini, and P. Montagna. 2005. Sleepwalking and other ambulatory behaviours during sleep. Neurol Sci 26 (Suppl 3): s193–198. Postuma, R. B., J. F. Gagnon, and J. Montplaisir. 2008. Cognition in REM sleep behavior disorder—A window into preclinical dementia? Sleep Med 9: 341–342. Postuma, R. B., J. F. Gagnon, M. Vendette, and J. Y. Montplaisir. 2009. Idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder in the transition to degenerative disease. Mov Disord 24: 2225–2232. Puzzo, D., L. Privitera, E. Leznik, et al. 2008. Picomolar amyloid-beta positively modulates synaptic plasticity and memory in hippocampus. J Neurosci 28: 14537–14545. Querfurth, H. W., and F. M. LaFerla. 2010. Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 362: 329–344. Ransmayr, G., B. Faucheux, C. Nowakowski, et al. 2000. Age-related changes of neuronal counts in the human pedunculopontine nucleus. Neurosci Lett 288: 195–198. Rauchs, G., M. Schabus, S. Parapatics, et al. 2008. Is there a link between sleep changes and memory in Alzheimer ’s disease? NeuroReport 19: 1159–1162. Ravassard, P., B. Pachoud, J. C. Comte, et al. 2009. Paradoxical (REM) sleep deprivation causes a large and rapidly reversible decrease in long-term potentiation, synaptic transmission, glutamate receptor protein levels, and ERK/ MAPK activation in the dorsal hippocampus. Sleep 32: 227–240.
198
Dementia
Redline, S., H. L. Kirchner, S. F. QuanF, D. J. Gottlieb, V. Kapur, and A. Newman. 2004. The effects of age, sex, ethnicity, and sleep-disordered breathing on sleep architecture. Arch Intern Med 164: 406–418. Roodveldt, C., J. Christodoulou, and C. M. Dobson. 2008. Immunological features of alpha-synuclein in Parkinson’s disease. J Cell Mol Med 12: 1820–1829. Saper, C. B., and D. C. German. 1987. Hypothalamic pathology in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurosci Lett 74: 364–370. Saper, C. B., T. E. Scammell, and J. Lu. 2005. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature 437: 1257–1263. Sawada, M., K. Imamura, and T. Nagatsu. 2006. Role of cytokines in inflammatory process in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm (Suppl) 70: 373–381. Schabus, M., T. T. Dang-Vu, G. Albouy, et al. 2007. Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 13164–13169. Schmeichel, A. M., L. C. Buchhalter, P. A. Low, et al. 2008. Mesopontine cholinergic neuron involvement in Lewy body dementia and multiple system atrophy. Neurology 70: 368–373. Schneider, J. A., Z. Arvanitakis, S. E. Leurgans, and D. A. Bennett. 2009. The neuropathology of probable Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 66: 200–208. Schwartz, J. R., and T. Roth. 2008. Neurophysiology of sleep and wakefulness: Basic science and clinical implications. Curr Neuropharmacol 6: 367–378. Sheng, J. G., S. H. Bora, G. Xu, D. R. Borchelt, D. L. Price, and V. E. Koliatsos. 2003. Lipopolysaccharide-induced-neuroinflammation increases intracellular accumulation of amyloid precursor protein and amyloid beta peptide in APPswe transgenic mice. Neurobiol Dis 14: 133–145. Shin, H. K., P. B. Jones, M. Garcia-Alloza, et al. 2007. Age-dependent cerebrovascular dysfunction in a transgenic mouse model of cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Brain 130: 2310–2319. Sotiropoulos, I., C. Catania, T. Riedemann, et al. 2008. Glucocorticoids trigger Alzheimer disease-like pathobiochemistry in rat neuronal cells expressing human tau. J Neurochem 107: 385–397. Sparkman, N. L., and R. W. Johnson. 2008. Neuroinflammation associated with aging sensitizes the brain to the effects of infection or stress. Neuroimmunomodulation 15: 323–330. Stamatakis, K. A., and N. M. Punjabi. 2010. Effects of sleep fragmentation on glucose metabolism in normal subjects. Chest 137: 95–101. Steriade, M. 1999. Coherent oscillations and short-term plasticity in corticothalamic networks. Trends Neurosci 22: 337–345. Sterpenich, V., G. Albouy, M. Boly, et al. 2007. Sleep-related hippocampo-cortical interplay during emotional memory recollection. PLoS Biol 5: e282. Stickgold, R., and M. P. Walker. 2007. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Sleep Med 8: 331–343.
Poor Sleep Quality
199
Szymusiak, R., I. Gvilia, and D. McGinty. 2007. Hypothalamic control of sleep. Sleep Med 8: 291–301. Tadavarty, R., T. K. Kaan, and B. R. Sastry. 2009. Long-term depression of excitatory synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal CA1 neurons following sleep-deprivation. Exp Neurol 216: 239–242. Tarasiuk, A., S. Greenberg-Dotan, T. Simon-Tuval, A. Oksenberg, and H. Reuveni. 2008. The effect of obstructive sleep apnea on morbidity and health care utilization of middle-aged and older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 56: 247–254. Tartar, J. L., C. P. Ward, J. T. McKenna, et al. 2006. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation. Eur J Neurosci 23: 2739–2748. Taylor, C. J., D. R. Ireland, I. Ballagh, et al. 2008. Endogenous secreted amyloid precursor protein-alpha regulates hippocampal NMDA receptor function, long-term potentiation and spatial memory. Neurobiol Dis 31: 250–260. Terzaghi, M., E. Sinforiani, C. Zucchella, et al. 2008. Cognitive performance in REM sleep behaviour disorder: A possible early marker of neurodegenerative disease? Sleep Med 9: 343–351. Thal, D. R., W. S. Griffin, R. A. de Vos, and E. Ghebremedhin. 2008. Cerebral amyloid angiopathy and its relationship to Alzheimer ’s disease. Acta Neuropathol 115: 599–609. Thal, D. R., U. Rub, M. Orantes, and H. Braak. 2002. Phases of A beta-deposition in the human brain and its relevance for the development of AD. Neurology 58: 1791–1800. Tiraboschi, P., D. P. Salmon, L. A. Hansen, R. C. Hofstetter, L. J. Thal, and J. CoreyBloom. 2006. What best differentiates Lewy body from Alzheimer ’s disease in early-stage dementia? Brain 129: 729–735. Truettner, J. S., T. Suzuki, and W. D. Dietrich. 2005. The effect of therapeutic hypothermia on the expression of inflammatory response genes following moderate traumatic brain injury in the rat. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 138: 124–134. Uryu, K., X. H. Chen, D. Martinez, et al. 2007. Multiple proteins implicated in neurodegenerative diseases accumulate in axons after brain trauma in humans. Exp Neurol 208: 185–192. Van Den Heuvel, C., E. Thornton, and R. Vink. 2007. Traumatic brain injury and Alzheimer ’s disease: A review. Prog Brain Res 161: 303–316. Vassalli, A., and D. J. Dijk. 2009. Sleep function: current questions and new approaches. Eur J Neurosci 29: 1830–1841. Vetrugno, R., M. Alessandria, R. D’Angelo, et al. 2009. Status dissociatus evolving from REM sleep behaviour disorder in multiple system atrophy. Sleep Med 10: 247–252. Volicer, L., D. G. Harper, B. C. Manning, R. Goldstein, and A. Satlin. 2001. Sundowning and circadian rhythms in Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 158: 704–711. Walker, M. P. 2008. Cognitive consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Med 9 (Suppl 1): S29–34.
200
Dementia
Walker, M. P. 2009. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann NY Acad Sci 1156: 168–197. Walther, T., D. Albrecht, M. Becker, et al. 2009. Improved learning and memory in aged mice deficient in amyloid beta-degrading neutral endopeptidase. PLoS One 4: e4590. Wasling, P., J. Daborg, I. Riebe, et al. 2009. Synaptic retrogenesis and amyloid-beta in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 16: 1–14. Wetter, T. C., C. Trenkwalder, O. Gershanik, and B. Hogl. 2001. Polysomnographic measures in Parkinson’s disease: a comparison between patients with and without REM sleep disturbances. Wien Klin Wochenschr 113: 249–253. Wisor, J. P., D. M. Edgar, J. Yesavage, et al. 2005. Sleep and circadian abnormalities in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease: a role for cholinergic transmission. Neuroscience 131: 375–385. Wurtman, R. J. 2006. Narcolepsy and the hypocretins. Metabolism 55: S36–39. Yamamoto, M., T. Kiyota, M. Horiba, et al. 2007. Interferon-gamma and tumor necrosis factor-alpha regulate amyloid-beta plaque deposition and betasecretase expression in Swedish mutant APP transgenic mice. Am J Pathol 170: 680–692. Yamin, G. 2009. NMDA receptor-dependent signaling pathways that underlie amyloid beta-protein disruption of LTP in the hippocampus. J Neurosci Res 87: 1729–1736. Yang, T. T., C. T. Hsu, and Y. M. Kuo. 2009. Cell-derived soluble oligomers of human amyloid-beta peptides disturb cellular homeostasis and induce apoptosis in primary hippocampal neurons. J Neural Transm 116: 1561–1569. Yao, Y. Y., D. M. Liu, D. F. Xu, and W. P. Li. 2007. Memory and learning impairment induced by dexamethasone in senescent but not young mice. Eur J Pharmacol 574: 20–28. Yehuda, S., B. Sredni, R. L. Carasso, and D. Kenigsbuch-Sredni. 2009. REM sleep deprivation in rats results in inflammation and interleukin-17 elevation. J Interferon Cytokine Res 29: 393–398. Zhang, B., S. C. Veasey, M. A. Wood, et al. 2005. Impaired rapid eye movement sleep in the Tg2576 APP murine model of Alzheimer ’s disease with injury to pedunculopontine cholinergic neurons. Am J Pathol 167: 1361–1369. Zhang, X., K. Zhou, R. Wang, et al. 2007. Hypoxia-inducible factor 1alpha (HIF1alpha)-mediated hypoxia increases BACE1 expression and beta-amyloid generation. J Biol Chem 282: 10873–10880. Zweig, R. M., W. R. Jankel, J. C. Hedreen, R. Mayeux, and D. L. Price. 1989. The pedunculopontine nucleus in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 26: 41–46.
Chapter 9
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy: A Tool for Understanding Brain Chemical Changes in Dementias Jacquelynn N. Copeland and H. Randall Griffith
With the number of older adults in the United States projected to grow larger than ever over the next few decades, prevalence of dementia is also expected to rise. Therefore, early detection of neurodegenerative disease processes is a growing concern. In addition, more accurate diagnosis and treatment planning are imperative. Neuroimaging is a term describing different methods utilized to “visualize” changes in the brain. With the computer revolution over the past several decades, neuroimaging has become an important means of detecting and diagnosing neurological diseases, including those that cause dementias. While all neuroimaging techniques help us to “see” some aspect of the brain, each technique provides distinct information about the brain, its function, and its dysfunction. Generally speaking, neuroimaging can be divided into two broad categories, structural and functional imaging. Structural imaging allows for the anatomy of the brain to be visualized, while functional imaging provides a means of seeing changes in blood flow, metabolism, or chemistry of the brain; both structural and functional neuroimaging have their place in research as well as clinical practice. Some types of neuroimaging that are used for dementia include structural
202
Dementia
imaging techniques such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), computed tomography (CT), and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), along with functional imaging techniques including functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), and magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS).These techniques help identify abnormalities that may aid clinicians in making accurate diagnoses, prescribing the appropriate treatment, examining treatment response, and monitoring brain changes over time. The most common neuroimaging techniques used initially when a patient presents with cognitive impairment or dementia like symptoms are a CT scan or an MRI scan, which both show brain structure. CT is less expensive and used primarily to rule out any potentially reversible causes of dementia such as brain tumor, stroke, bleeding, or normal-pressure hydrocephalus, a condition where excess fluid gradually builds up in the brain, resulting in dementia-like symptoms. In addition, CT can also reveal abnormal atrophy, or deterioration of brain volume, in general, or in particular regions of the brain (Petrella, Coleman, and Doraiswamy 2003; Scheltens et al. 2002). For instance, the medial temporal lobe and hippocampus, integral for learning and memory, along with other temporal and parietal regions and areas of the frontal lobe may be of particular importance to image for a patient with suspected dementia. A standard structural MRI is also sensitive to these conditions; in addition, an MRI can display different aspects regarding the makeup of the brain, such as the grey and white matter, and has the ability to detect other abnormalities such as white-matter hyperintensities, which show up on MRI as ultra-white patches, or lacunar infarcts, small areas of cell death caused by occlusion of small blood vessels in deeper parts of the brain, which both may contribute to a presentation of vascular dementia (Small et al. 2008). Although CT and MRI are commonly used for dementia workups, they are particularly limiting when no structural changes are observable, like in early stages of dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a stage of cognitive difficulties between normal aging and dementia. In these cases, the neuropathological disease process, or the changes in the brain related to the course of the disease, may not have yet affected the overall structure of brain tissue; however, cognitive difficulties and symptoms may be present due to functional changes in brain tissue chemistry or metabolism that have yet to affect the structure of the brain that can be seen on MRI or CT. Thus, functional imaging can provide valuable information about biological and chemical changes occurring in the brain and can support clinicians in early detection and diagnosis of dementia when structural changes are
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
203
absent. Furthermore, even when structural imaging reveals atrophy or other findings suggestive of dementia, functional imaging gives the doctors a way of understanding how the structural changes have affected the brain’s working. However, it is important to remember that neuroimaging is only one piece of information that clinicians use when conducting a dementia workup, making a diagnosis, and providing treatment. MAGNETIC RESONANCE SPECTROSCOPY Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) is a functional imaging technique that provides information regarding different metabolites, or molecules that play important roles in the functioning of brain cells. MRS works by the same physical principles as does a standard MRI, that being the use of a strong magnetic field and radio-frequency signals. The primary difference between MRI and MRS is that in MRS scans the data obtained by the MR scanner is interpreted based upon the chemical composition of the area of the body being imaged, while in MRI the radio-frequency signal is reconstructed into an image. MRS works based upon the type of molecule that is being measured; of which several molecules can be “visualized,” including those containing the ions hydrogen (1H), phosphorus (31P), carbon (13C), and fluorine (18F). The most common form of MRS measures brain chemicals based upon the presence and number of the hydrogen ions in different molecules (Minati, Grisali, and Bruzzone 2007), specifically referred to as proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H MRS) to define use of the hydrogen nucleus in contrast to other forms of MRS. Research supports the use of 1H MRS with suspected dementia cases because of its accessibility, sensitivity to detection by an MR signal, and good spatial resolution (Jones and Waldman 2004; Ross and Bluml 2001). Before conducting an MRS scan, the clinician chooses a region of interest; these regions are usually areas of the brain where metabolic changes often occur, or in some instances can be a whole slice of the brain, similar to what one snapshot from an MRI scan would show. The MRS scan then measures the concentrations of measurable brain chemicals in this region by sending in radio-frequency (RF) signals to vibrate the ions, which then respond back with their own identifiable RF, based upon the quantity of hydrogen ions in the molecules in that region. Instead of being constructed into an image as in an MRI, the data received from the scanner is plotted out into a fourier transform graph. This graph displays the relative concentrations, based on the RF signal intensity of the molecule, where higher signal intensity (peaks on the graph) indicates higher relative concentration of the brain chemicals, known as metabolites
204
Dementia
as they are the by-products of brain metabolism. Of note, concentrations are most often expressed as a ratio rather than absolute concentrations due to quantification and measurement difficulties. In addition, metabolites have different spectral patterns, such that some may have one peak, two peaks, or multiple peaks. The parameters of the the MRI RF scanning sequence, such as echo time (TE), also result in slightly different outputs, while increasing the magnetic strength of MRI scanner, such as doubling the magnet strength from 1.5 Tesla (or 30,000 times the strength of the Earth’s magnetic field) to 3 Tesla (or 60,000 times the strength of the earth’s magnetic field), helps separate the peaks and improves the ability to distinguish among metabolites. Common metabolites measured in 1H MRS include choline (Cho), creatine (Cr), glutamate-glutamine (Glx), myo-Inositol (mI), scyllo-Inositol (sI), and N-acetyleaspartate (NAA). Each metabolite is thought to represent a metabolic process (or processes) occurring in the brain, such as those associated with integrity of brain cells or breakdown of brain cell tissue. In the brain, most of these metabolites can be compared with the concentration of Cr, which is usually present at constant levels in all living tissue. Using these ratios, it is possible to establish levels of normal and abnormal concentration of metabolites in certain brain areas, such that abnormally low or high MRS ratios may indicate presence of brain diseases, such as Alzheimer ’s disease. Thus, MRS can measure biochemical information of a chosen area of the brain of a patient with suspected dementia, which can be compared directly to the typical metabolite ratios found in a sameage adult with no cognitive complaints or brain abnormalities. Abnormal metabolite ratios can serve as additional evidence supporting a specific diagnosis along with information collected from patient medical history, cognitive functioning, and structural imaging. Furthermore, MRS can be repeated over time and can be valuable to measure progression of the disease as well as response to potential medication treatments. Advantages of MRS The data provided by MRS is relatively unique and the technique is generally safe and readily available. Although other functional imaging techniques such as PET measure metabolic processes in the brain (such as use of glucose), MRS is the only technique that provides data concerning multiple metabolites within the same scan. Furthermore, MRS is particularly advantageous as it can be obtained within specified regions of the brain and is particularly useful when a specific area of the brain is commonly targeted by neuropathological disease processes, such as the
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
205
posterior areas of the brain in Alzheimer ’s disease. MRS is noninvasive and does not require injection of radioactive materials or any surgical procedures. In many instances this type of data can be obtained along with a clinical MRI scan and does not add unreasonable time burdens on the patient. Thus, information regarding the brain’s structure and function can be collected in one session and used together to evaluate disease. MRS can also be conducted on many of the clinically available MRI scanners, which means it has the potential to be utilized at many hospitals in the United States and worldwide.
Limitations of MRS Most studies using MRS have measured metabolic ratios in specific regions of interest rather than examining whole brain ratios. Other functional imaging techniques may be more beneficial for examining overall brain functioning. Overall, low specificity is a main disadvantage of MRS. That is, information and interpretation of metabolites can be somewhat unclear because some metabolites are very close to one another on the spectrum, especially with lower magnetic strength scanners, such as 1.5 Tesla. Additionally, MRS is very susceptible to distortion by nonbraintissue signals, such as signals arising from bone and air in the sinuses and fat deposits in the scalp. Choosing the area of the brain from which the MRS signal will be measured thus requires avoiding areas close to the skull and scalp, as well as close to the base of the brain, where there is signal contamination from the sinuses. However, future advances in technology will likely address some of these limitations. In addition, MRS is currently considered a new and investigational technique, but has the potential to become more commonly used clinically to aid in dementia workups and diagnosis. In combination with other MRI imaging techniques, MRS has the potential to become an important adjunct in the clinical diagnosis of dementias.
COMPARISON OF MRS TO OTHER NEUROIMAGING TECHNIQUES Comparison of MRS to Structural Techniques MRS and structural MRI use the same hardware, a magnetic resonance scanner, and use similar RF sequences to excite or “pulse” molecules. The critical difference is in how the resulting RF signal from the body tissue being studied is compiled and analyzed. In structural MRI, RF signal
206
Dementia
data is constructed to display images of the brain’s physical structures in high resolution, while MRS measures resonances of RF signal at different frequencies, which are “shifted” from the reference signal (most commonly the H20 signal) depending on the concentration of metabolites. It is this “chemical shift,” measured in parts-per-million (ppm), by which the metabolites are identified and quantified. Unlike MRS, which most often focuses on a region of interest, images created from an MRI typically (but not always) display the whole brain; however, images are created at different angles, called planes, each with a varying number of slices depending on the pulse sequence used during the MRI scan. Various anatomical structures can be identified depending on the particular slices examined and the plane of imaging. Furthermore, structural MRI analysis techniques can be used to measure volumes of particular structures using MRI images. Another type of MR structural imaging, DTI, measures how water diffuses across membranes in the brain to create images of the nerve bundle pathways in the brain, which connect different parts of the brain to one another. DTI is useful for identifying these nerve bundle paths (or “white matter” paths, referred to because the fatty sheath that lines the nerves makes these tissues white compared to the brain cells, or neurons, which appear “grey” to the naked eye) because water diffuses along these whitematter paths in a uniform fashion, as compared to a random diffusion within the grey matter. Unlike MRI, which images both white and grey matter in the brain, DTI focuses solely on white-matter tracts and supplies information regarding their integrity and orientation. Thus, in contrast to MRS, which can reveal abnormal metabolite ratios in a chosen area of the brain, DTI provides information regarding changes in connectivity in the brain, which may indicate less efficiency or diminished ability for communication between various aspects of the brain. However, structural MRI and DTI only provide information about structure, and cannot provide information regarding brain function. MRS Compared to Functional Imaging Techniques In addition to MRS, another functional technique that utilizes MR hardware is called fMRI. Although fMRI and MRS both utilize magnetic fields to supply information regarding the brain’s functioning, fMRI measures changes in signal intensity to represent changes in oxygen content of the blood as it is used by the brain cells. fMRI mostly is obtained while an active cognitive task is completed within the scanner so the difference
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
207
in blood oxygenation concentration can be compared when the brain is engaged in a cognitive task versus when at “rest.” Areas of the brain that show more oxygen use during a task are considered to be “activated.” Like MRS, though, fMRI has the ability to potentially identify functional abnormalities not seen on standard structural imaging. Finally, practical difficulties caused by the cognitive paradigms used during fMRI make it somewhat difficult for patients with dementias to maintain focus during the long periods required to complete a full fMRI scanning session. Recent advances in resting fMRI may obviate the need to use cognitive tasks within the scanner and may be able to provide information regarding changes in the efficiency of brain function. When used clinically, fMRI is potentially less expensive and is less invasive than PET (Small et al. 2008). PET, on the other hand, applies a different type of functional neuroimaging technology that requires intravenous injection of a radioactive tracer and imaging of this tracer in the brain, such that cerebral blood flow, utilization of glucose, or other metabolic processes can be measured. PET imaging can employ various types of radioactive tracers. A commonly used tracer in clinical practice, flourodeoxyglucose (FDG)-PET, identifies regional glucose metabolism in the brain, indirectly representing activity of neurons in the grey matter. However, other types of PET can examine specific aspects of the brain or brain pathology. For instance, PiB-PET has been developed to examine amyloid plaques, one of the neuropathological markers of Alzheimer ’s disease (for a review, see Noble and Scarmeas 2009). Therefore, in contrast to MRS, PET cannot provide information concerning several metabolites at one time as in MRS, but PET can detect significant changes in brain neurochemical activity, including hypometabolism, which can signify underactivity in particular regions of the brain. In addition, the invasive and radioactive nature of injected tracers, along with limited availability of the equipment needed to make these radioisotopes on site, are some current disadvantages of PET imaging. Another functional imaging technology, SPECT, measures blood flow in the brain by detecting an intravenously injected single-photon radioactive tracer. This technology indirectly reveals brain activity by examining blood flow in the brain; it is less expensive and the tracer is more readily available than PET. Nevertheless, similar disadvantages in PET apply to SPECT when comparing MRS; it is more invasive and requires additional technology. In further contrast to MRS, SPECT can only provide information about one biochemical at a time and cannot provide comparative information on metabolism as in MRS.
208
Dementia
RESEARCH WITH 1H MRS IN DEMENTIA AND MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT Main Metabolites of Interest The following metabolites are the most commonly used in dementia research.
N-Acetyleaspartate N-acetyleaspartate (NAA), resonating at 2.02 parts per million (ppm), represents the highest peak on a normal spectrum, due to its high proton metabolic concentration in the brain (Kwock 1998; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001). NAA is found primarily in the main cell body of neurons, the primary signaling cell of the brain, and their axons, the processes of neurons that send information from cell to cell. Furthermore, NAA is present throughout the brain and, due to its location in neurons and axons, found in both grey and white matter. Although the exact function of NAA is unknown, this metabolite serves as a neuronal marker, signifying the density, integrity, and viability of neurons. Measuring NAA in MRS contributes information regarding the number of functioning brain cells; thus, lower concentration ratios of NAA are considered an indication of less neuronal viability and integrity, presumably caused by neuronal death, or loss or injury of axons and dendrites (Birken and Oldendorf 1989). Choline With a peak at 3.22 ppm, choline (Cho) metabolite ratios represent several related neurochemicals in the brain containing choline, such as free Cho, phosphorylcholine, and glycerophosphorylcholine, and to a small extent acetylcholine (Firbank, Harrison, and O’Brien 2002; Kantarci 2007; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001). The greatest amount of choline in the brain is found in the phospholipids of cellular membranes (Kantarci 2007), which is the outmost layer of a cell that serves as a selective barrier; in addition, choline molecules are related to synthesis and turnover of these membranes (Minati, Grisoli, and Bruzzone 2007). Therefore, metabolite ratios in MRS are considered a crude metabolic marker of membrane density, integrity, and turnover. Furthermore, in 1 H MRS the Cho peak can signify inflammation (Mueller, Schuff, and Weiner 2006).
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
209
Creatine Resonating at 3.02 ppm, creatine (Cr) includes both creatine and phosphocreatine. In short, these metabolites represent the energy metabolism or storage of energy in a cell (Minati, Grisoli, and Bruzzone 2007; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001). Since creatine concentrations are relatively stable across individuals, they are considered constant and used to calculate ratio concentrations (i.e., NAA/Cr; Cho/Cr). Myo-Inositol A naturally occurring sugar alcohol found in the brain, the myoinositol (mI) peak resonates at 3.56 ppm. Not occurring in neurons, the main signaling cell of the brain, mI is found in higher concentrations in glial cells, brain cells that support and protect neurons. Thus, the mI peak is presumed a glial marker (Castillo et al. 1998; Downes and Macphee 1990; Garcia-Perez and Burg 1991) An increase in mI ratio concentrations may reflect an increase in the amount of glial cells or increased cell size, which is thought to represent inflammation or gliosis, an accumulation of cells as a response to damaged neurons (Brand, Richter-Landsberg, and Leibfritz 1993; Rosen and Lenkinski 2007; Strange et al. 1994; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001). Scyllo-Inositol In addition to mI, scyllo-inositol, a related metabolite resonating at 3.342 ppm is also presented as a peak on normal MRS spectrum. Less is known about sI’s chemical functions in the brain and how it differs from mI; however, sI is a product of mI metabolism (McLaurin et al. 2000). Glutamate-Glutamine The GLX peak (2.1–2.4 ppm) includes both metabolites: glutamate (Glu) and glutamine (Gln) since the concentration of each substance cannot be separated on lower magnetic strength scanners, such as a 1.5 Tesla. However, improved differentiation of Glu and Gln and their specific concentrations may be revealed using a higher field strength scanner, like a 3 Tesla (Kantarci et al. 2003; Kantarci 2007; Schubert et al. 2004). Glu is the most abundant excitatory neurotransmitter in the brain, thus it is a chemical that is transferred from neuron to another to increase signaling and activity between cells.
210
Dementia
As a precursor of Glu, Gln is believed to have an important neurobiological function related to the detoxification and regulation of Glu (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001). Glu and Gln are involved in neuronal function, metabolism, and plasticity (Antuono et al. 2001); thus, these metabolites are related to efficient transmission of information, energy production, and adaptability and repair of the brain. 1
H MRS Findings in Alzheimer ’s Disease
Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia affecting older adults (McMurtray et al. 2006). The disease is characterized by two particular abnormal findings in the brain called amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. Amyloid plaques are accumulations of beta amyloid protein that occur outside of neurons in the brain, and neurofibrillary tangles refer to twisted and tangled protein fibers that make up a neuron. Both result in death of neurons in the brain, particularly in a pattern with early involvement of areas very important for memory, the medial temporal lobes and hippocampus, but also with further spread into other regions of the brain related to additional cognitive abilities (Braak and Braak 1991). However, examination of the brain at the cellular level is needed for identification of these hallmark brain abnormalities, thus a definitive diagnosis of AD can usually only be made after death (Cummings et al. 1998). Therefore, MRS may help display a specific pattern of abnormal concentrations of metabolites that may represent the cellular changes occurring because of the underlying brain disease in AD, and has the potential to aid in differentiating this pattern from other types of dementias (Kantarci et al. 2008). In studies examining Alzheimer ’s disease, the most common finding is reduction of NAA ratio concentrations (Adalsteinsson et al. 2000; Chantal et al. 2002; Christiansen, Schlosser, and Henriksen 1995; Ernst et al. 1997; Heun et al. 1997; Parnetti et al. 1997; Rose et al. 1999; Schuff et al. 1997; Watanabe et al. 2002). In research where a particular region of interest was examined in patients with AD, decreased NAA/Cr ratios were consistently found in many studies in the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe (Chantal et al. 2002, 2004; Dixon et al. 2002; Jessen et al. 2000; Schuff et al. 1997; Watanabe et al. 2002), along with other specific areas of the temporal lobes (Frederick et al. 1997; Kantarci et al. 2000; Herminghaus et al. 2003; Parnetti et al. 1997) and parietal lobes (Antuono et al. 2001; Griffith, den Hollander, et al. 2007; Hattori et al. 2002; Herminghaus et al. 2003; Kantarci et al. 2000; Kantarci, Xu, et al. 2002; Kantarci, Smith, et al. 2002; Kantarci et al. 2003; Martinez-Bisbal et al. 2004; Rose et al. 1999).
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
211
Furthermore, other regions of the brain including the occipital lobes (Kantarci et al. 2000; Moats et al. 1994; Shonk et al. 1995; Waldman et al. 2002; Watanabe et al. 2002; Weiss et al. 2003) and frontal lobes (Chantal et al. 2002, 2004; Christiansen, Schlosser, and Henriksen 1995; Herminghaus et al. 2003; Parnetti et al. 1997) showed similar findings of depleted NAA/ Cr. Many studies have also displayed NAA reductions in white matter (Catani et al. 2001; Hattori et al. 2002; Herminghaus et al. 2003; Heun et al. 1997; Meyerhoff et al. 1994; Moats et al. 1994); however, a few studies did not find these results (Catani et al. 2002; Watanabe et al. 2002). Additionally, research using MRS techniques to examine larger areas of the brain or whole brain metabolites generally demonstrate a reduction of NAA in AD (Adalsteinsson et al. 2000; Pfefferbaum et al. 1999). Thus, MRS studies in AD seem to support NAA as a marker of both functioning ability of neurons (grey matter) and their axons (white matter). Overall, widespread reductions in NAA are consistent with disease progression and the neurofibrillary tangles present in AD, which may first occur in early stages of the disease in the medial temporal lobes and hippocampus but then spread to areas responsible for vision, sensory, and motor abilities such as the occipital lobe and parietal lobes in later stages of the disease (Braak and Braak 1991). So far, only a few studies have investigated changes in NAA ratios in AD over time. Four studies examined NAA ratios in patients with AD over a one-year period; two of them generally found NAA decreases over time (Adalsteinsson et al. 2000; Kantarci et al. 2007) in individuals with AD, while two other studies did not show declines (Dixon et al. 2002; Jessen et al. 2001). However, in one of the studies with negative results, patients with AD displayed lower ratios of NAA than older controls at both time points (Dixon et al. 2002). Thus, there is a possibility that greater reductions in NAA may indicate later stages of AD and may signify greater amount of brain abnormalities over time due to the disease; nonetheless, more studies are needed to support these results and further examine NAA ratios over longer amounts of time. Abnormalities in the Cho peak have been documented in some studies (Chantal et al. 2002,. 2004; Jessen et al. 2000; Kantarci et al. 2000, 2003; Lazeyras et al. 1998; MacKay et al. 1996; Meyerhoff et al. 1994); however, results have been conflicting. When comparing patients with AD to control participants, some studies find elevations of Cho (Lazeyras et al. 1998; MacKay et al. 1996; Meyerhoff et al. 1994), while others displayed reductions in Cho levels (Chantal et al. 2002, 2004; Jessen et al. 2000; Kantarci et al. 2000, 2003). The meaning behind these inconsistent results is unknown, although there are several hypotheses. Higher levels of Cho
212
Dementia
may be the result of the loss of functioning neurons during AD resulting in increased membrane turnover, or could be related to an increase in the response to help compensate for reduced acetycholine levels seen in AD (Kantarci et al. 2007). There is also the possibility that common medications for AD that are cholinergic, like donapezil, may indirectly result in abnormal Cho levels found in MRS (Griffith et al. 2008; Kantarci et al. 2007). Alternatively, variable results across studies could also be due to differences between methods using 1H MRS, such that variations in length of echo times or particular areas of interest chosen may influence the chance of finding increased or decreased Cho levels (Griffith, Stewart, and den Hollander 2009). Abnormal mI elevations have been found in areas consistent with regions of the brain most affected by the plaques and tangles and neuronal loss in AD, including aspects of the brain such as the temporal-parietal area (Chantal et al. 2002, 2004; Ernst et al. 1997; Parnetti et al. 1996), posterior cingulate gyrus/mesial parietal lobe (Griffith, den Hollander, et al. 2007; Herminghaus et al. 2003; Kantarci et al. 2000, 2003; Kantarci, Xu, et al. 2002; Lazeyras et al. 1998; Martinez-Bisbal et al. 2004; Rose et al. 1999; Waldman and Rai 2003), parietal white matter (Herminghaus et al. 2003; Moats et al. 1994), and occipital lobes (Moats et al. 1994; Shonk et al. 1995; Waldman et al. 2002). Furthermore, frontal lobes (Chantal et al. 2002, 2004; Herminghaus et al. 2003; Parnetti et al. 1997) and subcortical regions (Catani et al. 2001, 2002; Hattori et al. 2002; Heun et al. 1997), which are less commonly affected by the disease, less often display abnormal mI levels. Along with decreased NAA concentrations, increased mI levels are one of the most prominent and consistent findings in 1H MRS among AD patients. Thus, both metabolites seem to serve as separate indicators of the effects of AD on the brain’s functioning with NAA serving as a neuronal marker and mI representing as a glial marker. Cr, sI, and Glx have been less explored in 1H MRS studies with patients who have AD. Cr is consistently used as the denominator of ratios because of its suggested stability even in brain disease (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2001); moreover, AD patients generally show stable Cr levels compared to controls (Ernst et al. 1997; Pfefferbaum et al. 1999; Schuff et al. 1997). Investigation of sI has also been less common; one study revealed sI/Cr elevations in AD patients (Griffith et al. 2006), while another study showed raised concentrations in the normal aging brain (Kaiser et al. 2005). Elevations in sI may occur for the same reasons mI concentrations are increased since they are directly related; however, more research needs to be conducted on sI to support these findings and their possible explanation for elevations. Overall, findings regarding Glx in AD patients using 1H MRS
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
213
are inconsistent. Some studies have shown decreased Glx levels in specific posterior aspects of the brain (Antuono et al. 2001; Hattori et al. 2002) and specific regions in the temporal lobe (Herminghaus et al. 2003) and occipital lobe (Moats et al. 1994). Further research has been conducted to examine the ability for studies using 1H MRS to distinguish patients diagnosed with probable AD from healthy older adults. Several studies have demonstrated that NAA levels can aid in discrimination of normal controls from AD patients (Antuono et al. 2001; Schuff et al. 1997; Shonk et al. 1995). Furthermore, the addition of NAA metabolite levels can improve discrimination of AD patients when information from structural imaging is available (Dixon et al. 2002; Ernst et al. 1997; Kantarci, Xu, et al. 2002; MacKay et al. 1996). Using an NAA/mI ratio, in contrast to other metabolite ratios, seems to best discriminate AD patients from normal controls (Kantarci et al. 2007). Research has also been performed to investigate how cognitive abilities, everyday functioning, and brain abnormalities observed after death relate to 1H MRS findings in AD. The majority of studies have found correlations between 1H MRS and scores on a common mental status screening, the Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE) (Antuono et al. 2001; Dixon et al. 2002; Doraiswamy, Charles, and Krishnan 1998; Ernst et al. 1997; Heun et al. 1997; Jessen et al. 2000, 2001; Parnetti et al. 1997; Rose et al. 1999; Waldman and Rai 2003). The ability to perform everyday activities, like finances, has also been related to metabolite ratios found in 1H MRS (Griffith, Okonkwo, et al. 2007). Finally, NAA and mI findings from patients with probable AD using 1H MRS before and after death have been found to relate to the extent of damage seen in the brain as a result of senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles after death (Kantarci et al. 2008). 1
H MRS Findings in Frontotemporal Dementia
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a broad term that refers to a varied group of clinical syndromes that involve deterioration primarily of the frontal and/or temporal lobes of the brain by processes such as neuronal loss and gliosis. Moreover, FTD is characterized by early personality changes and behavior changes including apathy and disinhibition (Coulthard et al. 2006; Ernst et al. 1997). 1 H MRS studies with FTD patients have revealed metabolic abnormalities in several regions of the brain. Lower NAA/Cr ratios in the primarily diseased regions of the brain, the temporal and frontal lobes, was found in one study. In addition, mI/Cr ratios were increased in the frontal lobe region of interest; however, no metabolic abnormalities were present
214
Dementia
on the 1H MRS of a selected parietal lobe region (Coulthard et al. 2006). Furthermore, other research comparing FTD and AD patients supports differing regional patterns of metabolite abnormalities. That is, areas primarily more susceptible to reduced functioning and neuronal death in each disease may show abnormalities on the metabolite spectrum, such that findings in particular regions, such as the midfrontal grey matter in FTD and the temporoparietal grey matter in AD, may help distinguish between these two types of dementia (Ernst et al. 1997; Mihara et al. 2006). Moreover, clinical and cognitive features, such as severity of dementia using the Clinical Dementia Rating and global mental status measured by MMSE scores, were also associated with metabolic abnormalities in the frontal region and temporoparietal regions in a group of FTD, AD, and healthy controls (Ernst et al. 1997). 1
H MRS Findings in Vascular Dementia
Another common form of dementia, vascular dementia (VaD), pertains to a syndrome related to one or more cerebrovascular mechanisms, those which are related to the blood supply to the brain, that cause neuronal death and deterioration of brain functioning. Types of cerebrovascular problems in VaD include infarcts, which are areas of dead neurons and tissue due to deprivation of blood supply and oxygen, white matter lesions, which refer to areas of dead axons, and consequently, atrophy, or shrinkage of the brain (Wiederkehr et al. 2008). Because of VaD’s various causes, different presentation of symptoms, and high co-occurrence with characteristics of AD, diagnosis is particularly difficult (Holmes et al. 1999; Jones and Waldman 2004). Thus, 1H MRS may be useful clinically to distinguish VaD from other dementias, if a pattern of abnormal metabolite findings could be established in research. Research studies using 1H MRS in VaD have demonstrated widespread metabolite abnormalities. For instance, Herminghaus et al. (2003) explored metabolite ratios in five regions of interest including grey and white matter in the mid-parietal, mid-frontal and temporal gyrus. In contrast to controls, NAA/Cr ratios were reduced in all five regions of interests suggesting global areas of neuronal death. Furthermore, elevations of mI/Cr ratios were found in the parietal grey and white matter, frontal white matter, and the temporal lobe, while Glx ratios were also abnormally elevated in parietal grey matter and temporal lobe (Herminghaus et al. 2003). Results from a study by Kantarci et al. (2004) also displayed lower ratios of NAA/Cr in patients with VaD, although these reductions were found in the posterior cingulate region of the brain. However,
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
215
mI/Cr and Cho/Cr ratios were comparable to healthy controls in this brain area (Kantarci et al. 2004). Together, the pervasive abnormal metabolite findings suggest many brain regions directly related and indirectly related to those involved in vascular pathology, such as infarcts and white matter lesions, are disrupted in VaD. Moreover, the NAA/Cr decreases and mI/Cr elevations suggest that both neuronal dysfunction/death and damage to axons are occurring along with gliosis. Finally, abnormal NAA findings in regions such as the posterior cingulate, far away from areas with vascular pathology, are hypothesized to be indirectly related to the degeneration of neurons in these areas with vascular infarcts and lesions (Kantarci et al. 2004). 1
H MRS Findings in Dementia with Lewy Bodies
Dementia with Lewy Bodies (DLB) is named for the abnormal protein formations, Lewy bodies, which develop in neurons and are found at the cellular level when examining the brain after death. This type of dementia is characterized by worsening of cognitive dysfunction over time, accompanied by fluctuations in alertness and attention. Furthermore, visual hallucinations and motor symptoms like those seen in Parkinson’s disease, such as stiffness and rigidity or loss of the ability to initiate and maintain movement, are also main features of DLB (McKeith et al. 2005). Overall, less research has focused on 1H MRS in DLB; furthermore, findings from these studies are generally inconsistent at this time. Molina et al. (2002) found decreased NAA/Cr, Cho/Cr, and Glx/Cr ratios in patients with DLB compared to controls in one region of white matter, but did not find differences in grey matter in the mid-parietal lobe. Moreover, clinical, cognitive, and motor measures were not related to any of these abnormal metabolite findings (Molina et al. 2002). Studies examining other regions of the brain such as the hippocampus and posterior cingulate gyrus in patients with DLB revealed contradicting results. That is, in one study elevated NAA/Cr ratios were found in the hippocampi of DLB patients in contrast with controls; however, Cho/Cr ratios did not show group differences (Xuan, Ding, and Gong 2008). While another study showed Cho/Cr elevations in the posterior cingulate gyrus but no group differences in NAA/Cr or mi/Cr ratios of DLB patients versus healthy controls (Kantarci et al. 2004). Several explanations could explain variability of 1H MRS findings in patients with DLB. First of all, only a few studies have been conducted, and their methods vary considerably; thus differences in patient characteristics or imaging methods may have an effect on the data obtained.
216
Dementia
However, studies examining damage and loss of neurons in patients with DLB after death have also been inconsistent (Cordato et al. 2000; GomezIsla et al. 1999). Thus, the nature of DLB, itself, and the way it affects the brain may vary, such that it produces different results. 1
H MRS Findings in Parkinson’s Disease Dementia
Parkinson’s disease, primarily categorized as a movement disorder, many times also involves cognitive changes which may later develop into dementia. Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD) is a specific dementia in patients with Parkinson’s disease which involves onset of worsening cognition and functional impairment after at least one year after initial onset of the movement disorder (McKeith et al. 2005). PDD is thought of as a separate dementia with characteristics that can be distinguished from patients with DLB (Benecke 2003) and AD with late motor complications (Dickson 2000). Many studies examining PDD use two comparison groups: a nondemented PD group and a healthy control group. Overall, 1H MRS findings displayed abnormal brain metabolism in PDD patients when compared to both groups. Metabolite levels were first examined in the occipital cortex in PDD patients, where NAA levels were reduced when compared to non-demented PD patients, but not healthy controls (Summerfield et al. 2002). The posterior cingulate gyrus in PDD patients has displayed cellular changes and damage in brains when examined after death (Braak et al. 2004), in addition to abnormal blood flow (Osaki et al. 2005) and neurochemical changes (Brooks and Piccini 2006) in functional imaging studies. Because of these abnormalities, this region of interest was examined in a recent 1H MRS study. Providing further evidence of abnormal chemical changes in the brain, PDD patients displayed reduced NAA/Cr ratios when compared to healthy controls and nondemented PD patients. Furthermore, Glu/Cr ratios were also reduced compared to healthy controls (Griffith et al. 2008). Low NAA levels are thought to signify limited ability for neurons in the cingulate gyrus and occipital lobes to function in PDD, while Glu reductions could possibly relate to other disease processes. This pattern of metabolic abnormalities obtained from 1H MRS in the posterior cingulate gyrus seems to distinguish PDD patients with dementia from those without. In addition, the reduction of NAA/Cr ratios seems to not only discriminate PDD from PD without dementia, but also from normal healthy controls. Furthermore, mental status and cognitive function as measured by the MMSE and Dementia Rating Scale (DRS), also seem to be related to both NAA and Glu levels (Griffith et al. 2008). Ultimately,
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
217
1
H MRS studies examining brain metabolic abnormalities over time in patients with PD who may or may not develop dementia will need to be conducted to understand more about when these brain changes occur in comparison to clinical symptoms. 1
H MRS Findings in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment
Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) is a classification used to diagnose a stage of cognitive difficulties between normal cognition with aging and AD. There are specific criteria that need to be met for a diagnosis of amnestic MCI, which include: (1) complaints of memory loss by the patient, if possible confirmed by others; (2) impairment on memory testing when compared to performance of adults with the same age and education; (3) generally, normal performance on tests in other domains of cognition; and (4) overall, maintained ability to function in activities of daily living (Petersen et al. 2001). Furthermore, individuals diagnosed with MCI have a higher yearly rate of developing probable AD than cognitively normal peers (Ganguli et al. 2004; Petersen et al. 2001). Overall, 1H MRS studies examining patients with amnestic MCI crosssectionally (i.e., at only one period in time) have found consistent metabolic brain abnormalities between the levels seen with normal healthy controls and AD. For instance, one study found that mI/Cr ratios in the posterior cingulate in patients with MCI were significantly increased compared to controls; however, mI/Cr was significantly lower in MCI patients in contrast to patients with AD. Thus, increased ratios of mI in patients with MCI may have reflected early brain changes, such as gliosis, occurring before indication of neuronal damage or loss as usually seen by decrements in NAA (Kantarci et al. 2000). Additional studies have also found mI elevations in patients with MCI compared to controls in the posterior cingulate (Kantarci et al. 2003; Rami et al. 2007), left hippocampus (Franczak et al. 2007), and other areas of the brain including white matter (Catani et al. 2001) and the parietotemporal cortex (Chantal et al. 2004; Rami et al. 2007). However, some studies have not found differences between mI levels in AD and MCI (Catani et al. 2001; Chantal et al. 2004; Garcia Santos et al. 2008; Kantarci et al. 2003). mI elevations are not the only abnormal metabolite findings in crosssectional studies with MCI. Some studies have also found NAA and Cho abnormalities. Cho was found significantly increased in the right frontal cortex and posterior cingulate, but decreased levels of Cho and NAA were discovered in the left medial temporal lobe in patients with MCI (Chantal et al. 2004; Kantarci et al. 2003). More specifically, NAA decreases were
218
Dementia
seen in the right hippocampus of a small sample of MCI patients (Franczak et al. 2007), and NAA/Cr was found to be as equally reduced in the hippocampus of MCI patients and AD patients in another study (Ackl et al. 2005). In contrast, when the posterior cingulate region was examined MCI patients showed abnormal reduction in NAA, but not as severe as declines seen in AD in one study (Ackl et al. 2005; Kantarci et al. 2003), while in another, only NAA abnormalities were seen in AD patients (Ackl et al. 2005). Some studies have found no evidence of NAA changes in MCI (Garcia Santos et al. 2008; Kantarci, Smith, et al. 2002). However, the overall findings from 1H MRS cross-sectional studies generally suggest that abnormal metabolite ratios found in patients with MCI reflects the transitional phase between normal cognitive aging and Alzheimer ’s disease. That is, MCI patients, in general, do not display the normal metabolism seen in healthy older adults but also do not exhibit as severe abnormalities as found in studies with AD. This conclusion is further supported by findings concerning early cellular brain changes seen in AD (Braak and Braak 1991; Markesbery et al. 2006). In contrast with cross-sectional studies, very few 1H MRS studies have investigated metabolic changes over time in patients with MCI. One such study reported NAA/Cr decreases in the posterior cingulate region of the brain in MCI and AD patients one year later. Furthermore, MCI patients who converted to AD and those who did not convert to AD showed a similar rate of decline in NAA, while interestingly, Cho/Cr levels only declined in the nonconverters. Thus, it was hypothesized that some sort of cholinergic mechanism may have been functioning to help compensate for neuronal damage in MCI patients who remained stable. Of note, no mI/Cr abnormalities were found (Kantarci et al. 2007). Quite the opposite results were found by Bartnik Olson et al. (2008), where the posterior cingulate gyrus of MCI patients showed increased mI concentrations but no change in NAA or Cho concentrations approximately 11 months later. These diverging results may be due to different methodological techniques for measuring abnormal metabolite concentrations; nevertheless, they demonstrate the need for more longitudinal 1H MRS studies in MCI patients to better understand the chemical brain changes occurring in this particular stage of cognitive dysfunction. 1 H MRS studies have also explored relationship between abnormal metabolite findings and cognition in MCI patients. Posterior cingulate 1 H MRS ratios in MCI have been found to correlate with different cognitive abilities, such as global cognition and learning (Kantarci, Smith, et al. 2002), and hippocampal NAA/Cr in MCI patients was related to verbal fluency and naming ability (Ackl et al. 2005). Furthermore, the ability to
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
219
predict “conversion,” or worsening of diagnosis from MCI to dementia, from 1H MRS findings has also been investigated. In general, NAA/Cr baseline ratios and reductions show the most promise in predicting conversion from MCI to dementia (Metastasio et al. 2006; Modrego, Fayed, and Pina 2005); nevertheless, more research needs to be conducted using similar diagnostic criteria and methodological techniques to examine MCI participants and the chemical brain changes that occur over time in converters and nonconverters. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS OF MRS One of the most important purposes for conducting research is to eventually apply the findings to individual patients in clinical situations. Although MRS is still considered experimental and investigational at this point in time, research has expanded our knowledge of abnormal metabolite patterns associated with different dementia types. Future studies using 1 H MRS with dementia and MCI patients can confirm results and provide further evidence for the utility of MRS when conducting dementia evaluations in clinical situations. Potentially, consistent metabolite patterns found in research with dementias could possibly serve as a clinical biomarker for the type of neurodegenerative disease in the future. Furthermore, with technological advances such as stronger magnetic field strength of scanners, MRS may become more powerful and more commonly used clinically. Although the prospect of applying MRS clinically is very hopeful, MRS will only be one of many techniques used for information gathering for a dementia evaluation. MRS findings will be considered only an additional tool in an extensive dementia assessment that will supplement other structural and functional imaging results, patient medical history, report of cognitive and functional difficulties, and so on. In addition to its use in initial dementia evaluations and establishing a diagnosis, MRS also has the possibility to aid in monitoring of disease progression in those with a probable or suspected diagnosis of dementia. As more longitudinal research is conducted, more evidence will demonstrate types of changes in metabolites over time. Consistent patterns of abnormalities may develop that will give patients and their families a better idea of the course and progression of the disease and the accompanied brain changes. Since MRS is not intrusive and does not include exposure to radiation, it is easily repeatable over time. In our experience, it is also well tolerated by patients with dementias in the mild and moderate stages, and in those with movement disorders. Moreover, MRS also has further potential to act as a treatment indicator, to help clinicians monitor
220
Dementia
the value and effectiveness of different medications. That is, MRS could help track the patient’s response to certain medications by examining the chemical changes occurring in the brain to see if the disease process may have slowed or stabilized over time. Because of the widespread commercial availability of MRI scanners from which MRS data can potentially be obtained, MRS has the possibility to provide information to clinicians that could greatly benefit many future patients with dementia. More 1H MRS studies in patients with MCI and different dementias will hopefully establish better discrimination between converters and non-converters and between different types of dementia. Thus, future research advances in MRS and dementia studies may provide unique information to facilitate diagnosis and treatment of dementia in the 21st century.
REFERENCES Ackl, N., M. Ising, Y. A. Schreiber, M. Atiya, A. Sonntag, and D. P. Auer. 2005. Hippocampal metabolic abnormalities in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurosci Lett 384 (1–2): 23–28. Adalsteinsson, E., E. V. Sullivan, N. Kleinhans, D. M. Spielman, and A. Pfefferbaum. 2000. Longitudinal decline of the neuronal marker N-acetyl aspartate in Alzheimer ’s disease. Lancet 355 (9216): 1696–1697. Antuono, P. G., J. L. Jones, Y. Wang, and S. J. Li. 2001. Decreased glutamate + glutamine in Alzheimer ’s disease detected in vivo with 1H-MRS at 0.5 T. Neurology 56 (6): 737–742. Bartnik Olson, B. L., B. A. Holshouser, W. Britt III, C. Mueller, W. Baqai, S. Patra, F. Petersen, and W. M. Kirsch. 2008. Longitudinal metabolic and cognitive changes in mild cognitive impairment patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 22 (3): 269–277. Birken, D. L., and W. H. Oldendorf. 1989. N-acetyl-L-aspartic acid: A literature review of a compound prominent in 1H-NMR spectroscopic studies of brain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 13 (1): 23–31. Benecke, R. 2003. Diffuse Lewy body disease—a clinical syndrome or a disease entity? J Neurol 250 (Suppl 1): I39–42. Braak, H., and E. Braak. 1991. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 82 (4): 239–259. Braak, H., E. Ghebremedhin, U. Rub, H. Bratzke, and K. Del Tredici. 2004. Stages in the development of Parkinson’s disease–related pathology. Cell Tissue Res 318 (1): 121–134. Brand, A., C. Richter-Landsberg, and D. Leibfritz. 1993. Multinuclear NMR studies on the energy metabolism of glial and neuronal cells. Dev Neurosci 15 (3–5): 289–298.
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
221
Brooks, D. J., and P. Piccini. 2006. Imaging in Parkinson’s disease: The role of monoamines in behavior. Biol Psychiatry 59 (10): 908–918. Castillo, M., L. Kwock, J. Scatliff, and S. K., Mukherji. 1998. Proton MR spectroscopy in neoplastic and non-neoplastic brain disorders. Magn Reson Imaging Clin N Am 6: 1–20. Catani, M., A. Cherubini, R. Howard, R. Tarducci, G. P. Pelliccioli, M. Piccirilli, G. Gobbi, U. Senin, and P. Mecocci. 2001. (1)H-MR spectroscopy differentiates mild cognitive impairment from normal brain aging. NeuroReport 12 (11): 2315–2317. Catani, M., P. Mecocci, R. Tarducci, R. Howard, G. P. Pelliccioli, E. Mariani, A. Metastasio, C. Benedetti, U. Senin, and A. Cherubini. 2002. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy reveals similar white matter biochemical changes in patients with chronic hypertension and early Alzheimer ’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 50 (10): 1707–1710. Chantal, S., C. M. Braun, R. W. Bouchard, M. Labelle, and Y. Boulanger. 2004. Similar 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopic metabolic pattern in the medial temporal lobes of patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Brain Res 1003 (1–2): 26–35. Chantal, S., M. Labelle, R. W. Bouchard, C. M. Braun, and Y. Boulanger. 2002. Correlation of regional proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic metabolic changes with cognitive deficits in mild Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 59 (6): 955–962. Christiansen, P., A. Schlosser, and O. Henriksen. 1995. Reduced N-acetylaspartate content in the frontal part of the brain in patients with probable Alzheimer ’s disease. Magn Reson Imaging 13 (3): 457–462. Cordato, N. J., G. M. Halliday, A. J. Harding, M. A. Hely, and J. G. Morris. 2000. Regional brain atrophy in progressive supranuclear palsy and Lewy body disease. Ann Neurol 47 (6): 718–728. Coulthard, E., M. Firbank, P. English, J. Welch, D. Birchall, J. O’Brien, and T. D. Griffiths. 2006. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol 253 (7): 861–868. Cummings, J. L., H. V. Vinters, G. M. Cole, and Z. S. Khachaturian. 1998. Alzheimer ’s disease: Etiologies, pathophysiology, cognitive reserve, and treatment opportunities. Neurology 51 (1 Suppl 1): S2–S17; discussion S65–S67. Dickson, D. 2000. Alzheimer-Parkinson disease overlap: neuropathology. In Neurodegenerative dementias, ed. C. Clark and J. Trojanowski, 247–259. New York: McGraw-Hill. Dixon, R. M., K. M. Bradley, M. M. Budge, P. Styles, and A. D. Smith. 2002. Longitudinal quantitative proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the hippocampus in Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain 125 (Pt 10): 2332–2341. Doraiswamy, P. M., H. C. Charles, and K. R. Krishnan. 1998. Prediction of cognitive decline in early Alzheimer ’s disease. Lancet 352 (9141): 1678. Downes, C. P., and C. H. Macphee. 1990. Myo-inositol metabolites as cellular signals. Eur J Biochem 193 (1): 1–18.
222
Dementia
Ernst, T., L. Chang, R. Melchor, and C. M. Mehringer. 1997. Frontotemporal dementia and early Alzheimer disease: differentiation with frontal lobe H-1 MR spectroscopy. Radiology 203 (3): 829–836. Firbank, M. J., R. M. Harrison, and J. T. O’Brien. 2002. A comprehensive review of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies in dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 14 (2): 64–76. Franczak, M., R. W. Prost, P. G. Antuono, L. P. Mark, J. L. Jones, and J. L. Ulmer. 2007. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the hippocampus in patients with mild cognitive impairment: A pilot study. J Comput Assist Tomogr 31 (5): 666–670. Frederick, B. B., A. Satlin, D. A. Yurgelun-Todd, and P. F. Renshaw. 1997. In vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of Alzheimer ’s disease in the parietal and temporal lobes. Biol Psychiatry 42 (2): 147–150. Ganguli, M., H. H. Dodge, C. Shen, and S. T. DeKosky. 2004. Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic type: An epidemiological study. Neurology 63: 115–121. Garcia Santos, J. M., D. Gavrila, C. Antunez, M. J. Tormo, D. Salmeron, R. Carles, J. Jimenez Veiga, et al. 2008. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy performance for detection of dementia, Alzheimer ’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in a community-based survey. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 26 (1): 15–25. Garcia-Perez, A., and M. B. Burg. 1991. Renal medullary organic osmolytes. Physiol Rev. 71 (4): 1081–1115. Gomez-Isla, T., W. B. Growdon, M. McNamara, K. Newell, E. Gomez-Tortosa, E. T. Hedley-Whyte, and B. T. Hyman. 1999. Clinicopathologic correlates in temporal cortex in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 53 (9): 2003–2009. Griffith, H. R., J. A. den Hollander, O. C. Okonkwo, T. O’Brien, R. L. Watts, and D. C. Marson. 2008. Brain N-acetylaspartate is reduced in Parkinson disease with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 22 (1): 54–60. Griffith, H. R., J. A. den Hollander, C. C. Stewart, W. T. Evanochko, S. D. Buchthal, L. E. Harrell, E. Y. Zamrini, J. C. Brockington, and D. C. Marson. 2007. Elevated brain scyllo-inositol concentrations in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. NMR Biomed 20: 709–716. Griffith, H. R., O. C. Okonkwo, J.A. den Hollander, K. Belue, S. Lanza, L.E. Harrell, J.C. Brockington, D. G. Clark, and D. C. Marson. 2007. Brain proton MRS is correlated with financial abilities in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain Imaging and Behavior 1: 23–29. Griffith, H. R., C. C. Stewart, and J. A. den Hollander. 2009. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementias and mild cognitive impairment. Int Rev Neurobiol 84: 105–131. Griffith, H. R., C. C. Stewart, J. A. den Hollander, W. T. Evanochko, S. D. Buchthal, L. E. Harrell, E. Y. Zamrini, J. C. Brockington, and D. C. Marson. 2006. Invivo 3T 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the brain reveals elevated scyllo-inositol in patients with mild Alzheimer ’s disease. Paper read at
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
223
Proceedings of the International Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, at Seattle, WA. Hattori, N., K. Abe, S. Sakoda, and T. Sawada. 2002. Proton MR spectroscopic study at 3 Tesla on glutamate/glutamine in Alzheimer ’s disease. NeuroReport 13 (1): 183–186. Herminghaus, S., L. Frolich, C. Gorriz, U. Pilatus, T. Dierks, H. J. Wittsack, H. Lanfermann, K. Maurer, and F. E. Zanella. 2003. Brain metabolism in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia assessed by in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Psychiatry Res 123 (3): 183–190. Heun, R., S. Schlegel, M. Graf-Morgenstern, J. Tintera, J. Gawehn, and P. Stoeter. 1997. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementia of Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12 (3): 349–358. Holmes, C., N. Cairns, P. Lantos, and A. Mann. 1999. Validity of current clinical criteria for Alzheimer ’s disease, vascular dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Br J Psychiatry 174: 45–50. Jessen, F., W. Block, F. Traber, E. Keller, S. Flacke, R. Lamerichs, H. H. Schild, and R. Heun. 2001. Decrease of N-acetylaspartate in the MTL correlates with cognitive decline of AD patients. Neurology 57 (5): 930–932. Jessen, F., W. Block, F. Traber, E. Keller, S. Flacke, A. Papassotiropoulos, R. Lamerichs, R. Heun, and H. H. Schild. 2000. Proton MR spectroscopy detects a relative decrease of N-acetylaspartate in the medial temporal lobe of patients with AD. Neurology 55 (5): 684–688. Jones, R. S., and A. D. Waldman. 2004. 1H-MRS evaluation of metabolism in Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia. Neurol Res 26 (5): 488–495. Kaiser, L. G., N. Schuff, N. Cashdollar, and M. W. Weiner. 2005. Scyllo-inositol in normal aging human brain: 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy study at 4 Tesla. NMR Biomed 18 (1): 51–55. Kantarci, K. 2007. 1H Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementia. Br J Radiol 80: S146–S152. Kantarci, K., C. R. Jack Jr., Y. C. Xu, N. G. Campeau, P. C. O’Brien, G. E. Smith, R. J. Ivnik, et al. 2000. Regional metabolic patterns in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease: A 1H MRS study. Neurology 55 (2): 210–217. Kantarci, K., D. S. Knopman, D. W. Dickson, J. E. Parisi, J. L. Whitwell, S. D. Weigand, K. A. Josephs, B. F. Boeve, R. C. Petersen, and C. R. Jack Jr. 2008. Alzheimer disease: Postmortem neuropathologic correlates of antemortem 1H MR spectroscopy metabolite measurements. Radiology 248 (1): 210–220. Kantarci, K., R. C. Petersen, B. F. Boeve, D. S. Knopman, D. F. Tang-Wai, P. C. O’Brien, S. D. Weigand, et al. 2004. 1H MR spectroscopy in common dementias. Neurology 63 (8): 1393–1398. Kantarci, K., G. Reynolds, R. C. Petersen, B. F. Boeve, D. S. Knopman, S. D. Edland, G. E. Smith, R. J. Ivnik, E. G. Tangalos, and C. R. Jack Jr. 2003. Proton MR spectroscopy in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: comparison of 1.5 and 3 T. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 24 (5): 843–849.
224
Dementia
Kantarci, K., G. E. Smith, R. J. Ivnik, R. C. Petersen, B. F. Boeve, D. S. Knopman, E. G. Tangalos, and C. R. Jack Jr. 2002. 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy, cognitive function, and apolipoprotein E genotype in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 8 (7): 934–942. Kantarci, K., S. D. Weigand, R. C. Petersen, B. F. Boeve, D. S. Knopman, J. Gunter, D. Reyes, et al. 2007. Longitudinal 1H MRS changes in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 28 (9): 1330–1339. Kantarci, K., Y. Xu, M. M. Shiung, P. C. O’Brien, R. H. Cha, G. E. Smith, R. J. Ivnik, et al. 2002. Comparative diagnostic utility of different MR modalities in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 14 (4): 198–207. Kwock, L. 1998. Localized MR spectroscopy: Basic principles. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 8 (4): 713–731. Lazeyras, F., H. C. Charles, L. A. Tupler, R. Erickson, O. B. Boyko, and K. R. Krishnan. 1998. Metabolic brain mapping in Alzheimer ’s disease using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Psychiatry Res 82 (2): 95–106. MacKay, S., F. Ezekiel, V. Di Sclafani, D. J. Meyerhoff, J. Gerson, D. Norman, G. Fein, and M. W. Weiner. 1996. Alzheimer disease and subcortical ischemic vascular dementia: Evaluation by combining MR imaging segmentation and H-1 MR spectroscopic imaging. Radiology 198 (2): 537–545. Markesbery, W. R., F. A. Schmitt, R. J. Kryscio, D. G. Davis, C. D. Smith, and D. R. Wekstein. 2006. Neuropathologic substrate of mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 63 (1): 38–46. Martinez-Bisbal, M. C., E. Arana, L. Marti-Bonmati, E. Molla, and B. Celda. 2004. Cognitive impairment: Classification by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Eur J Neurol 11 (3): 187–193. McKeith, I. G., D. W. Dickson, J. Lowe, M. Emre, J. T. O’Brien, H. Feldman, J. Cummings, et al. 2005. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 65 (12): 1863–1872. McLaurin, J., R. Golomb, A. Jurewicz, J. P. Antel, and P. E. Fraser. 2000. Inositol stereoisomers stabilize an oligomeric aggregate of Alzheimer amyloid beta peptide and inhibit abeta -induced toxicity. J Biol Chem 275 (24): 18495–18502. McMurtray, A., D. G. Clark, D. Christine, and M. F. Mendez. 2006. Early-onset dementia: Frequency and causes compared to late-onset dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 21 (2): 59–64. Metastasio, A., P. Rinaldi, R. Tarducci, E. Mariani, F. T. Feliziani, A. Cherubini, G. P. Pelliccioli, G. Gobbi, U. Senin, and P. Mecocci. 2006. Conversion of MCI to dementia: Role of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neurobiol Aging 27 (7): 926–932. Meyerhoff, D. J., S. MacKay, J. M. Constans, D. Norman, C. Van Dyke, G. Fein, and M. W. Weiner. 1994. Axonal injury and membrane alterations in Alzheimer ’s disease suggested by in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Ann Neurol 36 (1): 40–47.
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
225
Mihara, M., N. Hattori, K. Abe, S. Sakoda, and T. Sawada. 2006. Magnetic resonance spectroscopic study of Alzheimer ’s disease and frontotemporal dementia/ Pick complex. NeuroReport 17 (4): 413–416. Minati, L., M. Grisoli, and M. G. Bruzzone. 2007. MR spectroscopy, functional MRI, and diffusion-tensor imaging in the aging brain: A conceptual review. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 20 (1): 3–21. Moats, R. A., T. Ernst, T. K. Shonk, and B. D. Ross. 1994. Abnormal cerebral metabolite concentrations in patients with probable Alzheimer disease. Magn Reson Med 32 (1): 110–115. Modrego, P. J., N. Fayed, and M. A. Pina. 2005. Conversion from mild cognitive impairment to probable Alzheimer ’s disease predicted by brain magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Am J Psychiatry 162 (4): 667–675. Molina, J. A., J. M. Garcia-Segura, J. Benito-Leon, C. Gomez-Escalonilla, T. del Ser, V. Martinez, and J. Viano. 2002. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementia with Lewy bodies. Eur Neurol 48 (3): 158–163. Mueller, S. G., N. Schuff, and M. W. Weiner. 2006. Evaluation of treatment effects in Alzheimer ’s and other neurodegenerative diseases by MRI and MRS. NMR Biomed 19 (6): 655–668. Noble, J. M. and N. Scarmeas. 2009. Application of pet imaging to diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int Rev Neurobiol. 84: 133–149. Osaki, Y., Y. Morita, M. Fukumoto, N. Akagi, S. Yoshida, and Y. Doi. 2005. Threedimensional stereotactic surface projection SPECT analysis in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Mov Disord 20 (8): 999–1005. Parnetti, L., D. T. Lowenthal, O. Presciutti, G. P. Pelliccioli, R. Palumbo, G. Gobbi, P. Chiarini, B. Palumbo, R. Tarducci, and U. Senin. 1996. 1H-MRS, MRI-based hippocampal volumetry, and 99mTc-HMPAO-SPECT in normal aging, ageassociated memory impairment, and probable Alzheimer ’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 44 (2): 133–138. Parnetti, L., R. Tarducci, O. Presciutti, D. T. Lowenthal, M. Pippi, B. Palumbo, G. Gobbi, G. P. Pelliccioli, and U. Senin. 1997. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy can differentiate Alzheimer ’s disease from normal aging. Mech Ageing Dev 97 (1): 9–14. Petersen, R. C., R. Doody, A. Kurz, R. C. Mohs, J. C. Morris, P. V. Rabins, K. Ritchie, M. Rossor, L. Thal, and B. Winblad. 2001. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 58 (12): 1985–1992. Petrella, J. R., R. E. Coleman, and P. M. Doraiswamy. 2003. Neuroimaging and early diagnosis of Alzheimer disease: A look to the future. Radiology 226 (2): 315–336. Pfefferbaum, A., E. Adalsteinsson, D. Spielman, E. V. Sullivan, and K. O. Lim. 1999. In vivo brain concentrations of N-acetyl compounds, creatine, and choline in Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56 (2): 185–192. Rami, L., B. Gomez-Anson, B. Bosch, R. Sanchez-Valle, G. C. Monte, A. Villar, and J. L. Molinuevo. 2007. Cortical brain metabolism as measured by proton
226
Dementia
spectroscopy is related to memory performance in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 24 (4): 274–279. Rose, S. E., G. I. de Zubicaray, D. Wang, G. J. Galloway, J. B. Chalk, S. C. Eagle, J. Semple, and D. M. Doddrell. 1999. A 1H MRS study of probable Alzheimer ’s disease and normal aging: implications for longitudinal monitoring of dementia progression. Magn Reson Imaging 17 (2): 291–299. Rosen, Y., and R. E. Lenkinski. 2007. Recent advances in magnetic resonance neurospectroscopy. Neurotherapeutics 4 (3): 330–345. Ross, B., and S. Bluml. 2001. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the human brain. The Anatomical Record 265 (2): 54–84. Scheltens, P., N. Fox, F. Barkhof, and C. De Carli. 2002. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in the practical assessment of dementia: Beyond exclusion. Lancet Neurol 1 (1): 13–21. Schubert, F., J. Gallinat, F. Seifert, and H. Rinneberg. 2004. Glutamate concentrations in human brain using single voxel proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 3 Tesla. NeuroImage 21 (4): 1762–1771. Schuff, N., D. Amend, F. Ezekiel, S. K. Steinman, J. Tanabe, D. Norman, W. Jagust, et al. 1997. Changes of hippocampal N-acetyl aspartate and volume in Alzheimer ’s disease. A proton MR spectroscopic imaging and MRI study. Neurology 49 (6): 1513–1521. Shonk, T. K., R. A. Moats, P. Gifford, T. Michaelis, J. C. Mandigo, J. Izumi, and B. D. Ross. 1995. Probable Alzheimer disease: Diagnosis with proton MR spectroscopy. Radiology 195 (1): 65–72. Small, G. W., S. Y. Bookheimer, P. M. Thompson, G. M. Cole, S. C. Huang, V. Kepe, and J. R. Barrio. 2008. Current and future uses of neuroimaging for cognitively impaired patients. Lancet Neurol 7 (2): 161–172. Strange, K., F. Emma, A. Paredes, and R. Morrison. 1994. Osmoregulatory changes in myo-inositol content and Na+/myo-inositol cotransport in rat cortical astrocytes. Glia 12 (1): 35–43. Summerfield, C., B. Gomez-Anson, E. Tolosa, J. M. Mercader, M. J. Marti, P. Pastor, and C. Junque. 2002. Dementia in Parkinson disease: A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Arch Neurol 59 (9): 1415–1420. Valenzuela, M. J., and P. Sachdev. 2001. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in AD. Neurology 56 (5): 592–598. Waldman, A. D., and G. S. Rai. 2003. The relationship between cognitive impairment and in vivo metabolite ratios in patients with clinical Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia: A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neuroradiology 45 (8): 507–512. Waldman, A. D., G. S. Rai, J. R. McConnell, M. Chaudry, and D. Grant. 2002. Clinical brain proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy for management of Alzheimer ’s and sub-cortical ischemic vascular dementia in older people. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 35 (2): 137–142.
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy
227
Watanabe, T., I. Akiguchi, H. Yagi, K. Onishi, T. Kawasaki, A. Shiino, and T. Inubushi. 2002. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy and white matter hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 977: 423–429. Weiss, U., R. Bacher, H. Vonbank, G. Kemmler, A. Lingg, and J. Marksteiner. 2003. Cognitive impairment: Assessment with brain magnetic resonance imaging and proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Clin Psychiatry 64 (3): 235–242. Wiederkehr, S., M. Simard, C. Fortin, and R. van Reekum. 2008. Comparability of the clinical diagnostic criteria for vascular dementia: A critical review. Part I. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 20 (2): 150–161. Xuan, X., M. Ding, and X. Gong. 2008. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy detects a relative decrease of N-acetylaspartate in the hippocampus of patients with dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neuroimaging 18 (2): 137–141.
About the Contributors
PAUL M. BUTLER, MTS, is an MD-PhD candidate at Boston University School of Medicine. His research interests and publications include topics at the interstices of evolution, medicine, and the humanities. JACQUELYNN N. COPELAND is a graduate student in the University of Alabama at Birmingham’s Medical/Clinical Psychology doctoral program. She received her Bachelor of Science degree in psychology and graduated summa cum laude from the University of Florida in 2006. Her main area of interest is geriatric neuropsychology, with particular focus on dementia and aging. Dr. PETER ENGEL is Geriatric Internist and currently a staff physician in the Geriatric Research, Education and Clinical Center of the VA Boston Healthcare System. Dr. Engel has appointments as Lecturer on Medicine at Harvard Medical School and Adjunct Instructor in Medicine, Boston University. He has a long-standing interest in dementia and degenerative brain disorders of late life. Previously, he was an Associate Professor of Medicine, Albany Medical College, Director of the Memory Clinic at the Albany VA, and co-director of the Partners in Dementia Care Project of the Upstate New York VA Healthcare System. Dr. Engel moved to Boston in 2009. MARIANA KNEESE FLAKS graduated from the Pontific Catholic University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil, with a bachelor ’s degree in psychology and clinical psychology license in 2000. From 2002 to 2004, she attended a hospital psychology specialization on neuropsychology and personality evaluation at the Psychiatry Institute of the Faculty of Medicine of the
230
About the Contributors
University of São Paulo. Since 2003, she has dedicated herself to scientific research in the field of cognitive effects of aging and the differential diagnosis to detect, at the very beginning, cases that are turning into mild cognitive impairment or dementia. She pursued her doctoral degree in science at the same institution between 2004 and 2008, focusing on validation and diagnosis properties of cognitive screening tests for attention and memory. In 2009 she initiated her postdoctoral studies on neuropsychological factors associated with resilience and vulnerability to post-traumatic stress disorder at the Federal University of São Paulo. LAURA FRATIGLIONI is the director of the Aging Research Center (ARC) and currently employed as a professor at the Karolinska Institutet. She is a medical doctor, specialized in both neurology and epidemiology. She has scientific, clinical, and pedagogic commitments. Under her supervision, 11 PhD students and two postdocs have completed their studies since 1996. She is currently supervising four PhD students. She regularly serves as a reviewer for various clinical and epidemiological journals. Since 1996, as principal investigator, she has regularly received grants from several of the major research councils in Sweden. She has been awarded the Luigi Amaducci Award by the Italian Neurological Association and has been recognized by the Swedish Society of Medicine. She is the scientific coordinator of the Kungsholmen Project on Aging and Dementia, co-investigator for the project “Harmony: A Twin Study on Dementia,” and the principal investigator for the SNAC-Kungsholmen population study. Her scientific production has led to 161 original publications, 31 review articles in peerreviewed journals, 17 chapters in edited volumes, and eight reports. H. RANDALL GRIFFITH, PhD, is a clinical neuropsychologist in a private practice in Birmingham, Alabama. He received his PhD in psychology from Rosalind Franklin University/Chicago Medical School and completed a postdoctoral fellowship in the University of Alabama (UAB) Department of Neurology where he worked for several years with the UAB Alzheimer ’s Center. His research interests include using neuroimaging to better understand changes in cognition and changes in everyday activities of persons with neurodegenerative dementias. HANS-HELMUT KÖNIG, MD, MPH, is Professor of Health Services Research and Health Economics, and co-chair of the Department of Medical Sociology and Health Economics at the University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf. Before joining the faculty of the University of Hamburg in 2010, he was Professor of Health Economics at the University
About the Contributors
231
of Leipzig. Hans-Helmut König studied medicine at the Universities of Tübingen, London and Oxford, received his doctoral degree from the University of Tübingen in 1993 and a master ’s degree in public health from Yale University in 1995. His main research fields are cost-of-illness studies, and empirical and model-based cost-effectiveness analyses, as well as the measurement of preferences for health and health care, with a special focus on mental health care. HANNA LEICHT is a research associate at the Department of Medical Sociology and Health Economics at the University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf. She completed a BA in philosophy, politics and economics at Oxford University in 2000 and graduated from the University of Potsdam with a diploma in psychology in 2006. She has worked as a research assistant at the University of Leipzig at the Department of Psychiatry, studying insight into illness in Alzheimer ’s disease patients for a dissertation on this subject and at the Health Economics Research Unit. Her publications cover both issues from her dissertation work and topics in cost-of-illness analysis. MELANIE LUPPA is a research fellow at the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig (Public Health Research Unit). She holds a degree in medical sciences and is about to complete her graduate diploma of psychotherapy (CBT). She studied psychology at the University of Leipzig, where she graduated in 1998. Her primary expertise lies in the field of epidemiology and health economics of mental disorders. Dr. LAURA E. MIDDLETON’s work is motivated by the goal of decreasing the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in old age. Her research has focused on the identification of modifiable lifestyle risk factors for dementia. She is particularly interested in empowering people to decrease their own risk of cognitive impairment outside of the health care system. Dr. Middleton’s PhD (Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada) and postdoctoral fellowship (University of California, San Francisco) examined the relationship between physical activity and cognitive change in old age. It appears that physical activity not only decreases the risk of dementia but also increases the chance of improved cognition in old age. One of her recent studies indicated that being physically active in teenage years reduced the likelihood of cognitive impairment in old age. She is currently conducting studies evaluating the relationship between daily activity (exercise, chores and other movement) and cognition. In addition, she is evaluating how rehabilitation programs might be able to improve
232
About the Contributors
cognitive and physical outcomes in patients who have mild cognitive impairment or who have suffered a ministroke. FERNANDA SPEGGIORIN PEREIRA graduated with a bachelor ’s degree in psychology from the University of Santa Catarina, Santa Catarina, Brazil, in 2002. She has been a neuropsychology specialist since 2004, and in March 2010 she completed her doctoral degree in psychiatry. Her studies, under the supervision of Dr. Orestes Forlenza and Dr. Mônica Yassuda at the Laboratory of Neuroscience at the University of São Paulo, focused on executive functions and functionality in the context of normal and pathological aging. She was then particularly interested in the relationship of executive dysfunction and instrumental activities of daily living. She teaches and conducts research on topics related to cross-cultural validation of neuropsychological instruments, cognitive and functional assessment and rehabilitation of the elderly. CHENGXUAN QIU, MD, PhD, a research scientist, received his medical degree from Shandong Medical University (China, 1980–1985), master ’s degree in medical epidemiology from Tianjin Medical University (China, 1987–1990), and doctoral degrees (PhD) in epidemiology and biostatistics from Tongji Medical University (China, 1996–1999) and in geriatric epidemiology from Karolinska Institutet (Sweden, 2001–2004). He completed research training as a post-doctoral fellow and visiting scientist at the National Institute on Aging (NIA)/National Institutes of Health (NIH) (2005–2006, 2008), USA. He is currently employed as a research scientist by Karolinska Institutet. Since 1999 Dr. Qiu has been with Karolinska Institutet focusing on epidemiology of dementia and brain aging. His research is based on several population-based databases, for example, the Kungsholmen Project, the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care (SNAC) in Kungsholmen, and the Swedish Brain Power Initiatives. Dr. Qiu’s research topics include the genetic (e.g., APOE genotype and familial aggregation), environmental (education, occupational exposures, and lifestyle factors), and biological (blood pressure, diabetes, and heart disease) factors and their interactions for dementia, Alzheimer ’s disease, and brain lesions (brain regional atrophy, infarcts, white matter changes, and cerebral microbleeds). Dr. Qiu’s research also involves collaboration with the U.S. NIA/NIH (Project: The Age, Gene/Environment Susceptibility-Reykjavik Study) and the National Institute for Health and Welfare in Helsinki, Finland (Project: The Cardiovascular Risk Factors in Dementia).
About the Contributors
233
WILM QUENTIN is a research fellow at the Department of Health Care Management at Berlin Technical University. He is a medical doctor and completed an MSc in health policy, planning and financing at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine and the London School of Economics in September 2009. He studied medicine and political sciences in Würzburg, Munich, Madrid, Leipzig, and Marburg, where he graduated in 2007. He has worked as a research assistant at the department of Health Economics of the University of Leipzig and published articles on a broad range of topics ranging from tobacco control policies over costs of HIV/ AIDS treatment to cost-of-illness of dementia. STEFFI G. RIEDEL-HELLER is working as a professor for public health at the University of Leipzig. She is a physician, specialized in psychiatry and psychotherapy, and obtained her master of public health degree from Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland. Her scientific interest lies in the interface of public health and psychiatry, especially in the field of epidemiology of mental disorders in old age and health service research. She has profound experience in conducting cohort studies in old age. She is also chief editor of a German scientific journal, Psychiatrische Praxis. Dr. MICHAEL J. VALENZUELA is a Research Fellow in Regenerative Neuroscience at the School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales (UNSW). His background is in psychology, clinical medicine, and neuroscience research. Dr. Valenzuela’s PhD focused on the topic of brain reserve and for this work he was awarded the prestigious Eureka Prize for Medical Research in 2006. Dr. Valenzuela’s current research interests are aimed at understanding the competing forces of brain plasticity and degeneration in the human brain. In particular, he is interested in how we can use the science of neuroplasticity to help prevent dementia in the first place. He has published over 30 scientific papers, gained over $1 million in research funds, and is the author of the best-selling popular science book It’s Never Too Late to Change Your Mind, which details the latest medical thinking about what you can do to avoid dementia (ABC Books, 2009). Dr. ART WALASZEK is a board-certified geriatric psychiatrist and Associate Professor of Psychiatry at the University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health. He received his medical degree from Northwestern University Medical School, completed psychiatry training at the University of Washington, and completed a fellowship in geriatric psychiatry at
234
About the Contributors
Northwestern Memorial Hospital in Chicago. Dr. Walaszek is currently the Director of Psychiatry Residency Training at the University of Wisconsin Hospital and Clinics. He directs the CME activities of the University of Wisconsin Department of Psychiatry and is the chair of the CME Committee of the Wisconsin Psychiatric Association. He is a member of the editorial board of Academic Psychiatry and is on the executive council of the American Association of Directors of Psychiatry Residency Training. The Association for Academic Psychiatry has recognized Dr. Walaszek’s educational contributions with the 2007 AAP/Forest Junior Faculty Career Award. As a member of the Wisconsin Geriatric Psychiatry Initiative, he speaks extensively on geriatric topics in various medical and nonmedical settings across the state. He has coauthored articles and book chapters on late-life emotional and behavioral problems, anxiety disorders in longterm care, and late-life depression. His clinical practice involves caring for primarily older adults with depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, and dementia, and their families. MÔNICA SANCHES YASSUDA graduated from the University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil, with a bachelor ’s degree in clinical psychology in 1990. She later moved to Gainesville, Florida, where she pursued her master ’s and doctoral degrees in developmental psychology. Her studies, under the supervision of Dr. Robin Lea West, focused on metamemory and memory training in the context of normal cognitive aging. She was then particularly interested in investigating the possibility of changing negative beliefs about cognition and aging and developing techniques for memory improvement among healthy seniors. Since 2005 she has been an assistant professor at the University of São Paulo. She teaches and conducts research in topics related to neuropsychological markers of pathological cognitive decline, memory interventions, frailty, and cognition.
About the Series Editor
PATRICK MCNAMARA, PhD, is Associate Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry at Boston University School of Medicine (BUSM) and is Director of the Evolutionary Neurobehavior Laboratory in the Department of Neurology at the BUSM and the VA New England Healthcare System. Upon graduating from the Behavioral Neuroscience Program at Boston University in 1991, he trained at the Aphasia Research Center at the Boston VA Medical Center in neurolinguistics and brain-cognitive correlation techniques. He then began developing an evolutionary approach to problems of brain and behavior and currently is studying the evolution of the frontal lobes, the evolution of the two mammalian sleep states (REM and NREM) and the evolution of religion in human cultures.
Index
A-beta, 184–189 Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes Memory in Diabetes Study (ACCORDMIND), 86 ACTIVE trial, 77, 112 Adenbrooke’s Cognitive ExaminationRevised (ACE-R), 157–158 age, 75; A-beta production and, 187–188; health and, 111–113; sleep and, 183–184, 186, 187 adipocytokines, 88 alcohol consumption, 13 Alzheimer ’s disease (AD), 5–17; decisional capacity and, 126–127; delayed onset of, 76; 1 H MRS findings and, 210–213; incidence, 4; occurrence, 1–5; prevalence, 2; prevention, 17–20, 76–90, 105, 108–109; sleep patterns and, 178, 180, 184–188. See also prevention amnestic, 217–219 amyloid plaques, 210 amyloid precursor protein, 6, 185, 186 anosognosia, 131 antemortem cognitive tests, 101 antidepressants, 137 antihypertensive therapy, 8, 18, 83
anti-inflammatory drugs, 12 antioxidants, 11, 82 antipsychotics, 133–134, 137–138 aphasia, 163 aphorisms, 69 APOE ε4 allele, 6, 8, 124. See also apolipoprotein E testing apolipoprotein E testing, 124, 125 arousal, sleep, 180 artificial hydration and nutrition, 141 assessment tools, 103, 104 (figure), 127–128, 130, 138–139. See also neuroimaging; neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI) score; neuropsychological tests; screening tools, cognitive atherosclerosis, 9 attention assessment, 160–161 atypical antipsychotics, 133–134, 137–138 autonomy, 123, 128, 165. See also decisional capacity basic activities of daily living (BADL), 165, 167 behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD), 133–135, 137
238
Index
Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS), 162 β-amyloid accumulation, 79, 88 Beyond Good and Evil (Nietzsche), 69 Binswanger, Otto, 64 bipolar disorder, 67–68. See also mood disorder blood-brain barrier, 88 body mass index (BMI), 9–10, 87 Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE), 163 Boston Naming Test, 162, 163 brain abnormality, 212, 213 brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), 106 brain injury, 17, 188 brain reserve capacity, 99–101; behavioral perspective, 103–105; cognitive perspective, 101–102; computational perspective, 102–103; neurocentric perspective, 100–101 B12, vitamin, 11 brain volume, 107 brain vulnerability, 189 brain weight, 101 Cache County Study, 4 CADASIL (cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subgcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy), 58, 64–71. See also CARASIL (Maeda’s syndrome) CAMDEX-R (Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly), 154 capacity improvement, 130–131 CARASIL (Maeda’s syndrome), 67. See also CADASIL (cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy)
cardiovascular disease, 9, 18, 87 Cardiovascular Health Study (CHS), 9 caregiver burden, 37, 50–51, 75 cellular mechanisms, 107 Centers for Medicare an Medicaid Services (CMS), 137 CERAD battery (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer ’s Disease), 154 cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA), 189 cerebrovascular problems, 9, 214 Charles Bonnet syndrome, 59, 61 Cho/Cr ratio, 215, 218 cholesterol, 88. See also serum cholesterol choline (Cho), 208, 211–212, 217. See also Cho/Cr ratio cholinergic activity, 184 cholinergic deficiency, 182 cholinesterase inhibitors (ChEI), 114, 132 cigarettes, 12–13 clock drawing test (CDT), 115, 161, 163 cognitive activity, 76–77, 107 cognitive enhancers, 130, 131–133 cognitive impairment, 113–114, 165, 202, 217–219 cognitive lifestyle, 100; beneficial mechanisms of, 105–109; defined, 103; enrichment and, 109–116; implications of, 100–105 cognitive tests, 101, 127–128 cognitive training, 77, 109–111, 115–116; computer based, 112–113; randomized controlled trials and, 111–114, 112 (figure); research challenges and, 114–115 complex mental activity, 15–16 computed tomography (CT), 202 consent, 126, 128, 131, 138–139 Controlled Oral Word Association (COWA), 161
Index cost categories, 42, 45 cost-effectiveness, 132 cost-estimation technique, 40, 45–46, 47, 49 cost-of-illness (COI) study, 35–36; health economics and, 36–37; literature review of, 44–50, 48–49 (table); methodological characteristics, 37–44, 45–46, 46–47 (table); role of neuropsychiatric symptoms and, 50–53, 52 (table), 53 (table); variation and, 38 (table), 38–39, 41 (table), 41–42, 49–50 C-reactive protein (CRP), 12 creatine (Cr), 209, 212. See also Cho/ Cr ratio; Glu/Cr ratio; mI/Cr ratio; NAA/Cr ratio data collection, 39–40 decisional capacity, 125–128 decision making. See decisional capacity dementia, 1, 39, 110 (figure). See also Alzheimer ’s disease (AD); cost-of-illness (COI) study; frontotempral dementia (FTD); Lewy body dementia (LBD); vascular dementia (VaD) Dementia Rating Scale, 216 depression, 17, 59, 62, 106 determinants, 5–17. See also specific determinants Deussen, Paul, 69 diabetes, 8, 84 (table), 86 diagnosis, medical, 57–58, 124–125 diagnostic categories, 58 diet, 10–11, 80–83 diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), 202, 206 Digit Span Forward, 160 Digit Symbol, 160 Direct Assessment of Functional Status-Revised (DAFS-R), 167
239
disease burden, 110 driving, 129 dyslipidemia, 84 (table), 87 education, 14–15, 76, 164 elder abuse, 135–136 electroencephalographic (EEG) desynchronization, 181 electrophysiological studies, 184 end-of-life care, 140–142 enrichment, 105, 106, 107 epidemiological research, 2–4, 18 epilepsy, 66 ethics, medical, 123–125, 142; clinical research and, 138; cognitive enhancer use and, 131–133; decisional capacity and, 125–130; diagnosis challenges and, 124–125; incapacity and, 126, 130–131; long-term care and, 136–138; symptom treatment and, 133–135 etiological factors, 19 (table) executive control assessment interview (EXIT-25), 161–162 executive function assessment, 161–162 executive functioning, 161–162, 166 exercise, 16, 105 EXIT-25. See executive control assessment interview (EXIT-25) extremely low-frequency electromagnetic fields (ELF-EMFs), 17 finances, 128. See also cost-of-illness (COI) study Financial Capacity Instrument, 128 folate, 11 Framingham cohort study, 11 French Three-City Study, 11 Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB), 156–157 frontotempral dementia (FTD), 213–214
240
Index
Fuld Object-Memory Evaluation (FOME), 159 functional assessment, 164–167 functional imaging, 206–207. See also functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI); positron emission tomography (PET); single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), 202, 206–207
Honolulu-Asia Aging Study, 8 Hooper Visual Organization Test, 163 hormone replacement therapy, 16 hospitalization, 134 hydrogen ions (1H), 203 hypertension, 7–8, 83, 84 (table), 85. See also antihypertensive therapy Hypertension in the Very Elderly Trial (HYVET), 85 hypnograms, 179 (figure) hypomania, 59, 62
gene mutations, 6 genetic counseling, 125 genetic risk factors, 6 glucocorticoids, 187–188 glucose-labeled PET studies, 107–108 Glu/Cr ratio, 216 glutamate (Glu), 209. See also Glu/Cr ratio glutamine (Gln), 209–210 Glx ratio, 214 glycemic control, 86–87, 88
identification of patients, 39 immune function, 188 incapacity, 126, 130–131 incidence, of dementia, 1, 3–4, 5 (table) independence, 123, 128, 165 inflammation, 12 informal care, 43–44 inheritance patterns, 66, 68 institutionalization, 134–135, 136–138 instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), 165, 167 insulin, 86, 88 intervention strategies, 18 involuntary hospitalization, 134 Iowa Gambling Task, 161 ischemic episodes, 65
hallucinations, 215 headache, 58–59, 61–62 Health and Retirement Study, 139 health economics, 36–37 Hegel, George Wilhelm Friedrich, 57 high blood pressure, 7–8, 83, 84 (table), 85. See also antihypertensive therapy hippocampus, 185, 210 historical research, 57–58 1 H MRS findings, 203, 204; Alzheimer ’s disease and, 210–213; dementia with Lewy bodies and, 215–216; frontotempral dementia (FTD) and, 213; mild cognitive impairment and, 217–219; Parkinson’s disease and, 216–217; vascular dementia (VaD) and, 214–215 homocysteine, 11–12
Kohlman Evaluation of Living Skills (KELS), 128 language assessment, 162–163 lateral hypothalamus (LH), 181 laterodorsal (LDT) tegmental, 180 learning assessment, 158–160 Lewy body dementia (LBD), 178, 180, 189–190, 215–216 Lifestyle Interventions and Independence for Elders (LIFE) Study, 79 Lifetime of Experience Questionnaire (LEQ), 103, 104 (figure) literature review, cost, 44–50
Index long-term care facilities, 136 long-term depression (LTD), 106 long-term potentiation (LTP), 106, 185, 187 MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T), 127, 138–139 magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 202, 206 magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), 203–205; vs. alternative neuroimaging techniques, 205–207; clinical applications of, 219–220. See also1H MRS mania, 59, 62 medical diagnosis, 57–58 medical ethics. See ethics, medical Mediterranean diet, 11, 81 memory, 106, 107–108, 154–157; learning assessment and, 158–160; sleep disturbance and, 180, 183–184 mentally stimulating activity, 15–16 metabolic syndrome, 85 (table), 87 metabolite, 204, 205, 208–210; abnormalities in, 214–215, 216–218. See also specific metabolites mI/Cr ratio, 213, 214, 215, 217, 218 migraine, 59, 61–62, 67 mild cognitive impairment (MCI), 113–114, 165, 202 Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), 39, 128, 132, 154, 216 mitochondrial myopathyencephalopathy-lactic acidosisstroke syndrome (MELAS), 68 Montreal Cognitive Assessment, 158 mood disorder, 59–60, 62 mortality rate, and dementia, 4–5, 140–141 motor skills, 155 myo-inositol (mI), 209
241
NAA/Cr ratio, 213, 214, 215, 216, 218, 219 N-acetyleaspartate (NAA), 208, 212, 216, 217, 218; ratios, 210–211, 213. See also NAA/Cr ratio naming trails, 162–163 Necker Cube copying, 163 net-cost studies, 42, 44 neuritic plaques, 7–8, 13 neurodegenerative disease, 12, 88. See also Alzheimer ’s disease (AD); dementia; frontotemporoal dementia (FTD); Lewy body dementia (LBD); vascular dementia (VaD) neurofibrillary tangles, 210 neurogenesis, 107 neuroimaging, 7, 12, 13, 16, 108, 151, 201–203. See also magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) neuronal numbers, 101, 102 neuropathological data, 8, 9, 13, 15 neuropeptide (NEP), 185 neuroplasticity, 100, 108–109 neuropsychiatric inventory (NPI) score, 52 (table), 53 (table) neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS), 50–53, 52 (table), 53 (table) neuropsychological tests, 113, 152, 158–167. See also specific tools neurotoxicity, 184–185 new cases, 2 NHS Economic Evaluation Database (NHSEED), 44 Nietzsche, Friedrich, 58; brilliance of, 70–71; diagnosis of, 64–68, 65 (table), 68–69; family history of, 60, 66, 68, 69; medical history of, 58–63; mood disorder and, 59–60, 62; pain of, 70–71 Nietzsche, Karl Ludwig, 60 NOTCH3 gene mutation, 64, 65, 66 nREM sleep, 177, 182, 184, 186
242
Index
nursing home placement, 51 nutrition, 10–11, 141 obesity, 85 (table), 87 OBRA (federal regulations for nursing homes), 137–138 occupational exposure, 17 occurrence, 1, 2–5 ocular disturbance, 60–61 omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs), 11, 82 orexin, 181 palliative care decisions, 141 parahippocmpal gyrus, 185 Parkinson’s disease, 61, 178, 190, 216–217 pedunculopointine (PPT) tegmental, 180, 190 pharmaceuticals, 75, 77, 151; antidepressants, 137; anti-inflammatory, 12; atypical antipsychotics, 133–134, 137–138; cognitive enhancers, 132; cost of, 132; pro-inflammatory, 188; psychotropic, 137 physical activity, 16, 77–79, 105 physical restraints, 137 plaque, 106, 210 positron emission tomography (PET), 202, 207 prevalence, of dementia, 1, 2–3, 3 (figure) prevalence cost, 42 prevention, 17–20, 75–76, 81 (figure), 89 (figure), 89–90, 105, 108–109; activity and, 76–77; diet and, 80–83; physical activity and, 77–79; social engagement and, 79–80; and vascular risk factors, 83–88 pro-inflammatory medication, 188 PS1 mutation, 125
PS2 mutation, 125 psychiatric hospital, 63, 134–135 psychological factors, 14–16, 19 (table), 19–20 psychotropic medications, 137 pyramidal neurons, 101 radio frequency (RF), 203, 205–206 regional variation, 2–3 REM sleep, 177, 181, 182 REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), 178, 180, 181, 189–191 research studies: consent and, 138–140; decisional capacity and, 126–128; hypertension and, 85; physical activity of and, 78–79. See also cognitive training; cost-of-illness (COI) study; data collection; electrophysiological studies; neuroimaging; neuropathological data; specific studies resource use, 36–37, 42–44 Resource Utilisation in Dementia (RUD) Instrument, 40 retinal inflammation, 59 REVEAL (Risk Evaluation and Education for Alzheimer ’s Disease) study, 124, 125 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), 159 Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure copying, 163 risk factors, 75, 80; cognitive lifestyle and, 103–105; genetic, 6; vascular, 7–10, 83–88, 84–85 (table) Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test (RBMT), 159 screening tools, cognitive, 153–158 scyllo-inositol, 209 secreted amyloid precursor protein alpha (sAPP), 185
Index self-neglect, 135 serum cholesterol, 10 serum homocysteine, 11–12 sexual relations, 129 single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), 202, 207 sleep: architecture, 184–187, 190; arousal, 180–183; aging and, 183–184, 186, 187; disturbances, 177–180, 186–187, 191–192; memory and, 183–184; nREM, 177, 182, 184, 186; REM, 177, 181, 182, 186; slow wave, 183–184. See also REM sleep behavioral disorder (RBD) sleep-regulating regions, 181–183, 182 (figure) slow wave sleep (SWS), 183–184 smoking, 12–13 social engagement, 15, 79–80 social justice, 124, 132 sources of information, 40 sphenoid meningioma, 67 statin therapy, 10 statistical data, 2. See also prevalence, of dementia stroke, 8–9, 57–58, 64 structural imaging, 201, 205–206. See also diffusion tensor imaging (DTI); magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) study design, 40 surrogate decision maker, 130–131, 139, 140, 141 survival time, 5 Swedish Kungsholmen Project, 4 symptom treatment, 133–135 Syndrom Kurztest (SKT), 155–156 syphilis, 58, 67 temporal lobes, 210 10/66 Dementia Research Group, 2
243
testamentary capacity, 129–130 Test Your Memory (TYM) instrument, 158 Tg2576, 186 TIAs (transient ischemic attacks), 65, 66 total-cost studies, 42, 44 total homocysteine (tHcy), 11, 12 toxicity, 184–185 traumatic brain injury, 17 treatment, 133–135. See also pharmaceuticals; specific treatments valuation, 36–37, 43–44 vascular dementia (VaD): cost-ofillness (COI) studies and, 39; determinants, 8, 9, 10; 1H MRS findings and, 214–215; incidence, 4; prevalence, 4; prevention of, 76–83 vascular factors, 13–14, 18–19, 19 (figure) vascular risk factors, 7–10, 82–88 ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO), 181 verbal fluency tests (VFT), 157, 161 Visual Reproduction and Logical Memory sub-tests, 159 visuo-spatial ability assessment, 163–164 vitamin B12, 11 vitamin D, 83 voluntarism, 126 voluntary hospitalization, 134 voting, 129 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) battery, 159, 160, 162 Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III), 159 welfare, 123 Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHI-MS), 16 World Heath Organization (WHO), 164
DEMENTIA Volume 2: Science and Biology Patrick McNamara, Editor
Brain, Behavior, and Evolution Patrick McNamara, Series Editor
This page intentionally left blank.
Copyright 2011 ABC-CLIO, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior permission in writing from the publisher. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Dementia / Patrick McNamara, editor. p. cm.—(Brain, behavior, and evolution) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-0-313-38434-9 (hard copy : alk. paper)—ISBN 978-0-313-38435-6 (ebook) 1. Dementia. 2. Alzheimer ’s disease. I. McNamara, Patrick, 1956– II. Series: Brain, behavior, and evolution [DNLM: 1. Dementia. WM 220] RC521.D4524 2011 616.8’3—dc22 2010041082 ISBN 978-0-313-38434-9 EISBN 978-0-313-38435-6 15
14
13
12
11
1
2
3
4
5
This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook. Visit www.abc-clio.com for details. Praeger An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC ABC-CLIO, LLC 130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911 Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911 This book is printed on acid-free paper Manufactured in the United States of America
This page intentionally left blank.
Contents
Series Foreword Preface: Hopeful Trends in Meeting the Challenge of the Dementias Patrick McNamara
vii ix
Volume 2: Science and Biology Chapter 1. The Genetics of Alzheimer ’s Disease Lars Bertram Chapter 2. Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease: A Kinship Theory of Gene Conflict in Aging and Dementia Paul M. Butler and Patrick McNamara Chapter 3. Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction: A Redoubtable Trio in Alzheimer ’s Disease Pathogenesis Sónia C. Correia, Renato X. Santos, Cristina Carvalho, Susana Cardoso, and Paula I. Moreira Chapter 4. Alzheimer ’s Disease: Increased Neurogenesis and Possible Disease Mechanisms Related to Neurogenesis Philippe Taupin Chapter 5. Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances in Dementia Anna Burke
1
25
89
115
135
vi
Chapter 6.
Chapter 7.
Chapter 8.
Chapter 9.
Contents
Dementia in Parkinson Disease: Current Concepts in Neuropathology, Neuroanatomy, and Neurochemistry Raymon Durso
159
Aspects in Neuropsychology: Depression and Dementia Ilan Halperin and Amos D. Korczyn
183
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease Erica Harris
221
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Howard S. Kirshner
Chapter 10. Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies Haruhiko Oda, Yasuji Yamamoto, and Kiyoshi Maeda
237
257
Chapter 11. Sleep Disorders in Dementia Kesha Wilford and Sanford Auerbach
279
Chapter 12. Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease Brooke K. Walter and James B. Leverenz
295
About the Contributors
315
About the Series Editor
321
Index
323
Series Foreword
Beginning in the 1990s, behavioral scientists—that is, people who study mind, brain, and behavior—began to take the theory of evolution seriously. They began to borrow techniques developed by the evolutionary biologists and apply them to problems in mind, brain, and behavior. Now, of course, virtually all behavioral scientists up to that time had claimed to endorse evolutionary theory, but few used it to study the problems they were interested in. All that changed in the 1990s. Since that pivotal decade, breakthroughs in the behavioral and brain sciences have been constant, rapid, and unremitting. The purpose of the Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series of titles published by ABC-CLIO is to bring these new breakthroughs in the behavioral sciences to the attention of the general public. In the past decade, some of these scientific breakthroughs have come to inform the clinical and biomedical disciplines. That means that people suffering from all kinds of diseases and disorders, particularly brain and behavioral disorders, will benefit from these new therapies. That is exciting news indeed, and the general public needs to learn about these breakthrough findings and treatments. A whole new field called evolutionary medicine has begun to transform the way medicine is practiced and has led to new treatments and new approaches to diseases, like the dementias, sleep disorders, psychiatric diseases, and developmental disorders that seemed intractable to previous efforts. The series of books in the Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series seeks both to contribute to this new evolutionary approach to brain and behavior and to bring the insights emerging from the new evolutionary approaches to psychology, medicine, and anthropology to the general public. The Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series was inspired by and brought to fruition with the help of Debora Carvalko at ABC-CLIO. The series editor,
viii
Series Foreword
Dr. Patrick McNamara, is the director of the Evolutionary Neurobehavior Laboratory in the Department of Neurology at Boston University School of Medicine. He has devoted most of his scientific work to development of an evolutionary approach to problems of sleep medicine and to neurodegenerative diseases. Titles in the series will focus on applied and clinical implications of evolutionary approaches to the whole range of brain and behavioral disorders. Contributions are solicited from leading figures in the fields of interest to the series. Each volume will cover the basics, define the terms, and analyze the full range of issues and findings relevant to the clinical disorder or topic that is the focus of the volume. Each volume will demonstrate how the application of evolutionary modes of analysis leads to new insights on causes of disorder and functional breakdowns in brain and behavior relationships. Each volume, furthermore, will be aimed at both popular and professional audiences and will be written in a style appropriate for the general reader, the local and university libraries, and graduate and undergraduate students. The publications that become part of this series will therefore bring the gold discovered by scientists using evolutionary methods to understand brain and behavior to the attention of the general public, and ultimately, it is hoped, to those families and individuals currently suffering from those most intractable of disorders— the brain and behavioral disorders.
Preface: Hopeful Trends in Meeting the Challenge of the Dementias Patrick McNamara
It is estimated that 24.3 million people around the world have dementia and that, with an estimated 4.6 million new cases every year, we can expect about 43 million people and their families to face the challenge of dementia by 2020. There are several forms of dementia, with the most common being Alzheimer ’s disease (40% of cases), vascular dementia with or without Alzheimer features (25%), and dementia with Lewy bodies (25%), the latter being related to the increasingly important form of dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. The annual healthcare costs for Alzheimer ’s disease alone is estimated at about $155 billion in the United States. A substantial portion of these costs is due to behavioral and neuropsychiatric disturbances associated with the dementing process— yet these neuropsychiatric and behavioral problems have only recently become the focus of study and treatment in the biomedical communities. The successes of neuropsychiatric approaches to the dementias is measured in reduced suffering for patients and their families and reduced healthcare costs for the system as a whole. The authors of the chapters in these three volumes, devoted to emerging trends in dementia studies, have virtually all emphasized identification, study, and treatment of behavioral and neuropsychiatric problems of patients and their families. The reason they have done so is the dawning realization in both the biomedical and caregiving communities that targeting behavioral and neuropsychiatric problems of dementia leads to some pretty effective scientific studies of mechanisms and very effective and low-cost treatment programs that act to alleviate both patients’ suffering and caregivers’ burdens.
x
Preface
Although the standard, it has long been established that dementia most commonly occurs in older people, and that primary symptoms are memory impairment (both short- and long-term), deficits in executive functions, and impairments of abstract thinking and judgment. It has now become crystal clear that some of the best and earliest predictors of dementia risk are mood and personality changes, which all too often are misdiagnosed as depression or some other common mood disorder. Family members may express concern to a primary care physician, but these concerns too often get ignored or shunted aside as a standard mood disorder. It is vitally important to take reports of significant behavioral changes seriously as identification of cognitive components of a dementing process—may be a later-occurring symptom than the behavioral changes. Although the three-step diagnostic process (single question about memory, MMSE, neuropsychological testing) has high positive predictive value, it only detects 18% of future dementia cases. It is the behavioral and neuropsychiatric disturbances, along with incipient cognitive changes, that may yield better detection rates for dementia. Tremendous progress has been made in identification of biomarkers for dementia. The use of functional imaging, proteomic, genetic, biochemical and electrophysiological markers, including sleep polysomnographic techniques, has meant that our ability to detect dementia early on has vastly improved. In addition, the new appreciation of the importance of behavioral and psychiatric problems in dementia as well as validated assessment tools to measure these behavioral problems suggests that it is time to deploy all these new techniques to identify those at risk for dementia so as to prevent or to slow onset of the disorder in these individuals. What is needed are large-scale, multisite, comparative studies that can evaluate optimal use and validity of these various techniques for detecting and selecting asymptomatic people at risk for dementia. The recent Leon Thal Symposium 2009 in Las Vegas, Nevada, explored algorithms, biomarkers, and assessment tools for identifying asymptomatic individuals at elevated risk for dementia. The consensus recommendations of symposium participants included: 1. Establishment of a National Database for Longitudinal Studies as a shared research core resource; 2. Launch of a large collaborative study that will compare multiple screening approaches and biomarkers to determine the best method for identifying asymptomatic people at risk;
Preface
xi
3. Initiation of a Global Database that extends the concept of the National Database for Longitudinal Studies for longitudinal studies beyond the United States; and 4. Development of an educational campaign that will promote healthy brain aging. (Khachaturian et al. 2010) These are all laudable recommendations. But behavioral and neuropsychiatric assessment tools must be included in these large multisite studies of at-risk individuals. A perusal of the essays in these volumes (volume 1 focuses on epidemiologic, descriptive, historical, and diagnostic innovations in dementia; volume 2 focuses on biobehavioral mechanisms of dementia; and volume 3 focuses on emerging treatment strategies including treatments for behavioral problems of dementia) leaves one with a sense of hope and confidence that the daunting challenges of the dementias, both for patients and for families, are finally being effectively addressed. REFERENCE Khachaturian, Z. S., D. Barnes, R. Einstein, et al. 2010. Developing a national strategy to prevent dementia: Leon Thal Symposium 2009. Alzheimer’s and Dementia 6 (2): 89–97.
Chapter 1
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease Lars Bertram
Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) is the most common form of age-related dementia and is characterized by progressive and insidious neurodegeneration of the central nervous system eventually leading to a gradual decline of cognitive function and dementia in affected individuals. The key neuropathological features of AD are abundant amounts of neurofibrillary tangles and β-amyloid (Aβ) in the form of senile plaques and blood vessel deposits, both prerequisites for a confirmed diagnosis of AD (Price et al. 1998) Although the knowledge of the molecular mechanisms leading to neuronal cell death still remains incomplete, it is now established that genes play a predominant role in determining predisposition for the disease and its clinical progression. Although largely indistinguishable in their clinical presentation or neuropathology, AD cases show a dichotomy of familial (i.e., rare) versus seemingly nonfamilial (i.e., common) forms (Tanzi 1999). The latter are also frequently described as “sporadic,” although it has become clear that genes also play a major role in determining onset and progression of these latter cases (Gatz et al. 2006; Bertram and Tanzi 2008). Similar to many other common adult-onset disorders, these genetic factors (a.k.a. “susceptibility genes”) are likely to be numerous, displaying intricate patterns of interaction with each other as well as with nongenetic factors, and—unlike classical Mendelian (“simplex”) disorders—exhibit no simple or single mode of inheritance. Therefore, the genetics of these diseases has been labeled as “complex.” AD is a typical example of a genetically complex disease. Early-onset familial AD (EOFAD), often transmitted as an autosomal
2
Dementia
dominant trait with onset ages usually below 65 years of age, is caused by rare, but highly penetrant mutations in at least three genes (APP, PSEN1, PSEN2; see following section). Overall, however, these cases probably represent not more than 5% of all AD cases, and the vast majority of AD is actually of late-onset (LOAD, i.e. usually beyond 65 years), most often not showing any obvious pattern of familial segregation. Despite intensive efforts in characterizing the genetic underpinnings of LOAD over the past three decades, to date genetic variants in only one gene (APOE, encoding apolipoprotein E) have been established to significantly modify the risk and onset age of LOAD across a multitude of independent samples and different ethnicities (Saunders et al. 1993; Strittmatter et al. 1993). Despite the considerable complexities of AD genetics, tremendous progress toward a better understanding of the etiological and pathophysiological mechanisms leading to neurodegeneration has been made. This chapter will outline a brief history of the genetics of AD and discuss its current status and future outlook, in particular focusing on recent findings suggesting the existence of several novel AD genes by means of genomewide association analysis (GWAS). EARLY-ONSET FAMILIAL ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE (EOFAD) GENES Only 5% (or fewer) of all AD cases can be explained by EOFAD (Raux et al. 2005). Despite its rarity, genetic studies of this form of AD are actually facilitated by the availability of large multigenerational pedigrees allowing genetic linkage analysis and subsequent positional cloning, which is usually not possible in LOAD families where fewer relatives survive the family-specific onset age and genetic information from parents is almost always lacking (see below). The search for causative mutations is expected to be vastly facilitated by means of only recently developed massively parallel sequencing technologies enabling to decipher an individual’s whole genome in one experiment at decreasing cost (Tucker, Marra, and Friedman 2009; Lupski et al. 2010). Using conventional genetics analyses, data was reported in 1987 that showed EOFAD linkage to the long arm of chromosome 21 encompassing a region that harbored the gene encoding the amyloid precursor protein (APP; gene: APP), a compelling candidate gene for AD (Tanzi et al. 1987). In 1991 the first APP missense mutation in a family with EOFAD was described (Goate et al. 1991). Since then, over 30 additional ADcausing mutations have been reported in APP which, in total, account for probably not more than one-tenth of all early-onset autosomal dominant
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
3
AD (see Table 1.1; for an up-to-date overview of AD mutations visit the “AD and FTD Mutation Database” at http://www.molgen.ua.ac.be/ ADMutations). Interestingly, most of the APP-mutations occur near the putative γ-secretase site between residues 714 and 717, suggesting that especially the γ-cleavage event of APP and/or its (dys-) regulation are critical for the development of AD. Recently, two additional APP variants were suggested to cause AD via increased levels of the wildtype protein: first, duplications of the APP-containing chromosomal segment causing AD with cerebral amyloid angiopathy (Rovelet-Lecrux et al. 2006), and second, promoter Table 1.1 Summary of Genetic Findings for Early-Onset Autosomal-Dominant Forms of AD
Gene (protein)
Chromosomal location
Number of pathogenic mutations Relevance to AD (affected families)a pathogenesis
APP (β-amyloid precursor protein)
21q21.3
32 (89)
PSEN1 (presenilin 1)
14q24.3
179 (394)
PSEN2 (presenilin 2)
1q31-42
14 (23)
Increase in Aβ production or Aβ42/Aβ40-ratio; mutations in Aβ or close to γ-secretase site; locus duplications Increase in Aβ42/ Aβ40-ratio; mutations throughout molecule; enzymatic role in γ-secretase complex Increase in Aβ42/ Aβ40-ratio; mutations throughout molecule; enzymatic role in γ-secretase complex
Source: Modified and updated based on Bertram and Tanzi 2008. a
(Source: “AD & FTD Mutation Database” [URL: http://www.molgen.ua.ac.be/ADMutations/], current on May 1, 2010).
4
Dementia
mutations leading to a possible increase in mRNA levels (Brouwers et al. 2006; Theuns et al. 2006). Both were found to be segregating with the disease in an autosomal-dominant fashion in several unrelated families. While more data is needed to estimate the overall contribution of these variants to the prevalence of EOFAD, these discoveries are in line with the decades-old observation that AD neuropathology almost invariably develops in patients with trisomy 21 (Down’s syndrome), in which the extra copy of APP leads to increases in the expression of APP and deposition of Aβ (Wisniewski et al. 1985; Rumble et al. 1989). Only one year after the discovery of the first APP mutation, a second AD linkage region—on chromosome 14q24—was reported almost simultaneously by four independent laboratories (Mullan et al. 1992; Schellenberg et al. 1992; St. George-Hyslop et al. 1992; Van Broeckhoven et al. 1992). It took three more years to clone the responsible gene (PSEN1) and identify the first AD-causing mutations (Sherrington et al. 1995). It is now known that PSEN1 encodes a highly conserved polytopic membrane protein, presenilin 1 (PS1), that plays an essential role in mediating intramembranous, γ-secretase processing of APP to generate Aβ from APP (Steiner, Fluhrer, and Haass 2008). Even more than a decade after the original description of PSEN1, there are several new AD-causing mutations reported in this gene every year, currently counting a total of nearly 180 (see Table 1.1). Soon after the discovery of PSEN1 as an AD gene, a second member of the presenilin family of proteins was identified via searching the then-available databases. It displayed significant homology to PSEN1 at the genomic as well as at the protein level (Levy-Lahad et al. 1995; Rogaev et al. 1995), and therefore, this gene was named PSEN2 (protein: PS2). It maps to the long arm of chromosome 1 and mutations in this gene account for the smallest fraction of all EOFAD cases. On average, mutations in PSEN2 also display a later age of onset and slower disease progression than APP or PSEN1 mutations. In conclusion, while the currently known AD-causing mutations occur in three different genes located on three different chromosomes, they all share a common biochemical pathway, that is, the altered production of Aβ leading to a relative overabundance of the Aβ42 species, which eventually results in neuronal cell death and dementia. Collectively, these discoveries provided the essential connection between the long-known familial aggregation of early-onset AD and the increase in Aβ production observed in the brains of autopsied AD patients, which originally gave rise to the “amyloid hypothesis of AD” (reviewed in Tanzi and Bertram 2005).
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
5
OTHER POTENTIAL EOFAD GENES Although no additional EOFAD gene has been unequivocally identified since the discovery of PSEN2 in 1995, several lines of evidence suggest that further genetic factors remain to be identified for this form of AD: (1) Numerous early-onset families do not show mutations in APP, PSEN1, or PSEN2 despite extensive sequencing efforts of open reading frames and adjacent intronic regions (Arango et al. 2001; Lleo et al. 2002; Rademakers et al. 2002; Raux et al. 2005); (2) Beyond APP and PS1 there are several additional key proteins involved in the γ- and β-secretase cleavage events and other aspects leading to the aggregation and deposition of Aβ (e.g., nicastrin, aph-1, pen-2, BACE), as well as the hyperphosphorylation of tau and the development of neurofibrillary tangles; (3) A full genome linkage screen performed by our group has identified at least four early-onset AD linkage regions in addition to the chromosomal location of PSEN1 on 14q24 (Blacker et al. 2003). Despite the to-date overall unsuccessful quest for novel EOFAD genes, recent—and still preliminary—reports have indicated the presence of disease-causing mutations in at least three additional genes, two of which are also strong biochemical candidates for an involvement in AD pathogenesis. First, a linkage study in a large and multigenerational clinically defined multiplex AD family from Belgium indicated the presence of an AD locus near the gene encoding tau on chromosome 17q (MAPT). Subsequently, a nonsynonymous mutation in exon 13 of MAPT (R406W) was reported to cosegregate with AD dementia in this family (Rademakers et al. 2003). While this same mutation was also reported in at least one other family with dementia resembling AD (Ostojic et al. 2004), the majority of cases affected by R406W appear to develop a syndrome fulfilling the criteria of frontotemporal dementia (FTDP-17; Rosso et al. 2003). It therefore remains to be determined whether at autopsy the clinically assessed Belgian AD family will prove to show pathological features in agreement with a definite diagnosis of AD. Second, the same group recently reported evidence of significant linkage with EOFAD to chromosome 7q36 in an extended multiplex AD family from the Netherlands (Rademakers et al. 2005). The same ∼10cM haplotype was also found to cosegregate with AD in three additional multiplex families, suggesting the presence of a disease-causing mutation in this chromosomal region. A synonymous mutation (Ala626) in the gene encoding PAX transcription activation domain interacting protein (PAXIP1), located ∼400,000bp downstream of the shared haplotype region, was discovered in AD patients of the index family, but absent
6
Dementia
from 320 control individuals. However, since it was also absent from the three additional 7q36 haplotype-sharing families, and—according to preliminary analyses—did not show evidence for functional abnormalities in mutation carriers, the overall evidence supporting PAXIP1 as a novel EOFAD gene remains relatively weak. Finally, a recent study reported the presence of an amino-acid changing mutation (Asp90Asn) in the gene encoding one of the γ-secretase components, pen-2 (PEN2; Sala Frigerio et al. 2005). In addition to being a strong pathophysiological candidate, this gene is also interesting positionally as it maps close to a highly significant linkage region on chromosome 19, approximately 9 Mb proximal of APOE (Bertram et al. 2004). However, as the familial transmission of this mutation with AD could not be determined due to a lack of DNA specimen, and since preliminary functional analyses did not reveal an effect of this mutation on APP metabolism in vitro, this finding can be considered the least convincing of these putative novel EOFAD loci. It can be expected that the now available ultra-high throughput, wholegenome sequencing approaches will allow the identification of additional AD-causing mutations in individual samples or small families that were otherwise not eligible for conventional analyses, that is, linkage followed by positional cloning. These technologies have already been successfully applied to a number of phenotypes (Tucker, Marra, and Friedman 2009), including neurodegenerative disorders (Lupski et al. 2010). LATE-ONSET ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE (LOAD) In contrast to EOFAD, late-onset Alzheimer ’s (LOAD) is characterized by a considerably more complex pattern of genetic and nongenetic factors that remains only poorly understood. Adding to the complexity are methodological difficulties inherent to common diseases in general, and late-onset diseases like AD in particular. Family data, for instance, is more often than not only incomplete (e.g., owing to relatives who died before the family-specific age of risk and/or the lack of genotypic information for parents). Another complication is the unknown number of “phenocopies,” that is, subjects with a nongenetic form of the disease or subjects suffering from other forms of age-related cognitive decline. These and other characteristics largely reduce the power to detect new loci in reasonably sized samples and continue to hamper the independent replication of proclaimed associations. This is demonstrated by the fact that even more than a decade after the discovery of APOE in AD (Saunders et al. 1993; Strittmatter et al. 1993), no other genetic risk factor has been found to consistently confer susceptibility to disease risk, despite intensive efforts in
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
7
many laboratories worldwide (Bertram et al. 2007). And although almost none of the over nearly 700 genes that have been tested for association with AD over the past 30 years have yielded consistent results to date, several lines of evidence suggest that further gene-hunting may indeed be worthwhile. First, there are a number of chromosomal regions showing evidence for genetic linkage from full-genome linkage studies (Bertram and Tanzi 2004; Butler et al. 2009). Second, a number of loci have recently emerged from genome-wide association studies, that is, studies for which the entire genome is interrogated by a relatively dense set of common genetic markers (Bertram and Tanzi 2009). While overlap in results across independent GWAS is still sparse, a few new loci have emerged that stand a rather good chance of representing true novel AD risk genes. Finally, systematic meta-analyses on all published AD genetic association studies show significant summary odds ratios for a few genes when all available genotype data is summed across studies (Bertram et al. 2007; see section below and Table 1.2). While many of these may still represent false-positive findings, it is interesting to note that several of the significantly associated variants are actually nonsynonymous or regulators of gene expression, which at least indirectly implies a functional basis for the observed statistical associations. APOLIPOPROTEIN E (APOE)— THE ONLY ESTABLISHED AD RISK GENE The first proof of principle for successful application of the “positional candidate gene strategy” (i.e., testing biologically plausible candidate genes in promising linkage regions) in AD was provided by the identification of APOE as a risk gene. The positional evidence was delivered almost simultaneously with the identification of the first APP mutations, when Pericak-Vance and colleagues reported linkage of chromosome 19q to cases of predominantly LOAD (Pericak-Vance et al. 1991). Two years later, after Aβ was found to bind apoE (which was also the first evidence for a potential biochemical involvement of apoE in AD), a common polymorphism in APOE, which maps near the 19q linkage region, was tested and shown to be associated with increased risk for AD (Saunders et al. 1993; Strittmatter et al. 1993). In contrast to all other genetic association reports in AD, this result has been overwhelmingly replicated in a large number of studies across many ethnic groups worldwide (Farrer et al. 1997; Bertram et al. 2007). Three major alleles occur at the APOE locus—ε2, ε3 and ε4—which translate into combinations of two amino acid changes at residues 112 and 158 of the apoE-protein (ε2: Cys/Cys; ε3: Cys/Arg; ε4: Arg/Arg,
Gene
APOE CLU PICALM SORL1 GWA_14q32.13 TNK1 ACE IL8 LDLR CST3 CR1 hCG2039140 CHRNB2 SORCS1 TNF CCR2 DAPK1 GAB2 TF MTHFR LOC651924 OTC ADAM10
Rank
1* 2* 3* 4 5* 6* 7 8 9 10 11* 12 13 14 15 16 17 18* 19 20 21* 22 23
Model
APOE_e2/3/4 e4 vs. e3 rs11136000 T vs. C rs541458 C vs. T rs2282649 T vs. C rs11622883 A vs. T rs1554948 A vs. T rs1800764 C vs. T rs4073 A vs. T rs5930 A vs. G rs1064039 A vs. G rs6656401 A vs. G rs1903908 T vs. C rs4845378 T vs. G rs600879 m vs. M rs4647198 C vs. T rs1799864 A vs. G rs4878104 T vs. C rs2373115 T vs. G rs1049296 C2 vs. C1 rs1801133 T vs. C rs6907175 A vs. G rs5963409 (F) A vs. G rs17269348 G vs. A
Polymorphism All All All A All All C All All C All All All All A C All All All All All All All
36 9 6 3 5 5 4 4 4 8 7 4 4 4 3 3 7 12 16 26 6 5 4
9413 26690 21915 1666 3568 3712 1565 1593 1501 3014 17181 2865 1363 2856 771 1426 5789 12577 9233 8604 4882 2864 10982
Ethnicity N studies N samples‡
Table 1.2 Summary of Genetic Findings for Late-Onset Complex Genetics Forms of AD
3.69 (3.30−4.12) 0.86 (0.82−0.89) 0.87 (0.83−0.91) 1.30 (1.13−1.50) 0.84 (0.77−0.93) 0.84 (0.76−0.93) 0.79 (0.68−0.92) 1.27 (1.08−1.50) 0.85 (0.72−0.99) 1.16 (1.00−1.33) 1.19 (1.09−1.29) 1.23 (1.06−1.44) 0.67 (0.50−0.90) 1.24 (1.04−1.48) 1.37 (1.05−1.79) 0.73 (0.56−0.97) 0.88 (0.82−0.96) 0.85 (0.76−0.94) 1.18 (1.06−1.31) 1.13 (1.04−1.23) 0.89 (0.82−0.97) 1.17 (1.04−1.33) 1.15 (1.03−1.28)
OR (95% CI)† <1×10−20 3.3×10−16 3.5×10−10 1.7×10−4 3.4×10−4 5.6×10−4 0.00217 0.00381 0.0386 0.0434 3.1×10−5 0.00703 0.00729 0.0168 0.0185 0.0239 0.00156 0.00227 0.00248 0.00311 0.00439 0.00842 0.00981
P-value A A A A A A A A A A B B B B B B C C C C C C C
Credibility
NEDD9 CH25H LOC439999 CALHM1 GRN IL33 IL1B PGBD1 THRA ENTPD7 TFAM IL1A ECE1 PRNP GAPDHS
rs760678 rs13500 rs498055 rs2986017 rs5848 rs7044343 rs1143634 rs3800324 rs939348 rs911541 rs2306604 rs1800587 rs213045 rs1799990 rs4806173
G vs. C T vs. C G vs. A T vs. C A vs. G C vs. T T vs. C A vs. G T vs. C G vs. A G vs. A T vs. C A vs. C G vs. A G vs. C
All All All C All All C All All All All C All C All
8 7 7 10 4 4 6 7 6 4 6 19 5 11 4
6697 3413 5285 8441 3711 9865 2720 6670 5841 7172 2336 8212 3691 6301 3462
0.89 (0.81−0.98) 1.44 (1.07−1.94) 1.15 (1.02−1.30) 1.18 (1.03−1.35) 1.13 (1.02−1.25) 0.85 (0.73−0.98) 1.16 (1.02−1.32) 1.21 (1.02−1.45) 1.10 (1.01−1.19) 1.10 (1.01−1.21) 0.86 (0.75−0.99) 1.09 (1.00−1.18) 0.86 (0.75−1.00) 0.92 (0.85−1.00) 0.87 (0.75−1.00)
0.0116 0.0137 0.0161 0.0162 0.0173 0.0213 0.0214 0.0279 0.0296 0.0336 0.0364 0.0386 0.0443 0.0445 0.0478
C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C
Indicates loci/variants originally identified by GWAS (see also Table 1.3).
Summary ORs and 95% confidence intervals (CI) are based on random-effects allelic contrasts comparing minor vs. major alleles at each polymorphism.
†
Number of samples refers to the number of independent case-control samples used in the meta-analyses; multiple samples may be reported in the same publication and are considered separately if they are independent, i.e., nonoverlapping. Samples overlapping across publications are only used once, usually by including the datasets with the largest number of available genotypes.
‡
*
Source: Modified after content on the AlzGene website (http://www.alzgene.org; current on May 1, 2010). List of loci, in descending order of genetic effect size, containing at least one polymorphism showing nominally significant (P-value ≤0.05) summary ORs. To be considered as “Top Result,” summary OR needs to be significant across samples from all ethnic backgrounds (“ALL”) or ethnicity-specific strata (“C” Caucasians only, “A” Asian only). Note that AlzGene is continuously updated, so results displayed online may differ from the results above; consult the AlzGene website for up-to-date numbers and additional meta-analyses in these and other loci.
24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31* 32 33 34 35 36 37 38
10
Dementia
respectively). While the ε4 allele has been demonstrated to significantly increase the risk for AD, its minor allele—ε2—has been (less consistently) associated with a decreased risk for AD (Corder et al. 1994). Although the association between ε4 and AD is robust, it is not specific. In contrast to the genetic effects in EOFAD, the presence of ε4 is neither necessary nor sufficient to actually cause the disease. Rather, it appears to be a modifier of disease susceptibility, predominantly by decreasing the age of onset in a dose-dependent manner, in the sense that homozygous carriers on average show a younger onset age than carriers of just a single copy (Blacker et al. 1997; Meyer et al. 1998). In terms of disease risk, this effect translates into an increased relative risk (as measured by the odds ratio) of roughly threefold for heterozygous, and ∼fifteen-fold for homozygous ε4-carriers as compared to the ε3/3 genotype (Farrer et al. 1997). Similar trends can be seen across different ethnic groups, ranging from odds ratios of around twofold in Hispanic to more than thirty-threefold in Japanese ε4/4 individuals. Interestingly, APOE ε4, or genetic markers being significantly correlated with this allele, are also by far the most significant signal emerging from any of the currently published genomewide studies on AD (see Table 1.3), suggesting that probably all other AD risk-alleles either exert much smaller effects or are much less common in the general population. Despite the overwhelming support from genetic data suggesting an important contribution of this gene to AD risk or onset age variation, the potential biological consequences of the APOE polymorphisms remain only incompletely understood. The most straightforward hypothesis proposes that the different variants directly influence Aβ-accumulation (Strittmatter et al. 1993). Several lines of evidence support this notion, showing an increased number of Aβ-plaques in the brain of ε4-allele carriers and marked differences in the deposition of Aβ-plaques depending on the presence or absence of human APOE in transgenic mice overexpressing APP (for recent reviews on APOE function, see Bu 2009; Vance and Hayashi 2010). Furthermore, there is evidence for an APOE dependent onset age variation in EOFAD caused by PSEN1 mutations (Nacmias et al. 1995). However, no consensus has yet been reached regarding the predominant mechanism(s) underlying these effects. A large number of in vitro and in vivo studies have suggested direct or indirect effects of the different apoE isoforms on Aβ production, Aβ clearance, Aβ fibrillization, tangle formation, synaptic plasticity and repair, and neuronal toxicity (Bu 2009; Vance and Hayashi 2010). Furthermore systemic dysfunction in lipid transport and, more specifically, cholesterol homeostasis is a possible pathway. Interestingly, cholesterol has also been shown to both increase
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
11
Table 1.3 Overview of All Published Genome-Wide Association Studies (GWAS) in AD
GWAS
Design
Population
Grupe CaseUSA and et al. 2007 control UK
No. SNPs 17,343
Reiman CaseUSA, Neth- 502,627 et al. 2007 control erlands Li et al. CaseCanada 469,438 2008 control and UK USA Poduslo Familyet al. 2009 based and casecontrol Abraham CaseUK‡ # et al. 2008 control Bertram FamilyUSA et al. 2008 based
489,218
561,494 484,522
No. AD GWAS (followup)†
380 (1428) 396 (1666) APOE*, ACAN, BCR, CTSS, EBF3, FAM63A**, GALP, GWA_14q32.13, GWA_7p15.2, LMNA, LOC651924, MYH13, PCK1, PGBD1, TNK1, TRAK2, UBD 446 (415) 290 (260) APOE*, GAB2 753 (418)
9 (199)
736 (249) APOE*, GOLM1, GWA_15q21.2, GWA_9p24.3 10 (225) TRPC4AP
1239 APOE*, LRAT (1400) 941 (1767) 404 (838) APOE*, ATXN1, CD33, GWA_14q31 492 (238) 496 (220) APOE*, FAM113B 844 (1547) 1255 APOE*, (1209) PCDH11X 1082 (−)
532,000 Beecham CaseUSA‡ et al. 2009 control CaseUSA 313,504 Carrascontrol quillo et al. 2009 Harold CaseEurope and 529,205 3941 (2023) et al. 2009 control USA‡ Lambert CaseEurope‡ et al. 2009 control
No. CTRL GWAS (follow- “Featured” up)† Genes
537,029 2032 (3978)
7848 APOE*, CLU (APOJ), (2340) PICALM 5328 APOE*, CLU (3297) (APOJ), CR1 (Continued)
12
Dementia
Table 1.3 (Continued) Potkin CaseUSA et al. 2009 control (ADNI)
Heinzen CaseUSA‡ et al. 2009 control
516,645
172 (−)
n.g.
331 (−)
209 (−) APOE*, ARSB, CAND1, EFNA5, MAGI2, PRUNE2 368 (−) APOE*, CHRNA7
Source: Modified after content on the AlzGene website (http://www.alzgene.org; current on May 1, 2010). Studies are listed in order of publication date (determined by PubMed ID).“Featured Genes” are those genes/loci that were declared as “associated” in the original publication, note that criteria for declaring association may vary across studies. *
In many studies, surrogate markers were used for APOE.
**
This locus was originally named “THEM5.”
†
Numbers of “AD GWAS” and “CTRL GWAS” refers to sample sizes used in initial GWA screening, whereas “Follow-up” refers to follow-up samples (where applicable).
‡
Some subjects and datasets overlap across these studies; see AlzGene website for more details.
#
This study is based on a pooled genotyping approach which may lead to invalid allele frequency estimates and results.
Aβ-production and to stabilize the peptide in the brains of transgenic AD mice. Thus, it is possible that APOE-ε4 confers risk for AD via mechanisms that are shared with its effects on cardiovascular disease, for example, by increasing a carrier ’s risk for hypercholesterolemia, as this would also elevate accumulation of Aβ. SYSTEMATIC FIELD SYNOPSIS AND META-ANALYSES OF AD GENETIC ASSOCIATION STUDIES Due to the increasingly large number of genetic association studies in the field of AD, it has become virtually impossible to systematically follow, evaluate, or interpret these findings. In an attempt to facilitate this situation, we have created a publicly available database, “AlzGene” (http://www.alzgene.org), which systematically collects, summarizes and meta-analyzes all genetic association studies published in the field of AD (Bertram et al. 2007). After thorough and ongoing searches of the available scientific literature, studies published in peer-reviewed journals that are available in English are included in AlzGene. Key variables (such as ancestry, type of AD diagnosis, sample size, onset age, and genotype
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
13
distributions) are extracted from the original publications. Furthermore, published genotype data from independent case-control samples are systematically meta-analyzed. Because this approach quantitatively synthesizes all of the published genotype data for each polymorphism, it facilitates the overall interpretation of association findings: rather than relying on the either positive (“significant”) or negative (“insignificant”) outcomes of individual studies, it produces summary odds ratios that take into consideration all data at once and account for within and between study variation. At the day of this writing (May 2010), AlzGene includes more than 1,300 individual studies and showcases the results of nearly 300 individual meta-analyses. In these, nearly 40 genes that are not related to the wellestablished APOE ε4-allele show nominally significant risk effects (see Table 1.2). Interestingly, about one-fourth of these were originally implicated by one of the twelve published genome-wide association studies (GWAS; see below), although independent replication by other groups is still lacking for most of these findings. As can be seen in Table 1.2, the average allelic summary odds ratio (OR), for non APOE-related effects are very modest (∼1.2 for “risk” alleles and ∼0.8 for “protective” alleles) compared to an OR of ∼3–4 for a single copy of the APOE ε4-allele. These modest effect sizes are in good agreement with those found in other genetically complex diseases (Ioannidis et al. 2003; Lohmueller et al. 2003; Allen et al. 2008) and have important (and well-known) implications for the design of future genetic association studies in AD. For instance, sample sizes will need to be vastly increased in order to detect or exclude ORs of ∼1.2 or below with sufficient confidence. In addition to the calculation of summary ORs for eligible polymorphisms, AlzGene also assesses the “epidemiologic credibility” of all nominally significant meta-analysis findings using guidelines recently proposed by investigators from the Human Genome Epidemiology Network (HuGENet; (Ioannidis et al. 2008; Khoury et al. 2009). In brief, these guidelines take into account the power, degree of heterogeneity, and evidence for bias across each meta-analysis, and subsequently assign a grade categorizing the overall degree of epidemiologic credibility for each polymorphism. The grading scheme distinguishes between “strong” (grade A), “moderate” (grade B), and “weak” (grade C) epidemiologic credibility. Of all 38 genes and loci currently showing nominally significant summary ORs in AlzGene meta-analyses, 10 are classified as displaying “strong” epidemiologic credibility based on these guidelines (see Table 1.2).
14
Dementia
GENOME-WIDE ASSOCIATION STUDIES (GWAS) IN AD An alternative to the traditional candidate gene approach is afforded by recent advances in large-scale genotyping technologies that now enable researchers to perform comprehensive and largely hypothesisfree genome-wide association studies (GWAS; Craddock, O’Donovan, and Owen 2008; McCarthy et al. 2008). For many genetically complex diseases, the GWAS approach has yielded an unexpectedly large number of genome-wide significant findings that were confirmed by independent follow-up studies (reviewed in McCarthy et al. 2008; Ioannidis, Thomas, and Daly 2009). In many instances, these findings promise to advance our understanding of the pathogenetic forces underlying the investigated phenotypes, hopefully enabling researchers and clinicians not only to improve diagnostic accuracy, but also to deliver a whole array of new drug targets hopefully leading to more efficient treatment and disease prevention options in the not too distant future. In AD, GWAS results have thus far proved to be less consistent (see Table 1.3 and Bertram and Tanzi 2009 for review), with the exception of the APOE locus, whose association with AD was identified in all but one study, and always found to be orders of magnitude more significant than any of the other, newly implicated loci. Regardless of the currently observed lack of consistency across studies, the jury is still out as to how many and which of the potential new AD loci will replicate in independent follow-up studies as most of the currently published GWAS have only appeared within the past two years. Based on the data available at the time of this writing, the most compelling novel GWAS signals are observed in CLU (clusterin; a.k.a. apolipoprotein J [APOJ]), CR1 (complement component (3b/4b) receptor 1), GAB2 (GRB2-associated binding protein 2), and PICALM (phosphatidylinositol binding clathrin assembly protein), followed by less consistently replicated signals in PGBD1 (piggyBac transposable element derived 1), and TNK1 (tyrosine kinase, nonreceptor 1). Finally, there are at least three replicated loci in hitherto uncharacterized genomic intervals on chromosomes 14q32.13, 14q31.2 and 6q24.1 likely implicating the existence of novel AD genes in these regions. It is important to emphasize that while the statistical support for GWAS signals is—by virtue of their design—very strong, the observed effect sizes (ORs) may not be. For instance, the GWAS signal with the strongest statistical support (lowest P-value) after APOE is elicited by genetic variants in the CLU gene. However, the effect exerted by these variants is only very modest, suggesting a mere ∼15% decrease in AD risk in carriers versus noncarriers of the minor alleles.
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
15
CURRENT TOP-RANKED AD CANDIDATE GENES Taking into account all of the available association data from both GWAS and candidate gene studies, current meta-analysis results implicate some three dozen loci to show at least nominally significant association with risk for AD (see Table 1.2). Applying recently proposed guidelines for the cumulative assessment of genetic association data in complex disorders suggests that at least 10 of these loci show particularly strong “epidemiologic credibility” (grade A) when all of the available genotype data is taken into consideration. These loci and their potential pathogenetic implications in AD are briefly discussed in the following paragraph. The gene currently showing the strongest association with AD risk after APOE is CLU (clusterin; a.k.a. apolipoprotein J [APOJ]), a 75 kDa chaperone protein that is expressed at high levels in the brain and binds to and promotes the clearance of Aβ, a function potentially shared with apoE (Nuutinen et al. 2009). Besides APOE, CLU currently represents the only locus to show genome-wide significant association in more than one GWAS (Harold et al. 2009; Lambert et al. 2009). Another recent GWAS locus (Harold et al. 2009) is located close to PICALM (phosphatidylinositol binding clathrin assembly protein), which encodes for a protein that is involved in VAMP2 trafficking—a process involved in synaptic neurotransmitter release (Harel et al. 2008)—and which may also be linked to the production of Aβ via clathrin-mediated endocytosis (Harold et al. 2009). SORL1 (sortilin-related receptor) belongs to a family of sorting receptors that contain a VPS10 (vacuolar protein sorting protein 10) domain, via which these proteins mediate a variety of intracellular sorting and trafficking functions (Yamazaki et al. 1996). Biochemical evidence suggests that the protein encoded by SORL1 directly influences the production of Aβ by affecting the processing/trafficking of APP (Andersen et al. 2005; Offe et al. 2006) via binding to a complement-type repeat (CR) domain located near 3’ half of the gene (Andersen et al. 2006), which collectively also shows the strongest association with AD risk (Rogaeva et al. 2007). The GWA_14q32 locus was identified in one of the first GWAS published in AD (Grupe et al. 2007) and has not yet been assigned to a specific gene. Until further genetic experiments uncover the functional basis for the observed association(s) their potential pathogenetic relevance (if any) in AD remains unclear. TNK1 (tyrosine kinase, non-receptor, 1), also originally identified in the GWAS by Grupe et al. (2007), is a nonreceptor tyrosine kinase originally known as “thirty-eight-negative kinase 1” that could be linked to AD via its reported role to enable tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α)-induced apoptosis (Azoitei et al. 2007). Thus, TNK1
16
Dementia
may act as a novel molecular switch that can determine the properties of TNF-α signaling and, potentially, neuronal cell death. ACE (angiotensin converting enzyme 1) is a ubiquitously expressed zinc metalloprotease that is involved in blood pressure regulation. Of potential relevance for AD is the observation that the ACE-protein is able to degrade naturally secreted Aβ in vitro (Hu et al. 2001; Hemming and Selkoe 2005), which could explain the observed increase in AD risk. In addition, its risk-exerting effect could also be the result of ACE’s prominent role in blood pressure regulation, as some epidemiological studies suggest that high mid-life blood pressure may increase the risk for AD in later life (Takeda et al. 2008). IL8 (interleukin 8) is a member of the CXC chemokine family and represents a major mediator of inflammatory responses. In AD, IL8 (a.k.a. CXCL8) has been reported to be increased in the brains of AD patients versus controls in some studies (Mines, Ding, and Fan 2007). Besides potentially playing a role in mediating inflammatory responses accompanying AD neuropathology, a more specific role has recently been suggested in a study showing that the receptor for IL8 (CXCR2) may be involved in APP metabolism and Aβ production via modulation of γ-secretase activity (Bakshi et al. 2008). LDLR (low-density lipoprotein receptor) is a membrane-spanning glycoprotein that plays a critical role in removing low-density lipoproteins (LDL and VLDL) from the blood and is the main receptor for apoE in neurons. Recent in vitro and in vivo experiments suggest that LDLR may have an important function in AD neuropathogenesis, in particular the clearance and deposition of Aβ (Kim et al. 2009; Abisambra et al. 2010). Finally, CST3 (cystatin C), which is the most abundant extracellular inhibitor of cysteine proteases, was found to bind Aβ (Vinters et al. 1990) and to inhibit Aβ-fibril formation in a concentration-dependent manner in vitro (Sastre et al. 2004) and in vivo (Kaeser et al. 2007). These findings, together with the observation that there is a general reduction in neuroprotection when levels of CysC are decreased in vitro may be the functional correlates of the observed epidemiological association between this gene and increased risk in AD (Crawford et al. 2000). While many of the proposed biochemical mechanisms outlined above appear plausible and well supported by functional genetics data, it should be added that the majority of the genes currently highlighted in AlzGene were originally tested for genetic association precisely because they emerged as promising functional candidates. This is even true for the top-ranking non-APOE GWAS hit, CLU, which owing to its functional relatedness with apoE actually represents one of the first studied candidate genes in AD
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
17
(Tycko et al. 1996). At the time, however, lack of power had precluded an earlier recognition of this locus as an AD risk gene. Thus, more functional and molecular genetic experiments are necessary to decipher the precise role of the implicated sequence variants in AD pathogenesis and to determine their potential usefulness in diagnostic or pharmacogenetic settings. OUTLOOK TO FUTURE GENETIC STUDIES OF AD While the systematic collection and analysis of data already existing in the AD literature clearly facilitates the interpretation and follow-up of the hitherto proposed candidate genes, the loci investigated to date represent only a fraction of the sequence currently known to exert functional roles in the human genome. The ongoing development and availability of better and more affordable genotyping and high-throughput sequencing technologies will allow for a much more systematic and efficient assessment of the remainder of the genome in the search for common and rare disease modifiers over the coming years. Whole genome sequencing, in particular, will reveal whether or not the currently observed association results all point to the same underlying disease-modifying allele (as is likely the case for ε4 in APOE), or whether they merely represent a common chromosomal background for much less common mutational events that cannot currently be interrogated by any of the available GWAS microarrays. Due to the vastly increased amount of data generated, however, the outcome of such studies will even more critically than before depend on a careful study design, which includes choosing appropriate ascertainment strategies, maintaining a high phenotyping accuracy, devising a careful analysis plan able to handle the immense multiple testing problem, and successfully and consistently replicating the observed effects in independent samples from different populations before drawing any further-reaching conclusions. Based on the experiences gathered from AlzGene and other related projects (Ioannidis et al. 2003; Lohmueller et al. 2003; Allen et al. 2008; Khoury et al. 2009), however, the single most important factor in identifying genuine and reproducible susceptibility variants will be the use of sufficiently sized, and thus: sufficiently powered, samples. Assuming that most of the still elusive genes will exert small effects, that is, increasing or decreasing disease risk by ∼25% or less, combined sample sizes of 6,000 to over 25,000 cases and controls (for minor allele frequencies between 50% to 5%, respectively) may be necessary to achieve 80% power to detect genome-wide significance (i.e., a P-value of ∼5 × 10-8). Considering that far less than 5% of all studies in the field of AD genetics published to
18
Dementia
date actually have enough power to detect such small effects, a concerted collaborative effort among individual laboratories is needed in order to arrive at the required sample sizes and to allow these promising new technologies to reach their full potential. CONCLUSION Despite the great progress in the field of AD genetics that has led to the discovery and confirmation of three autosomal-dominant early-onset genes (APP, PSEN1, PSEN1) and one late-onset risk-factor (APOE), strong evidence exists suggesting the presence of additional AD genes for both forms of the disease. The hunt for these genes is aggravated by factors that generally complicate the identification of complex disease genes: locus and/or allelic heterogeneity, small effect sizes of the underlying variants, unknown and difficult to model interaction patterns, population differences, insufficient sample sizes and/or sampling strategies, and linkage disequilibrium among polymorphisms other than those initially associated with the disease. The emergence of more powerful and efficient genotyping and sequencing technologies (e.g., whole genome sequencing) as well as analysis tools (e.g., systematic and continuously updated meta-analyses) should enable us to better disentangle the genetics of AD and other complex diseases. Eventually, the insights gained from such studies will lead to a better understanding of the pathophysiological mechanisms leading to neurodegeneration and dementia. This knowledge will lay the foundation to developing new treatment strategies that will ultimately allow curing, delaying, or even preventing this devastating disease. REFERENCES Abisambra, J. F., T. Fiorelli, J. Padmanabhan, P. Neame, I. Wefes, and H. Potter. 2010. LDLR expression and localization are altered in mouse and human cell culture models of Alzheimer ’s disease. PLoS One 5: e8556. Allen, N. C., S. Bagade, M. B. McQueen, J. P. Ioannidis, F. K. Kavvoura, M. J. Khoury, R. E. Tanzi, and L. Bertram. 2008. Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies in schizophrenia: The SzGene database. Nat Genet 40: 827–834. Andersen, O. M., J. Reiche, V. Schmidt, M. Gotthardt, R. Spoelgen, J. Behlke, C. A. von Arnim, et al. 2005. Neuronal sorting protein-related receptor sorLA/LR11 regulates processing of the amyloid precursor protein. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 13461–13466. Andersen, O. M., V. Schmidt, R. Spoelgen, J. Gliemann, J. Behlke, D. Galatis, W. J. McKinstry, et al. 2006. Molecular dissection of the interaction between
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
19
amyloid precursor protein and its neuronal trafficking receptor SorLA/ LR11. Biochemistry 45: 2618–2628. Arango, D., M. Cruts, O. Torres, H. Backhovens, M. L. Serrano, E. Villareal, P. Montanes, et al. 2001. Systematic genetic study of Alzheimer disease in Latin America: Mutation frequencies of the amyloid beta precursor protein and presenilin genes in Colombia. Am J Med Genet 103: 138–143. Azoitei, N., A. Brey, T. Busch, S. Fulda, G. Adler, and T. Seufferlein. 2007. Thirtyeight-negative kinase 1 (TNK1) facilitates TNFalpha-induced apoptosis by blocking NF-kappaB activation. Oncogene 26: 6536–6545. Bakshi, P., E. Margenthaler, V. Laporte, F. Crawford, and M. Mullan. 2008. Novel role of CXCR2 in regulation of gamma-secretase activity. ACS Chem Biol 3: 777–789. Bertram, L., M. B. McQueen, K. Mullin, D. Blacker, and R. E. Tanzi. 2007. Systematic meta-analyses of Alzheimer disease genetic association studies: The AlzGene database. Nat Genet 39: 17–23. Bertram, L., R. Menon, K. Mullin, M. Parkinson, M. L. Bradley, D. Blacker, and R. E. Tanzi. 2004. PEN2 is not a genetic risk factor for Alzheimer ’s disease in a large family sample. Neurology 62: 304–306. Bertram, L., and R. E. Tanzi. 2004. Alzheimer ’s disease: One disorder, too many genes? Hum Mol Genet 13 (Spec. No. 1): R135–141. Bertram, L., and R. E. Tanzi. 2008. Thirty years of Alzheimer ’s disease genetics: The implications of systematic meta-analyses. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 768–778. Bertram, L., and R. E. Tanzi. 2009. Genome-wide association studies in Alzheimer ’s disease. Hum Mol Genet 18: R137–145. Blacker, D., L. Bertram, A. J. Saunders, T. J. Moscarillo, M. S. Albert, H. Wiener, R. T. Perry, et al. 2003. Results of a high-resolution genome screen of 437 Alzheimer ’s disease families. Hum Mol Genet 12: 23–32. Blacker, D., J. L. Haines, L. Rodes, H. Terwedow, R. C. Go, L. E. Harrell, R. T. Perry, S. S. Bassett, G. Chase, D. Meyers, M. S. Albert, and R. Tanzi. 1997. ApoE-4 and age at onset of Alzheimer ’s disease: The NIMH genetics initiative. Neurology 48: 139–147. Brouwers, N., K. Sleegers, S. Engelborghs, V. Bogaerts, S. Serneels, K. Kamali, E. Corsmit, et al. 2006. Genetic risk and transcriptional variability of amyloid precursor protein in Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain 129: 2984–2991. Bu, G. 2009. Apolipoprotein E and its receptors in Alzheimer ’s disease: Pathways, pathogenesis and therapy. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 333–344. Butler, A. W., M. Y. Ng, M. L. Hamshere, P. Forabosco, R. Wroe, A. Al-Chalabi, C. M. Lewis, and J. F. Powell. 2009. Meta-analysis of linkage studies for Alzheimer ’s disease—A web resource. Neurobiol Aging 30: 1037–1047. Corder, E. H., A. M. Saunders, N. J. Risch, W. J. Strittmatter, D. E. Schmechel, P. C. Gaskell Jr., J. B. Rimmler, et al. 1994. Protective effect of apolipoprotein E type 2 allele for late onset Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 7: 180–184. Craddock, N., M. C. O’Donovan, and M. J. Owen. 2008. Genome-wide association studies in psychiatry: Lessons from early studies of non-psychiatric and psychiatric phenotypes. Mol Psychiatry 13: 649–653.
20
Dementia
Crawford, F. C., M. J. Freeman, J. A. Schinka, L. I. Abdullah, M. Gold, R. Hartman, K. Krivian, et al. 2000. A polymorphism in the cystatin C gene is a novel risk factor for late-onset Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 55: 763–768. Farrer, L. A., L. A. Cupples, J. L. Haines, B. Hyman, W. A. Kukull, R. Mayeux, R. H. Myers, M. A. Pericak-Vance, N. Risch, and C. M. van Duijn. 1997. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. JAMA 278: 1349–1356. Gatz, M., C. A. Reynolds, L. Fratiglioni, B. Johansson, J. A. Mortimer, S. Berg, A. Fiske, and N. L. Pedersen. 2006. Role of genes and environments for explaining Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 168–174. Goate, A., M. C. Chartier-Harlin, M. Mullan, J. Brown, F. Crawford, L. Fidani, L. Giuffra, et al. 1991. Segregation of a missense mutation in the amyloid precursor protein gene with familial Alzheimer ’s disease. Nature 349: 704–706. Grupe, A., R. Abraham, Y. Li, C. Rowland, P. Hollingworth, A. Morgan, L. Jehu, et al. 2007. Evidence for novel susceptibility genes for late-onset Alzheimer ’s disease from a genome-wide association study of putative functional variants. Hum Mol Genet 16: 865–873. Harel, A., F. Wu, M. P. Mattson, C. M. Morris, and P. J. Yao. 2008. Evidence for CALM in directing VAMP2 trafficking. Traffic 9: 417–429. Harold, D., R. Abraham, P. Hollingworth, R. Sims, A. Gerrish, M. L. Hamshere, J. S. Pahwa, et al. 2009. Genome-wide association study identifies variants at CLU and PICALM associated with Alzheimer ’s disease. Nat Genet 41: 1088–1093. Hemming, M. L., and D. J. Selkoe. 2005. Amyloid beta-protein is degraded by cellular angiotensin-converting enzyme (ACE) and elevated by an ACE inhibitor. J Biol Chem 280: 37644–37650. Hu, J., A. Igarashi, M. Kamata, and H. Nakagawa. 2001. Angiotensin-converting enzyme degrades Alzheimer amyloid beta-peptide (A beta); retards A beta aggregation, deposition, fibril formation; and inhibits cytotoxicity. J Biol Chem 276: 47863–47868. Ioannidis, J. P., P. Boffetta, J. Little, T. R. O’Brien, A. G. Uitterlinden, P. Vineis, D. J. Balding, et al. 2008. Assessment of cumulative evidence on genetic associations: Interim guidelines. Int J Epidemiol 37: 120–132. Ioannidis, J. P., G. Thomas, and M. J. Daly. 2009. Validating, augmenting and refining genome-wide association signals. Nat Rev Genet 10: 318–329. Ioannidis, J. P., T. A. Trikalinos, E. E. Ntzani, and D. G. Contopoulos-Ioannidis. 2003. Genetic associations in large versus small studies: An empirical assessment. Lancet 361: 567–571. Kaeser, S. A., M. C. Herzig, J. Coomaraswamy, E. Kilger, M. L. Selenica, D. T. Winkler, M. Staufenbiel, E. Levy, A. Grubb, and M. Jucker. 2007. Cystatin C modulates cerebral beta-amyloidosis. Nat Genet 39: 1437–1439.
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
21
Khoury, M. J., L. Bertram, P. Boffetta, A. S. Butterworth, S. J. Chanock, S. M. Dolan, I. Fortier, et al. 2009. Genome-wide association studies, field synopses, and the development of the knowledge base on genetic variation and human diseases. Am J Epidemiol 170: 269–279. Kim, J., J. M. Castellano, H. Jiang, J. M. Basak, M. Parsadanian, V. Pham, S. M. Mason, S. M. Paul, and D. M. Holtzman. 2009. Overexpression of lowdensity lipoprotein receptor in the brain markedly inhibits amyloid deposition and increases extracellular A beta clearance. Neuron 64: 632–644. Lambert, J. C., S. Heath, G. Even, D. Campion, K. Sleegers, M. Hiltunen, O. Combarros, et al. 2009. Genome-wide association study identifies variants at CLU and CR1 associated with Alzheimer ’s disease. Nat Genet 41: 1094–1099. Levy-Lahad, E., W. Wasco, P. Poorkaj, D. M. Romano, J. Oshima, W. H. Pettingell, C. E. Yu, P. D. Jondro, et al. 1995. Candidate gene for the chromosome 1 familial Alzheimer ’s disease locus. Science 269: 973–977. Lleo, A., R. Blesa, R. Queralt, M. Ezquerra, J. L. Molinuevo, J. Pena-Casanova, A. Rojo, and R. Oliva. 2002. Frequency of mutations in the presenilin and amyloid precursor protein genes in early-onset Alzheimer disease in Spain. Arch Neurol 59: 1759–1763. Lohmueller, K. E., C. L. Pearce, M. Pike, E. S. Lander, and J. N. Hirschhorn. 2003. Meta-analysis of genetic association studies supports a contribution of common variants to susceptibility to common disease. Nat Genet 33: 177–182. Lupski, J. R., J. G. Reid, C. Gonzaga-Jauregui, D. Rio Deiros, D. C. Chen, L. Nazareth, M. Bainbridge, et al. 2010. Whole-genome sequencing in a patient with Charcot-Marie-Tooth neuropathy. N Engl J Med 362: 1181–1191. McCarthy, M. I., G. R. Abecasis, L. R. Cardon, D. B. Goldstein, J. Little, J. P. Ioannidis, and J. N. Hirschhorn. 2008. Genome-wide association studies for complex traits: Consensus, uncertainty and challenges. Nat Rev Genet 9: 356–369. Meyer, M. R., J. T. Tschanz, M. C. Norton, K. A. Welsh-Bohmer, D. C. Steffens, B. W. Wyse, and J. C. Breitner. 1998. APOE genotype predicts when—not whether—one is predisposed to develop Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 19: 321–322. Mines, M., Y. Ding, and G. H. Fan. 2007. The many roles of chemokine receptors in neurodegenerative disorders: Emerging new therapeutical strategies. Curr Med Chem 14: 2456–2470. Mullan, M., H. Houlden, M. Windelspecht, L. Fidani, C. Lombardi, P. Diaz, M. Rossor, et al. 1992. A locus for familial early-onset Alzheimer ’s disease on the long arm of chromosome 14, proximal to the alpha 1-antichymotrypsin gene. Nat Genet 2: 340–342. Nacmias, B., S. Latorraca, P. Piersanti, P. Forleo, S. Piacentini, L. Bracco, L. Amaducci, and S. Sorbi. 1995. ApoE genotype and familial Alzheimer ’s disease: A possible influence on age of onset in APP717 Val—>Ile mutated families. Neurosci Lett 183: 1–3.
22
Dementia
Nuutinen, T., T. Suuronen, A. Kauppinen, and A. Salminen. 2009. Clusterin: A forgotten player in Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain Res Rev 61: 89–104. Offe, K., S. E. Dodson, J. T. Shoemaker, J. J. Fritz, M. Gearing, A. I. Levey, and J. J. Lah. 2006. The lipoprotein receptor LR11 regulates amyloid beta production and amyloid precursor protein traffic in endosomal compartments. J Neurosci 26: 1596–1603. Ostojic, J., C. Elfgren, U. Passant, K. Nilsson, L. Gustafson, L. Lannfelt, and S. Froelich Fabre. 2004. The tau R406W mutation causes progressive presenile dementia with bitemporal atrophy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 298–301. Pericak-Vance, M. A., J. L. Bebout, P. C. Gaskell, L. H. Yamaoka, W. Y. Hung, M. J. Alberts, A. P. Walker, et al. 1991. Linkage studies in familial Alzheimer disease: Evidence for chromosome 19 linkage. Am J Hum Genet 48: 1034–1050. Price, D. L., R. E. Tanzi, D. R. Borchelt, and S. S. Sisodia. 1998. Alzheimer ’s disease: Genetic studies and transgenic models. Annu Rev Genet 32: 461–493. Rademakers, R., M. Cruts, B. Dermaut, K. Sleegers, S. M. Rosso, M. Van den Broeck, H. Backhovens, J. van Swieten, C. M. van Duijn, and C. Van Broeckhoven. 2002. Tau negative frontal lobe dementia at 17q21: Significant finemapping of the candidate region to a 4.8 cM interval. Mol Psychiatry 7: 1064–1074. Rademakers, R., M. Cruts, K. Sleegers, B. Dermaut, J. Theuns, Y. Aulchenko, S. Weckx, et al. 2005. Linkage and association studies identify a novel locus for Alzheimer disease at 7q36 in a Dutch population-based sample. Am J Hum Genet 77: 643–652. Rademakers, R., B. Dermaut, K. Peeters, M. Cruts, P. Heutink, A. Goate, and C. Van Broeckhoven. 2003. Tau (MAPT) mutation Arg406Trp presenting clinically with Alzheimer disease does not share a common founder in Western Europe. Hum Mutat 22: 409–411. Raux, G., L. Guyant-Marechal, C. Martin, J. Bou, C. Penet, A. Brice, D. Hannequin, T. Frebourg, and D. Campion. 2005. Molecular diagnosis of autosomal dominant early onset Alzheimer ’s disease: An update. J Med Genet 42: 793–795. Rogaev, E. I., R. Sherrington, E. A. Rogaeva, G. Levesque, M. Ikeda, Y. Liang, H. Chi, et al. 1995. Familial Alzheimer ’s disease in kindreds with missense mutations in a gene on chromosome 1 related to the Alzheimer ’s disease type 3 gene. Nature 376: 775–778. Rogaeva, E., Y. Meng, J. H. Lee, Y. Gu, T. Kawarai, F. Zou, T. Katayama, et al. 2007. The neuronal sortilin-related receptor SORL1 is genetically associated with Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 39: 168–177. Rosso, S. M., Donker L. Kaat, T. Baks, M. Joosse, I. de Koning, Y. Pijnenburg, et al. 2003. Frontotemporal dementia in The Netherlands: Patient characteristics and prevalence estimates from a population-based study. Brain 126: 2016–2022. Rovelet-Lecrux, A., D. Hannequin, G. Raux, N. Le Meur, A. Laquerriere, A. Vital, C. Dumanchin, et al. 2006. APP locus duplication causes autosomal dominant
The Genetics of Alzheimer’s Disease
23
early-onset Alzheimer disease with cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Nat Genet 38: 24–26. Rumble, B., R. Retallack, C. Hilbich, G. Simms, G. Multhaup, R. Martins, A. Hockey, P. Montgomery, K. Beyreuther, and C. L. Masters. 1989. Amyloid A4 protein and its precursor in Down’s syndrome and Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 320: 1446–1452. Sala Frigerio, C., P. Piscopo, E. Calabrese, A. Crestini, L. Malvezzi Campeggi, R. Civita di Fava, S. Fogliarino, et al. 2005. PEN-2 gene mutation in a familial Alzheimer ’s disease case. J Neurol 252: 1033–1036. Sastre, M., M. Calero, M. Pawlik, P. M. Mathews, A. Kumar, V. Danilov, S. D. Schmidt, R. A. Nixon, B. Frangione, and E. Levy. 2004. Binding of cystatin C to Alzheimer ’s amyloid beta inhibits in vitro amyloid fibril formation. Neurobiol Aging 25: 1033–1043. Saunders, A. M., W. J. Strittmatter, D. Schmechel, P. H. George-Hyslop, M. A. Pericak-Vance, S. H. Joo, B. L. Rosi, et al. 1993. Association of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 43: 1467–1472. Schellenberg, G. D., T. D. Bird, E. M. Wijsman, H. T. Orr, L. Anderson, E. Nemens, J. A. White, et al. 1992. Genetic linkage evidence for a familial Alzheimer ’s disease locus on chromosome 14. Science 258: 668–671. Sherrington, R., E. I. Rogaev, Y. Liang, E. A. Rogaeva, G. Levesque, M. Ikeda, H. Chi, et al. 1995. Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in earlyonset familial Alzheimer ’s disease. Nature 375: 754–760. St. George-Hyslop, P., J. Haines, E. Rogaev, M. Mortilla, G. Vaula, M. Pericak-Vance, J. F. Foncin, et al. 1992. Genetic evidence for a novel familial Alzheimer ’s disease locus on chromosome 14. Nat Genet 2: 330–334. Steiner, H., R. Fluhrer, and C. Haass. 2008. Intramembrane proteolysis by gammasecretase. J Biol Chem 283: 29627–29631. Strittmatter, W. J., A. M. Saunders, D. Schmechel, M. Pericak-Vance, J. Enghild, G. S. Salvesen, and A. D. Roses. 1993. Apolipoprotein E: High-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90: 1977–1981. Takeda, S., N. Sato, T. Ogihara, and R. Morishita. 2008. The renin-angiotensin system, hypertension and cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer ’s disease: New therapeutic potential. Front Biosci 13: 2253–2265. Tanzi, R. E. 1999. A genetic dichotomy model for the inheritance of Alzheimer ’s disease and common age-related disorders. J Clin Invest 104: 1175–1179. Tanzi, R. E., and L. Bertram. 2005. Twenty years of the Alzheimer ’s disease amyloid hypothesis: A genetic perspective. Cell 120: 545–555. Tanzi, R. E., J. F. Gusella, P. C. Watkins, G. A. Bruns, P. St. George-Hyslop, M. L. Van Keuren, D. Patterson, S. Pagan, D. M. Kurnit, and R. L. Neve. 1987. Amyloid beta protein gene: cDNA, mRNA distribution, and genetic linkage near the Alzheimer locus. Science 235: 880–884.
24
Dementia
Theuns, J., N. Brouwers, S. Engelborghs, K. Sleegers, V. Bogaerts, E. Corsmit, T. De Pooter, C. M. van Duijn, P. P. De Deyn, and C. Van Broeckhoven. 2006. Promoter mutations that increase amyloid precursor-protein expression are associated with Alzheimer disease. Am J Hum Genet 78: 936–946. Tucker, T., M. Marra, and J. M. Friedman. 2009. Massively parallel sequencing: The next big thing in genetic medicine. Am J Hum Genet 85: 142–154. Tycko, B., L. Feng, L. Nguyen, A. Francis, A. Hays, W. Y. Chung, M. X. Tang, et al. 1996. Polymorphisms in the human apolipoprotein-J/clusterin gene: Ethnic variation and distribution in Alzheimer ’s disease. Hum Genet 98: 430–446. Van Broeckhoven, C., H. Backhovens, M. Cruts, G. De Winter, M. Bruyland, P. Cras, and J. J. Martin. 1992. Mapping of a gene predisposing to earlyonset Alzheimer ’s disease to chromosome 14q24.3. Nat Genet 2: 335–339. Vance, J. E., and H. Hayashi. 2010. Formation and function of apolipoprotein E-containing lipoproteins in the nervous system. Biochim Biophys Acta 1801 (8): 806–818. Vinters, H. V., G. S. Nishimura, D. L. Secor, and W. M. Pardridge. 1990. Immunoreactive A4 and gamma-trace peptide colocalization in amyloidotic arteriolar lesions in brains of patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Pathol 137: 233–240. Wisniewski, K. E., A. J. Dalton, C. McLachlan, G. Y. Wen, and H. M. Wisniewski. 1985. Alzheimer ’s disease in Down’s syndrome: Clinicopathologic studies. Neurology 35: 957–961. Yamazaki, H., H. Bujo, J. Kusunoki, K. Seimiya, T. Kanaki, N. Morisaki, W. J. Schneider, and Y. Saito. 1996. Elements of neural adhesion molecules and a yeast vacuolar protein sorting receptor are present in a novel mammalian low density lipoprotein receptor family member. J Biol Chem 271: 24761–24768.
Chapter 2
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease: A Kinship Theory of Gene Conflict in Aging and Dementia Paul M. Butler and Patrick McNamara
In this chapter we review epigenetic factors that influence Parkinson’s disease pathophysiology and that might predict Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD). We emphasize effects of imprinted genes on developing catecholaminergic systems, mitochondrial functioning, and neurobehavioral functioning. We identify five key imprinted genes, 23 potentially imprinted genes (identified via bioinformatics modeling), five X-linked, mtDNA haplogroups, and seven PD-associated genes with loci in known imprinting centers that significantly influence PD pathophysiology. These analyses yield a major and novel prediction concerning effects of the DGK-theta gene on production of the etiopathogenesis of PD. We integrate these analyses into a theoretical framework of evolutionary theories of aging, intergenerational transfer of resources, and the kinship theory of intragenomic conflict. Our kinship theory of conflict in aging predicts that to increase inclusive fitness the matrilineal genome functions to extend life beyond reproductive capacity, whereas patrilineal genes antagonize the process of aging.
26
Dementia
Within this theoretical framework, PD can be understood as an aberrant side effect of epigenetic dysregulation over genes influencing aging, with PD susceptibility increasing with either full or partial loss-of-maternal or gain-of-paternal gene function. OVERVIEW OF THE CLINICAL AND NEUROPATHOLOGICAL HALLMARKS OF PD Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that disrupts proper functioning of neostriatal and mesocortical catecholaminergic systems (Lotharius and Brundin 2002). The best predictors of PD onset are age and sex. The average age of onset is 61 years and males are twice as likely as females to get PD (Standaert and Cantuti-Castelvetri 2008). The cardinal clinical features of PD are bradykinesia postural instability, rigidity, resting tremor, and/or gait disturbances (Hardy et al. 2006). Response to dopaminergic medication provides further support for the diagnosis of clinical PD (Litvan et al. 2007). Neurobehavioral features of PD include mood, social cognitive, and executive function deficits (McNamara, Durso, and Harris 2007, 2008; Williams-Gray et al. 2007; Huang et al. 2007). A shift in personality has also been noted with elderly (rather than early-onset) PD patients characterized as harm avoidant, overly conscientious, moralistic, ambitious, and low on novelty-seeking behaviors (McNamara, Durso, and Harris 2007, 2008). Between 30% and 80% of persons with PD are at risk for developing dementia as the disease progresses. The cholinesterase inhibitor rivastigmine has been shown to slow (but unfortunately not prevent) decline in selected cognitive functions and to reduce psychiatric disturbances (hallucinations or apathy) that often accompany dementia. Amantadine may increase the time from onset of Parkinson’s disease to dementia in persons at risk for dementia. One of the risk factors for dementia is signs of cognitive or thinking problems in early stages of the disease. If we can identify persons at high risk for dementia in PD we can offer these high-risk individuals early treatment with one of these drugs so as to slow down progression to dementia. Identifying new genetic risk factors is therefore of extreme clinical importance. The pathological features of Parkinson’s disease result primarily from the loss of dopaminergic neurons in the ventrolateral substantia nigra (Dauer and Przedborski 2003). The London Brain Bank criteria require the presence of Lewy bodies in the substantia nigra for the pathological diagnosis of PD. Of those individuals clinically diagnosed with PD, over 90%
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
27
that come to autopsy demonstrate Lewy body formation in substantia nigral tissue (Hughes et al. 1992). Lewy bodies are intracellular inclusion bodies of proteinaceous aggregates comprised mainly of alpha-synuclein. They are typically 15 micrometers in diameter, eosinophilic, and spherical. These intra-cytoplasmic bodies are composed of alpha synuclein, parkin, ubiquitin, and neurofilaments, and contain a dense hyaline core surrounded by a clear halo (Bras et al. 2008). Besides the substantia nigra, Lewy body formation can occur in the locus coeruleus, dorsal raphe, nucleus basalis of Meynert, dorsal motor nucleus of vagus, cingulate cortex, entorrhinal cortex, olfactory bulb, and autonomic nervous system (Francis and Perry 2007). The clinical correlates of dopamine (DA) dysfunction are best understood despite the involvement of other neurotransmitter systems, such as the noradrenergic and serotonergic systems. Breakdown of neurotransmitter systems other than DA are thought to occur in later stages of the condition and are perhaps age-related (Dauer and Przedborski 2003). GENETICS OF PARKINSON’S DISEASE In his 1817 monograph, James Parkinson suggested that the “shaking palsy” was due to stress (Parkinson 1817). Decades later, Gowers (1888) hypothesized a familial connection inherent to PD based on his observation that PD seemed to intermittently afflict members within extended families. Until the 1990s, a genetic component to PD vulnerability was largely dismissed. The first breakthrough came in the mid-1990s when Polymeropoulos et al. (1996, 1997) demonstrated genetic linkage to human chromosome 4 in the Contursi Greek/Italian kindred. The so-called PARK1 gene was localized to 4q21-23 and shown to code for the alpha-synuclein protein (SNCA). Albeit rare, numerous other mutations in SNCA were discovered subsequently (Athanassiadou et al. 1999; Bostantjopoulou et al. 2001; Kruger et al. 1998). Since these initial discoveries, researchers have turned to investigation of epigenetic factors to identify causal mechanisms of PD. The pathogenesis of PD is thought to relate to two fundamental processes: (1) misfolded and aggregate proteins lead to cell death in substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc) dopaminergic neurons, and (2) mitochondrial dysfunction and consequent oxidative stress induces toxic and oxidized DA species (Dauer and Przedborski 2003). These biochemically intertwined processes result in cells selectively targeted for programmed cell death. The connection between misfolded proteins, production of reactive oxygen species (ROS; e.g., superoxides O2–.), and apoptosis is only partially elucidated. Both genetic
28
Dementia
and toxin-induced models of PD have helped to illuminate several of the final common biochemical pathways leading to PD. Monogenic PD: Alpha-Synuclein, LRRK2, DJ-1, PINK1, and Parkin Overproduction of SNCA is directly related to age-of-onset of PD. The greater the accumulation of alpha-synuclein protein beyond what is necessary for normal cellular function, the earlier PD-type symptoms emerge (Ross et al. 2008). The relationship between levels of excess alpha synuclein and cell death is dose-sensitive. Alpha-synuclein is part of a family of related proteins expressed in the central nervous system. SNCA is concentrated in presynaptic nerve terminals (Jakes, Spillantini, and Goedert 1994). Evidence suggests it plays a prominent role in synaptic plasticity through its facilitation of vesicular release of neurotransmitters (Liu et al. 2004). Alpha-synuclein binds lipid membranes, which alters the conformation of a previously unfolded N-terminus into an alpha-helix. This allegedly stabilizes curvature of lipid membranes, thereby modulating neurotransmitter-holding vesicular function (Lotharius and Brundin 2002). Evidence suggests that SNCA aggregation is selectively cytotoxic in DA-ergic neurons (Xu et al. 2002). Through interaction with ROS, oligomeric accumulation of alpha-synuclein in human dopamine neurons leads to apoptosis. Non-DA-ergic neurons do not exhibit the same sensitivity to oligomeric alpha-synuclein prompted cytotoxicity. Xu et al. (2002) showed that dopamine-related cytotoxicity is mediated by 54-83-kD soluble protein aggregates that contain alpha-synuclein and a specific 14-3-3 protein. Substantia nigra cells in PD patients selectively express elevated levels of these proteins. Aggresomes of soluble alpha-synuclein and 14-3-3 protein complexes render endogenous intracellular dopamine toxic. This evidence points to a mechanism to explain the tissue selectivity of neuronal loss in PD. It is well established that aggregated alpha-synuclein containing inclusion bodies (Lewy bodies) form lesions that result in the pathologic features of many neuro-degenerative diseases. Several cellular scenarios can lead to the accumulation of alpha-synuclein aggregates: (a) gene mutations that increase alpha synuclein production, (b) gene mutations coding for proteins involved in degrading and turning over misfolded proteins (e.g., parkin/ubiquitin), and (c) exposure to ROS or nitration, which can lead to alpha-synuclein misfolding and aggregation (Giasson et al. 2000). Levels of oxidized proteins increase with age, and DA metabolism generates ROS (Xu et al. 2002). Chaperones (e.g., heat shock protein 70) target proteins for proteasomal degradation by polyubiquination. This process
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
29
becomes less efficient in aging. So, a deleterious feedback cycle emerges in normal aging—an inevitable decrease in efficiency in cell handling of misfolded proteins, which subsequently increases the interaction time with ROS giving way to more misfolded proteins and eventual sequestration and inclusion body formation. Indeed, Lewy body formation and neuronal loss in subcortical areas is apparent in nondemented older adults based on neuropathological cohort studies (Byford et al. 2009). Critical cellular regulation of ROS is under dynamic control by mitochondrial homeostatic mechanisms. During acute states of elevated ROS levels, prolonged periods of mild ROS elevation, or loss of efficacious enzymatic machinery to deal with ROS, mitochondrial homeostasis can become disturbed leading to fission and spilling of ROS and pro-apoptotic modulators (e.g., cytochrome c) into the cytoplasm. Release of cyctochrome c from mitochondrial cristae induces alpha-synuclein aggregation (Chu 2009). Further, cytochrome c is a potentiator of the apoptotic intrinsic pathway (Skulachev 1998). Correlates of apoptosis include Bax, Caspase-8 and -9, and are all found in higher levels in SNpc neurons in post-mortem tissue samples of PD patients (Ho et al. 2009). Caspase-8 is located in deafferented presynaptic terminals and once activated might lead to a retrograde “dying back” of DA neurons in the striatum (Bernheimer et al. 1973). Abnormalities in Complex I of the mitochondrial electron transport chain are also found in PD neurons and peripheral platelet tissue samples (Betarbet et al. 2006). Both complex I and III of the transport chain are key generators of ROS during oxidative phosphorylation. Abnormalities in handling ROS lead to compensatory mitochondrial fusion mechanisms (Chu 2009). When these compensatory systems cannot handle the ROS load, and protective mitochondrial fusion fails, this can lead to mitochondrial fission with subsequent spilling of bioactive and pro-apoptotic chemicals into the cytoplasm. Mitochondrial fission-fusion dynamics are essential then for controlling ROS, cytotoxicity, and apoptosis. Besides increase in intracellular ROS and alpha-synuclein overproduction, dysregulation in other cellular machinery can lead to PD. Mutations in the leucine-rich repeat kinase 2 gene (LRRK2/PARK8) are believed to cause between 2% and 40% (depending on the population) of PD cases (Bonifati 2006). All of the functions of LRRK2 are not understood. It is a large, multidomain protein with GTPase kinase activity, and it associates with mitochondrial outer membranes and lysosomal vesicles (Marin 2006). Several lines of research have illuminated the role mutations in LRRK2 play in PD histopathology. For instance, LRRK2 might link tau and alphasynuclein misfolding through phosphorylation of alpha-synuclein. Tau and alpha-synuclein can bind one another and stimulate phosphorylation
30
Dementia
and aggregation of homopolymers, which eventually become cytotoxic (Devine and Lewis 2008). The most common LRRK2 mutation, the G2019S, causes an increase in autophosphorylation and kinase activity (Saha et al. 2009). Hyperphosphorylated alpha-synuclein aggregates comprise PD-associated Lewy bodies. Also, LRRK2 interacts with death receptor Fas-associated protein with death domain (FADD). LRRK2 inhibits FADD and caspase-8 (extrinsic death pathway). Mutations increase interaction with FADD and increase cell death via caspase 8 recruitment (Ho et al. 2009). Allegedly then, deviation in LRKK2 function yields PD patterns of neuronal brain damage because of its influence over phosphorylation of alpha-synuclein and tau, and control of the extrinsic apoptotic pathway. PARK7 (DJ-1) mutations are loss-of-function alterations associated with early-onset PD (Bonifati et al. 2006; Taira et al. 2004). Experimental data with drug-induced PD models demonstrate that rotenone disturbs mitochondrial function, which results in aberrant DJ-1 and alpha-synuclein regulation (Betarbet et al. 2006). DJ-1 is thought to localize to mitochondria during times of oxidative stress in order to serve as a protective redox sensor. PINK1 (PARK6) codes for PTEN-induced kinase 1, which contains an N-terminal localization signal for the mitochondria (Valente et al. 2004). Research demonstrates that PINK1 protects against proteasome-mediated apoptosis in neuronal cell populations (Chu 2009; Dagda et al. 2009). The proposed mechanisms include PINK1 protecting against LRRK2 neurite injury, promotion of mitochondrial fusion as a protective mechanism against oxidative stress, and maintenance of cristae membrane structure and Ca2+ balance. PINK1 deficiency leads to abnormal cristae structure and membrane potential, mitochondrial calcium dysregulation, and oxidative stress. This is believed to increase mitochondrial fragmentation via a perturbation of fission-fusion homeostasis. Parkin (PARK2) codes for an ubiquitin-associated protein that leads to early-onset PD with loss-of-function mutations (Sun et al. 2006). Parkin is an ubiquitin-protein ligase involved in the polyubiquination pathways that tag proteins for degradation in proteasomes (Shimura et al. 2000). Regulating protein turnover, namely alpha-synuclein, is of homeostatic importance to prevent cytotoxicity. Inability to recycle misfolded proteins increases exposure time to ROS, which can induce protein aggregation and ultimately cytotoxicity and apoptosis. Before moving on to discuss the role of epigenetics (namely gene imprinting) in PD risk, it is important to note that preliminary evidence suggests involvement of epigenetic dysregulation, such as hypomethylation of DNA, in PD susceptibility (Urdinguio, Sanchez-Mut, and Esteller 2009).
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
31
SNCA can translocate to the nucleus and associate with histones, modifying the acetylation process (Kontopoulos, Parvin, and Feany 2006). SNCAmediated toxicity is thought to interrupt a major regulator of aging, sirtuin 2 (Outeiro et al. 2007).
GENOMIC IMPRINTING AND PD PD Susceptibility, a “Multi-Hit” Hypothesis, and Functional Haploidy The pathways leading to PD susceptibility over time are likely influenced by the function of several key proteins. An increased risk for developing PD could emerge given a hypothetical individual with increased ROS due to normal aging, high intrinsic DA turnover, and a SNP polymorphism in a polyubiquinator important to DA-ergic neuronal function that delays protein recycling. Much like Knudson’s “multi-hit” hypothesis for cancer risk (Knudson 1971), there are likely many subtle changes in numerous aspects of cellular machinery that enhance risk of PD. Subtle change in protein function at several key bioregulatory nodes, rather than complete loss-of-function mutations, can increase the probability of PDrelated brain damage accumulating over time. Knudsen’s “multi-hit” hypothesis applied to PD susceptibility refers to both overt mutational events leading to loss- or gain-in-function protein expression patterns and polymorphism-induced alterations in expression levels. A recent metabolomics study by Gieger et al. (2008) demonstrated that a single nucleotide polymorphism in the PARK2 (parkin) gene was one of five key proteins to dramatically alter concentrations of many core amino acids measured in human serum. While the complete loss-of-function mutation in parkin results in juvenile PD, one can imagine the impact of a functional polymorphism in parkin in conjunction with changes in function of several other vital enzymatic regulators. Indeed, subtle changes in the function of key bioregulatory nodes increase risk of PD. For example, polymorphisms in key mtDNA genes can lead to either a protection from, or vulnerability to PD as a function of age (Pyle et al. 2005; Wild and Dikic 2010). With the “multi-hit: framework in mind, functionally haploid gene systems are increasingly vulnerable as loci for disease susceptibility from a probabilistic perspective because haploid gene expression cannot rely on compensatory mechanisms of biallelic gene dosage. For males, X-linked gene expression is functionally haploid and with over 90% of mtDNA being derived from a primary oocyte, functional haploidy generally applies to mtDNA-associated risk (Giles et al. 1980). Epigenetic processes,
32
Dementia
such as genomic imprinting, represents another type of functional haploidy currently neglected in studies of PD genetic risk. Genomic imprinting refers to the epigenetic silencing of alleles in an individual according to the parent-of-origin of the allele (Burt and Trivers 2006; Haig 2004). Classic imprinted gene mechanisms (complete silencing of either paternally or maternally received alleles) results in functional haploid gene expression such that mutant alleles are exposed to more direct selection. If alteration in imprinting mechanisms occurs, then this functional haploidy increases the risk for complete loss-of-function or extreme gain-of-function (Wilkins and Haig 2003, “What good”; Ubeda and Wilkins 2008). Imprinted genes often regulate growth and development such that alteration in gene expression can amplify dysfunction in cellular proliferation, apoptosis, or differentiation. PD Susceptibility, Gene Imprinting, and the Kinship Theory of Genomic Imprinting Apart from functional haploid gene expression increasing likelihood for mutational or dysregulatory events across time, several other lines of research suggest that gene imprinting is likely involved in PD susceptibility. Evidence is accumulating that epigenetic approaches are crucial to understand many neuropsychiatric conditions and neurological disorders that, like PD, involve a diathesis stress etiological model of exposure (e.g. to an environmental toxin) with underlying genetic risk (Abdolmaleky, Thiagalingam, and Wilcox 2005; Bertram et al. 2000; Lamb et al. 2005; Mayeux et al. 2002). Many of the genes known to be imprinted are expressed in the brain (Davies, Isles, and Wilkinson 2005). The well-described syndromes of gene imprinting dysfunction, which include Prader-Willi, Angelman’s, Beckwith-Widemann, and Silver-Russell syndromes (dysregulated imprinting at 15q11-13, 11p15.5, 7p11.2, or 5q35), greatly alter behavior and cognition (Davies, Isles, and Wilkinson 2001; Eliot and Maher 1994; Flint 1992; Whittington et al. 2004). An overwhelming amount of evidence connects genomic imprinting with brain development, modulation of behavior, and neuropsychotic spectrum disorders and neurodegenerative disease, including Alzheimer ’s disease, autism, epilepsy, schizophrenia, ADHD, bipolar disorder, and Huntington’s disease (Bassett, Avramopoulos, and Fallin 2002; Crespi 2008; Goos and Silverman 2006; Lamb et al. 2005; Ottman et al. 1988; Wilkinson, Davies, and Isles 2007). The kinship theory of genomic imprinting suggests that genes are selected for expression or silencing depending on their parent-of-origin and their effects on the growth and fitness of mothers, their progeny, and
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
33
other genetically asymmetrical kin (Day and Bonduriansky 2004; Haig 1997; Haig 2000, “Genomic imprinting”; Wilkins and Haig 2003, “What good”). Genes that enhance offspring fitness at a cost to the mother and other maternal kin will more likely be paternally expressed and maternally silenced through nucleotide methylation, posttranslational histone modification, or noncoding RNA (ncRNA) mechanisms. Contrarily, maternally expressed genes will likely restrain extraction of maternal resources by offspring (e.g., restrain placental growth) and silence paternal alleles. As predicted by Haig’s theory of intragenomic conflict, numerous imprinted genes have been discovered across differing mammalian species that can be understood to express this genetic tug-of-war over maternal resources and growth of offspring. From this theoretical perspective intragenomic conflict is essentially about a struggle over the pace and bio-flow of energy resources. After presenting evidence for the presumed involvement of imprinted genes in PD susceptibility, we draw from the kinship theory of intragenomic conflict to address the evolutionary dynamics behind this connection. Methodological Approach The list of known human imprinted genes is constantly growing. Bioinformatics models have enhanced our ability to predict novel loci. Algorithms designed to identify differences between imprinted and nonimprinted genes have been powerful tools in the effort to locate imprinted loci and genes. A methodological caveat is important to note at this point. The genes discussed as either maternally or paternally imprinted in the discussion to follow do not take into full consideration possibilities of more complex imprinting patterns, such as partial imprinting, in which paternal and maternal allele expression differ by degree (Morison, Ramsay, and Spencer 2005; Wolf et al. 2008). Imprinted genes may express a parentof-origin effect selectively in certain tissue (e.g., hippocampal neurons vs. cardiac myocytes) or cell lineages, at particular developmental stages (e.g., fetal vs. postnatal weaning), or exhibit pleiotrophic effects via polymorphic imprinted states, reciprocal patterning, multiple splice variants, etc. (Buetner et al. 2004; Kishino 2006; Sandovici et al. 2003; Weinstein 2001). Biochemical mechanisms to underlie gene imprinting can be quite diverse—differential methylation of DNA elements at imprinting clusters inducing an enhancer or blocker effect on allele expression by parent of origin, cis-mediated silencing of a given allele by a noncoding RNA element, or via methylation of micro-imprinted domains of promoter regions
34
Dementia
in oocyte-derived genetic material (Wood and Oakey 2006). Also, complex imprinting patterns of bipolar homozygous or heterozygous bipolar dominance and callipgye-type expression patterns exist (Kim et al. 2004; Wolf et al. 2008). The mechanisms of gene imprinting are just beginning to be understood in their full complexity. Take, for example, imprinted noncoding RNA—ncRNA genes are a group (e.g., Piwi-interacting RNAs, microRNAs, and C/D small nucleolar RNAs) of untranslated transcripts that are expressed in a parent-of-origin pattern, which can function as cisacting silencers of chromatin at imprinting centers or as anti-sense transacting gene expression regulators. ncRNA imprinting has already been implicated in Prader-Willi syndrome, several types of cancers, and neural plasticity (Royo and Cavaille 2008). Another important note of caution is that while many of the genes discussed in the sections to follow are known to be imprinted in humans, the genes we draw upon from bioinformatics predictions (Luedi et al. 2007) will need empirical validation before full inclusion in this model of PD risk is warranted. Empirical investigations of bioinformatically identified imprinted genes have yielded mixed results (Pollard et al. 2008; Ruf et al. 2007; Wolf et al. 2008). These partial failures to empirically confirm bioinformatically identified genes may be due to several factors: (1) not all major tissue types were assessed for imprinting, (2) varying developmental stages were not sampled, (3) gender effects were usually not addressed, and (4) human tissue was not tested. Despite the impressive cross-validation check of the linear algorithm used in many bioinformatics models, the false positive rates of prediction from the Luedi et al. (2007) bioinformatics method will need future empirical confirmation. Whether or not the predictions of novel imprinted genes by bioinformatics end up fully or partially accurate, the evidence we present here (not entirely dependent on prediction models) suggests that genetic imprinting plays a significant role in the pathophysiology of PD.
IMPRINTED GENES AND DEVELOPING MIDBRAIN DA SYSTEMS FOXA2: Forkhead Box Protein A2; Maternally Imprinted Foxa2 is a member of a larger class of proteins that serve as transcriptional factors vital for prenatal development of various tissue types, cell survival, and longevity (Greer and Brunet 2005; Tothova and Gilliland 2007). Foxa2 is located at human chromosome 20p11.21 and is predicted to be maternally imprinted (Luedi et al. 2007). Foxa2 is a forkhead transcription factor known to play a critical role in the development of endoderm,
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
35
midline structures, notochord, and floor plate (Ang and Rossant 1994; Epstein, McMahon, and Joyner 1999; Sasaki and Hogan 1994). Using a mouse model, Kittappa et al. (2007) demonstrated that the foxa2 gene controls both the birth and death of dopaminergic neurons in old age. They showed that midbrain dopaminergic neurons are derived from the floor plate under the tight regulation of the Foxa2 gene. Further, mice deficient in foxa2 late in life developed motor problems evidenced histopathologically by selective neuronal loss in the substantia nigra. This study is of particular importance because of its use of a complete lifespan model. Most models of PD currently focus on inducing abnormalities via toxin exposure in animals after assumed normal development of dopaminergic systems. Assessing the overall growth, maintenance, and trajectory of dopaminergic midbrain neurons seems an improvement in scope and applicability. LMX1B: LIM Homeobox Transcription Factor 1, Beta; Maternally Imprinted Foxa2 gene is an important, albeit not exhaustive, regulator of mesencephalic dopaminergic neurodevelopment. Other important contributions are made by numerous genes, including but not limited to sonic hedgehog (SHH), LIM homeobox transcription factor 1 alpha and beta (LIM1A and LIM1B), fibroblast growth factor 8 (Fgf8), paired-like homeodomain transcription factor 3 (Pitx3), bicoid class homeobox transcription factor 1 and 2 (Otx1 and Otx 2), and the proneural basic helix-loop-helix transcription factor neurogenin 2 (Ngn2) (Bergman et al. 2009; Fuchs et al. 2009; Lin et al. 2009). Many of these genes crucial for dopaminergic system building and maintenance are predicted with high confidence to be imprinted: maternally imprinted Foxa2 at 20p11.21, paternally imprinted SHH at 7q36.3, maternally imprinted LMX1B at 9q33.3, and maternally imprinted Otx1 at 2p15 (see Smidt and Burbach 2007 for review of genes; Luedi et al. 2007). Table 2.1 contains an extended list of the imprinted genes (most predicted) with known protein products that exert a neurotrophic effect on mesodiencephalic DA neurons. Both LMX1A and LMX1B are important for proper differentiation of midbrain dopamine neuron birth and development (Alavian, Scholz, and Simon 2008; Smidt et al. 2000). Bergman et al. (2009) assessed SNPs in both LIM homeobox genes and PD susceptibility in 357 Swedish PD subjects. Three SNPs in LMX1A and one in LMX1B were associated with PD. Additionally, the risk for PD by gender varied by SNP type. Lin et al. (2009) investigated the relationship between LMX1B and Foxa2 in development of mesodiencephalic dopaminergic neurons. Their
36
Dementia
Table 2.1 Imprinted Genes Predicted by Bioinformatics Models (Luedi et al. 2007) with Known Protein Products with Trophic Effects on Embryonic Mesodiencephalic DA Neurons Locus (gene)
Chromosomal locus
GDNF
5p13.2
Neurod2 Pitx2
17q12 4q25
Pitx1
5q31.1
GFRA4
20p13
BMP1
8p21.3
BMP8
1p34.3
EGFL3/MEGF6
1p36.33
EGFL7
9q34.33
SHH FGFR3
7q36.3 4p16.3
BMP4
14q22.2
Foxa2 LMX1B
20p11.21 9q33.3
Otx1
2p15
Encoded protein Glial cell linederived neurotrophic factor Neurogenin 2 Paired-like homedomain transcription factor 2 Paired-like homedomain transcription factor 1 GDNF family receptor alpha-4 receptor Bone morphogenetic protein 1 Bone morphogenetic protein 8 Multiple epidermal growth factorlike domain 6 Epidermal growth factor-like 7; vascular endothelial statin Sonic hedgehog Fibroblast growth factor receptor 3 Bone morphogenetic protein 4; BMP2B1 Forkhead box A2 LIM homeobox transcription factor 1, beta Orthodenticle 1, homolog of drosophila
Mode of inheritance Paternal
Paternal Paternal
Paternal
Paternal
Paternal Paternal Paternal
Paternal
Paternal Maternal Maternal
Maternal Maternal
Maternal
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
37
Table 2.1 (Continued) Pax6 CDKNN1C
11p13 11p15.5
TFAP2A
6p24.3
Paired box gene 6 Cyclin dependent kinase inhibitor 1C Transcription factor AP 2 alpha reg
Maternal Maternal* Maternal
* Denotes known imprinted gene
research revealed that Foxa2 functioned as an upstream modulator of LMX1B positively modulating gene expression and dopamine neuron development. The study also demonstrated that tyrosine hydroxylase (TH; maternally imprinted) is a target for Foxa2. The cooperative, feedforward modulation between Foxa2, TH, and LMX1B is interesting given that both are known and predicted to be maternally imprinted genes, respectively. The interactions between these imprinted genes promoting DA-ergic midbrain neuron development involve cross-regulation from other imprinting centers elsewhere in the genome. For instance, the imprinting zone at 11p15.5, which includes TH, IGF2, and H19, becomes activated during dopaminergic cell differentiation (Freed et al. 2008). Freed and colleagues examined gene expression in human embryonic stem cells through the process of cell differentiation into dopamine neurons. A number of genes previously known to be correlated with dopamine neuron development were identified, such as MSX1, CDKN1C, Pitx1, and Pitx2, in addition to several novel loci, which covered the 11p15.5 imprinting center expressing genes H19, TSSC4, TH, and IGF2. Other Imprinted Genes and DA System Control Table 2.1 displays several imprinted genes (mostly predicted) that contribute to development and longevity of midbrain dopaminergic neurons. While the initial growth and establishment of the DA system receives greater paternal genetic contributions, the control of programmed DA cell death seems to be largely under maternal control. From Brainstem Building to Catecholamine Regulation—AP-2α (TFAP2A), Transcription Factor AP-2 alpha, Maternally Imprinted Apparent from the above discussion regarding the role of genetic elements in building dopaminergic pathways in the midbrain, the impact of
38
Dementia
differential gene expression can extend into adulthood and aging (e.g., programmed DA cell death). Transcription factor AP-2 alpha (AP-2α) is an example of a gene with substantial regulatory control of brainstem development and continued impact across development. Activating enhancer-binding protein 2 contains a family of transcription factors with five isoforms described to date: AP-2α, AP-2β, AP-2γ, AP-2δ, and AP-2ε (Eckert et al. 2005; Hensch et al. 2008). The AP-2α and AP-2β isoforms have been shown to regulate gene expression during embryonic development of neural crest cell lineages and neuroectodermal cells, including those in the midbrain (Moser, Ruschoff, and Buetnner 1997). Extending into mammalian adulthood, AP-2α expression specifically correlates with levels of catecholamine metabolites in the frontal cortex and midbrain (e.g., DOPAC, HVA, 5-HIAA; Damberg et al. 2001). Additionally, transcription of AP-2α and AP-2β seems necessary to prevent cell specific activation of apoptotic pathways (Brewer et al. 2004; Moser et al. 1997). Thus, in addition to its important neurodevelopmental role, AP-2α is an important determinant for maintenance, functional characteristics, and regulation of target catecholamine gene expression. Several catecholamine-related genes involved in CNS neurotransmitter function contain AP-2 binding sites in their regulatory regions, for example, DβH, AADC, 5-HT receptor 2A, and TH (Du et al. 1994; Greco et al. 1995; Hahn et al. 1993; Kim et al. 2001; Kobayashi et al. 1989; McMahon and Sabban 1992). AP-2α, located at human chromosome 6p24.2, is predicted to be maternally imprinted (Luedi et al. 2007). Because this gene interacts with other catecholaminergic regulatory genes that are known to be imprinted (see next section) the confidence level in the imprinting status of AP-2α is increased. Transcriptional activation mediated by AP-2 can be induced by two different signal transduction pathways: the phorbol ester and diacylglycerol-activated protein kinase C or the cAMP-dependent protein kinase A pathways (see DGK-theta section below for a discussion on the potential role of DGK-theta in PD etiopathogenesis). IMPRINTED GENES AND CATECHOLAMINE METABOLISM Figure 2.1 depicts an overview of the intragenomic conflict over important regulators of catecholamine metabolism. The discussion below details more specifically the biochemical role of each contributor. TH: Tyrosine Hydroxylase; Maternally Imprinted TH is the rate-limiting catalyst in the conversion of L-tyrosine to L-DOPA. It is vital to the production of catecholamines. Four different types of TH are
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
39
Figure 2.1
Conflict over regulation and gene expression in the catecholamine system: depiction of metabolic pathways with imprinted genes. ** denotes ratelimiting step in DA metabolism, and * denotes X-linked gene. TH – tyrosine hydroxylase; L-DOPA – L-3,4-dihydroxyphenylalanine; DDC – DOPA decarboxylase; DβH – dopamine beta-hydroxylase; MAO-B – monamine oxidase type B; COMT – catechol-O-methyltransferase; 5-HT – serotonin; DA – dopamine; DRD4 – dopamine receptor 4; 5HTR2A – serotonin receptor type 2A; DOPAC – 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acide; HVA – homovanillic acid; 5-HIIA – 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid; ♀ – female imprinted gene; ♂ – male imprinted gene. Note that all genes are known imprinted human genes, except AP-2α and DβH, which are both predicted to be maternally imprinted genes by bioinformatics models (Luedi et al. 2007).
expressed in the human brain and adrenal medulla (Grima et al. 1987). In the brain, TH is most highly concentrated in the substantia nigra and locus coeruleus. Various TH isoforms exist to contribute to the functional diversity of TH. One mechanism is alternative splicing of TH mRNA, which is implicated in some neurologic diseases (Nagatsu and Ichinose 1991). Polymorphisms in the TH gene, a maternally imprinted gene located at 11p15.5, are associated with male longevity (De Benedictis et al. 1998).
40
Dementia
TH is also an independent marker for size at birth and obesity. Zhou et al. (1995) showed that dopamine-deficient mice become hypoactive after birth and stop feeding. Treatment with L-DOPA resolves these behavioral shortcomings within minutes. Continued treatment with L-DOPA leads to normal growth, movement, and feeding. TH gene mutations have been implicated in PD (Ludecke, Dworniczak, and Bartholome 1995; Swaans et al. 2000). Most evidence suggests that TH gene mutations causing significant loss of enzymatic function are related to infantile or juvenile onset of parkinsonism. L-DOPA medication was very effective at managing these cases. More recently, haplotypes associated the IGF2-INS-TH cluster at 11p15.5 were associated with both risk of and protection from PD (Sutherland et al. 2008). DBH: Dopamine Beta-Hydroxylase; Maternally Imprinted DBH is a 290-kDa copper containing oxygenase that metabolizes dopamine to produce norepinephrine (Rush and Geffen 1980). Located at 9q34.2, DBH is predicted to be a maternally imprinted gene (Luedi et al. 2007). Healy et al. (2004) demonstrated in a study of 809 PD patients that the DBH SNP polymorphism -1021 T/T genotype was underrepresented in PD’s versus controls. These results suggest some type of protective effect in the T/T SNP against PD. DBH gene promoter polymorphism -1021 C/T appears to regulate plasma DBH activity. A T/T SNP at -1021 lowers DBH activity, consequently increasing serum dopamine levels (Zabetian et al. 2001). Lower levels of DBH activity (-1021 T/T genotype) have a neuroprotective effect against PD. DDC: DOPA Decarboxylase; Paternally Imprinted DDC, generally known as aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase (AAAD), is a homodimeric, lyase enzyme responsible for synthesizing dopamine and serotonin (Ichinose et al. 1989). The biosynthetic pathway for dopamine involves the dietary ingestion of the essential amino acid phenylalanine and subsequent conversion into L-tyrosine by the enzyme phenylalanine hydroxylase. The rate-limiting step is controlled by the maternally imprinted tyrosine hydroxylase, which converts L-tyrosine to L-DOPA. In the periphery and in the central nervous system AAAD catabolizes L-DOPA to dopamine. Dopamine is found in numerous areas of the brain with high concentrations in the basal ganglia (Hyland et al. 1992; Pons et al. 2004). AAAD is not the rate-limiting step in dopamine and serotonin synthesis. AAAD becomes the rate-limiting enzyme in individuals treated
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
41
with L-DOPA or 5-HT for conditions, such as PD (LeWitt 2008). Tehranian et al. (2006) discovered molecular interaction between alpha-synuclein and AAAD. Dopaminergic cells overexpressing alpha-synuclein display reduced AAAD and TH activity. Alpha-synuclein acted to diminish phosphorylation and activity of both TH and AAAD. This evidence furthered the argument that alpha-synuclein is directly involved in dopamine synthesis and metabolism. DeLuca et al. (2003) showed that AAAD polymorphisms (to attenuate paternal allele expression/function) contribute to longevity in drosophila. Also, polymorphisms in tyrosine hydroxylase are implicated in longevity in C. elegans species (DeLuca et al. 2001). Menheniott et al. (2008) demonstrated genomic imprinting in the DDC gene with a paternal pattern of expression. The neighboring Grb10 (see section below) displays a reciprocal maternal imprinting pattern. Perturbations in the multifunctional enzyme DDC have been reported for a range of neurodegenerative and psychiatric conditions, such as Parkinson’s disease and bipolar disorder (Borglum et al. 2003). Ishikawa et al. (2009) discovered an interaction between DDC, TH, and DJ-1. In human dopaminergic neurons DJ-1 directly bound to TH and DDC and positively regulated their activities. Mutant forms of DJ-1 possessing Cystein-106 with SO(2)H and SO(3)H types inactivated the regulatory control that DJ-1 maintained over TH and DDC. This interesting finding indicates an essential role that DJ-1 might have in DA metabolism. While the imprinting status of DJ-1 (PARK7) is not currently known, its locus at 1p36.3 is surrounded by over 30 alleged imprinted genes, which are mostly predicted to be maternally imprinted (Luedi et al. 2007). While many genes in imprinting centers are impacted by imprint mechanisms, one cannot assume the effects of imprinting by mere proximity. However, loci within imprinting centers do raise greater suspicion regarding imprinting status. 5-HTR2A: Serotonin Receptor, 2A; Maternally Imprinted In PD there can be extensive loss of serotonergic function due to loss of neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus (Francis and Perry 2007). Loss of serotonergic terminal boutons can be compromised by 40–50% in projections to the frontal and temporal cortex and putamen (D’Amato et al. 1987). Because PD primarily impacts DA-ergic neurons, other neurotransmitters systems have been lesser studied in relation to PD susceptibility. One investigation demonstrated a significant correlation between sporadic PD and a SNP (rs6311) in the 5-HTR2A gene in a Russian population (Shadrina et al. 2008).
42
Dementia
Conflicting data exists regarding the imprinting status of the 5-HTRA2 gene (Bunzel et al. 1998; Fukuda et al. 2006). However, sufficient evidence exists to demonstrate that gene expression varies across tissue type and developmental stage with a predominately monoallelic maternal expression pattern. Allelic variation has been repeatedly confirmed using real-time quantitative polymerase chain reaction techniques, despite the fact that 5-HTR2A at 13q14 is not located in a known imprinting center (Lo et al. 2003). The current data on 5-HTR2A gene imprinting in humans demonstrates that gene expression between alleles (paternal or maternal) can change across time, tissue type, and perhaps via epigenetic idiosyncrasies dependent on individuals’ internal milieu and environmental exposures.
IMPRINTED GENES AND MITOCHONDRIAL FUNCTION NDUFS4: NADH-Ubiquinone Oxidoreductase Fe-S Protein 4; Paternally Imprinted Complex I, or NADH ubiquinone oxidoreductase, is a multimeric protein comprised of at least 41 subunits. Seven subunits are controlled by the mitochondrial genome while the remaining 34 are under nuclear genomic control. Complex I is a critical component of the mitochondrial electron transport chain and essential to subunit assembly (Lazarou et al. 2007). In properly functioning mitochondria, Complex I removes electrons from NADH and passes them to ubiquinone via a chain of protein-associated redox controllers. This process begins an ensemble of mitochondrial actions that is pivotal to ATP formation (van den Heuvel et al. 1998). One important subunit vital to the function of NADH oxidation is NADH-ubiquinone oxidoreductase Fe-S Protein 4 (NDUFS4). Proper functioning of NDUFS4 is essential to the machinations of Complex I because of its association with the iron-sulfur clusters, which mediate electron transfer. Feng, Bussiere, and Hekimi (2001) demonstrated that inhibition or mutation (loss of function) at the Fe-S subunits of the mitochondrial transport chain complexes in C. elegans increased life span. Evidence suggests that mutations in the NDUFS4 gene lead to Complex I deficiencies (Ugalde et al. 2004; van den Heuvel et al. 1998) and is implicated in neurologic disease (Hekimi and Guarente 2003; Papa et al. 2009). Severe Complex I deficiencies can lead to Leigh syndrome, which is an early onset neurodegenerative disorder with characteristic neuropathology consisting of brain stem, basal ganglia, cerebellar, and thalamic focal lesions (Dahl 1998; David,
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
43
Gomez, and Okazaki 1970). Indeed, mutations specific to NDUFS4 have been described to induce Leigh syndrome (Budde et al. 2000). Dysfunctional NDUFS4 decreases Complex I function in addition to Complex III function through an unknown mechanism. Important to note, complex I and III are potent generators of ROS (Murphy 2009). NDUFS4 is located at 5q11.1 (Emahazion et al. 1998) and is predicted to be paternally imprinted (Luedi et al. 2007). NDUFA4: NADH-Ubiquinone Oxidoreductase Protein A, 4; Paternally Imprinted Another important gene coding for a functionally important subunit of Complex I is NDUFA4. Located at 1p13.3, this gene is predicted to be paternally imprinted as with the previously discussed NDUFS4 (Luedi et al. 2007). Without traversing territory already explored, suffice it to say that Complex I mitochondrial function is vital for understanding PD susceptibility. While many of the Complex I–associated genes have been investigated as relevant to PD, NDUFA4 is of particular interest for the following reasons. First, the hydrophobicity profile demonstrates that this protein is an inner mitochondrial membrane spanning subunit vital for electron transfer dynamics (Lazarou et al. 2007). Next, recent work by Hayashi et al. (2009) demonstrates a clear chemical interaction between DJ-1 and NDUFA4. In their study, Hayashi et al. found that DJ-1 bound directly to NDUFA4 and ND-1, nuclear and mitochondrial DNA-encoding subunits of Complex I. In DJ-1 knockdown models, complex I activity was reduced. Mitochondrial Genes Contributing to Complex I Function; Maternal Control As previously mentioned, seven mitochondrial genes contribute to Complex I function (mt-nd1, mt-nd2, mt-nd3, mt-nd4, mt-nd4l, mt-nd5, and mt-nd6) in addition to the gene for coenzyme Q, or complex III, function (mt-cyb). An X-linked gene, NDUFA1 (at X24), is important for Complex I function. As stated previously, Complex I and III are the most important generators of ROS production. MtDNA controls the important pro-apototic retaining complex IV, cytochrome c oxidase (mt-co1, ct-co2, and mt-co3). An important struggle over mitochondrial function is demonstrated in these examples of paternally imprinted nuclear and maternally controlled x-linked and mtDNA-associated genetic loci. Elstner et al. (2009) presented evidence for dysfunction in NADH-ubiquinone oxidoreductase, subunit 2 (mt-nd2) is a major risk factor for PD. Also, studies
44
Dementia
have correlated several polymorphic changes in mtDNA (e.g., haplotype cluster UKJT) with decreased risk for PD with a potential gender effect (Gaweda-Walerych et al. 2008; Pyle et al. 2005). Luoma et al. (2004) demonstrated a connection between parkinsonism and mitochondrial DNA polymerase γ (POLG) mutations in a clinical and molecular genetics study. Mutations in POLG, an enzyme that synthesizes all mtDNA, were found in all of the familial PD cases studied. In a study of a multigenerational PD family, Swerdlow et al. (1998) showed maternally inherited complex I gene dysfunction from mtDNA in all PDaffected members. Compared to cell lines cultured with paternal mtDNA, cybrid lines containing maternal mtDNA from this affected multigenerational family exhibited lower complex I activity, increased ROS production, increased radical scavenging enzymatic activity, and higher numbers of abnormally shaped mitochondria. These findings were present in all asymptomatic young maternal descendants in the familial study—note the differential impact by sex. Thus, while female mitochondrial function was affected, males were more severely impacted, allegedly because an equilibrium-maintenance or homeostatic line of compensatory protective mechanistic processes was crossed.
IMPRINTED GENES AND LEWY BODY FORMATION GATA2: GATA-Binding Protein 2; Maternally Imprinted GATA genes express transcription factors with zinc fingers in their DNA binding domains (Tsai et al. 1994). GATA transcription factors are a family of proteins that regulate gene expression in hematopoietic cells. Depending on type, GATA proteins function prominently in nonhematopoietic cell interactions also. GATA2, in particular, is expressed in hematopoietic progenitor cells as well as nonhematopoietic embryonic stem cells (Lee et al. 1991). GATA2 is expressed in white adipocyte precursor cells and terminally traps development in the pre-adipocyte stage (Tong et al. 2000). To trap nascent adipocyte cells, GATA2 suppresses peroxisome proliferators-activated receptor-gamma (PPARG), which is predicted to be a paternally imprinted gene on chromosome 3p25.2. It is not surprising that GATA2 is a growth retarding maternally imprinted gene (Luedi et al. 2007). These two directly acting genes oppose one another in growth, development, and imprinting status. GATA2 deficiency is associated with obesity (Tong et al. 2000). This interaction between the maternally imprinted GATA2 growth retardant and PPARG the paternally imprinted growth promoter is aligned with predictions from the kinship theory of gene imprinting (Haig 2004). Similar to other imprinted genes, GATA2 expression plays a prominent role in placental and
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
45
embryonic development, placental angiogenesis, and mother to fetus control of nutrients (Hemberger and Zechner 2004; Ma et al. 1997). Recently, Scherzer et al. (2008) discovered a relationship between GATA genes and alpha-synuclein (SNCA) expression. They found high levels of SNCA expression in red blood cells and erythropoietic precursors. Further, they discovered that both GATA1 (x-linked) and GATA2 (maternally imprinted) regulate expression of SNCA. GATA2 is highly expressed in substantia nigra and frontal lobe cells, occupying intron-1 of SNCA modulating its expression in dopaminergic cells. This demonstrated a link between SNCA expression and GATA2 in PD vulnerable brain areas. The major protein aggregate in Lewy bodies is alpha-synuclein. The relative expression of SNCA directly relates to the pathology of both familial and idiopathic PD. Increase in SNCA gene expression has been consistently demonstrated in PD. Even small increases in SNCA expression can have an additive effect over time leading to accumulation of cytotoxic phosphorylated oligomers of alpha-synuclein in dopaminergic neurons. GATA2 regulation serves as a single mechanism underlying the relative expression of SNCA in both hematopoietic and neuronal cells. Neuronal cells with silenced GATA2 induced a 28% decrease in SNCA mRNA transcription and a 46% decrease in SNCA protein production (Scherzer et al. 2008). Contrastingly, cells with GATA2 or GATA1 expression caused a 62-fold increase in mRNA SNCA transcripts and a 6.9-fold increase in SNCA protein translation. The complete function of GATA2 is difficult to obtain in vivo experimentation because knock-out GATA2 mice leads to embryonic lethality. Figure 2.2 provides an overview of the relationship between imprinted genes and Lewy body formation, catecholamine metabolism, and mitochondrial function. PREDICTION: DGK-θ: DIACYLGLYCEROL KINASE, THETA; MATERNALLY IMPRINTED Our review of the effects of imprinted genes on PD pathogenesis leads us to predict a major role for a newly discovered PD susceptibility gene called DGK-θ in production of PD neurobehavioral symptomology. The rationale for this prediction follows. The largest genome-wide association study to date to investigate susceptibility genes for familial PD identified a novel locus on the petit arm of chromosome 4 (Pankratz et al. 2009). The strongest evidence of association (p < 6 * 10–6) was with three SNPs in the GAK/DKG-θ region. The study assessed 1,119 familial PD cases, and identified this novel locus in addition to confirming two previously known susceptibility regions, one near SNCA on 4q21 and MAPT on 17q21.
46
Dementia
Figure 2.2
Schematic diagram of intracellular biochemical interaction between imprinted genes and PD-related pathways. ♀ – female imprinted gene; ♂ – male imprinted gene; TH – tyrosine hydroxylase; DDC – dopamine decarboxylase; DβH – dopamine beta-hydroxylase; 5-HTR2A – Serotonin Receptor type 2A; DJ-1 – PARK7 gene; NDUFA4 – NADH dehydrogenase (ubiquinone) 1 alpha subcomplex, 4; NDUFS4 – NADH dehygrogenase (ubiquinone) iron-sulfur protein, 4; mtDNA – mitochondrial DNA; ROS – reactive oxygen species.
The statistical significance in disease susceptibility with SNPs at 4p16.3 in the GAK/DGK-θ region exceeded that of SNPs proximal to the genes for SNCA and MAPT. The evidence of the potential association between GAK (cyclin G associated kinase, a cell cycle modulator) and PD susceptibility is plausible because it is one of the 137 genes differentially expressed in PD mid-brain tissue. Grunblatt et al. (2004) demonstrated a 1.56-fold increase in GAK expression in substantia nigra pars compacta tissue in PDs compared to controls. Whether altered regulation of GAK is a compensatory change to loss of DA-ergic neurons or a primary cause of a final biochemical pathway leading to PD histopathological changes remains unknown. Beside the role of cyclin-dependent kinases (CDKs) in cell cycle processes, some evidence supports a role for CDKs in both pro-apototic and anti-apoptotic pathways (Kwon et al. 2008; Padmanabhan et al. 1999; Toiber, Greenberg, and Soreq 2009). No research to date has examined the role of GAK in nigrostriatal neurons in relation to apoptosis. At this point, it is not possible to speculate whether upregulation of GAK in substantia nigra pars compacta PD tissue is causative of cell death or a compensatory preventative cell reaction.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
47
A potential case for the involvement of DGK-θ in PD-related neuronal cell death is more likely given available evidence. The complete cellular workings of diacylglycerol kinase type theta (DGK-θ) are not understood. Important to note, DGK-θ is predicted to be a maternally imprinted gene in a cluster of 10 imprinted genes at the 4p16.3 locus (Luedi et al. 2007). The evidence for imprinting is strengthened by two lines of research. First, deletions in the 4p16.3 region that include DGK-θ lead to Wolf-Hirschhorn syndrome, a disorder dependent on parent-of-origin effects and a hemizygous deletion of the distal short arm of chromosome 4 (Anvret et al. 1991; Hirschhorn, Cooper, and Firschein 1965; Thies et al. 1992; Wolf et al. 1965). Wolf-Hirschhorn syndrome is characterized clinically by severe growth retardation and mental defects, microcephaly, “Greek helmet” facies, cleft lip and/or palate, cardiac septal defects, and coloboma of the eye. Animal models with radiation-induced deletion of a region syntenic to human 4p16.3 lead to growth retardation, susceptibility to seizures, midline structural abnormalities, craniofacial and ocular disturbances, cerebellar hypoplasis, and shortened cerebral cortex (Naf et al. 2001). Another line of research also increases suspicion of gene imprinting of DGK-θ. Imprinted gene products often interact biochemically with geneproducts from other imprinted genes, such as the interaction between the paternally imprinted insulin-like growth factor 2 (11p15.5) and the maternally imprinted insulin-like growth factor 2 receptor (6q25). Likewise, DGK-θ contains a Ras-associated binding domain, and Ras (HRAS gene at 11p15.5) has known imprinted gene expression across varying stages of development (Kratz et al. 2007). So, allegedly the maternally imprinted DGK-θ interacts chemically with paternally imprinted HRAS and insulin (11p15.5), which has been shown to increase DGK-θ activity in cerebral cortex and synaptosomes in adult rat brain tissue (Zulian, Ilincheta de Boschero, and Giusto 2006). This taken in conjunction with the bioinformatics predictive models, and the hemizygous parent-of-origin impact of 4p16.3 deletions, greatly increase the probability that DGK-θ is subject to gene imprinting effects. The primary structure of the DGK-θ isotype contains a proline-rich region, three cysteine-rich domains, a Ras-associating domain, a pleckstrin homology domain, and a catalytic region (Houssa et al. 1997). DGK-θ is a member of a larger class of enzymes called diacylglycerol kinases (DGKs), which are known to be important regulators of cellular signaling and homeostasis (Merida, Avila-Flores, and Merino 2008; Sakane et al. 2007). DGK phosphorylates diacylglycerol (DAG) to generate phosphatidic acid (PA). Maintaining a balance between these two bioactive lipids, DAG and PA, is a primary function for DGKs. The pleckstrin domain within DGK-θ allows binding to phosphatidylinositol lipids in membranes, G-proteins,
48
Dementia
and protein kinase C (Haslam, Koide, and Hemmings 1993). Recent research suggests that DGK-θ can also localize to the nucleus within neurons and regulate gene transcription (Tu-Sekine and Raben 2009). The question arises, how might alterations in the function of DGK-θ lead to PD pathology? One potential explanation involves the relationship between DGK-θ, dopamine metabotropic signaling, and lipid peroxidation. Regulating levels of DAG is key to the intracellular signaling of DA, which utilizes metabotropic second messenger systems (Girault and Greengard 2004). DA binding activates adenylyl cyclase and phospholipase C (PLC) via the D1/ D5 receptor mechanism, and inhibits via the D2/D3/D4 receptor system. This, in turn, regulates intracellular levels of second messengers, such as, cyclic adenosine 3′, 5′-monophosphate (cAMP), DAG, and intracellular Ca2+ concentration (Nishino et al. 1993). Proper functioning of DGK is essential for DA signaling homeostasis because of its regulatory role in phosphorylating DAG. Dysregulation of intracellular DAG levels can lead to oxidative stress and ignition of inflammatory pathways by way of the following mechanism. All neural membranes are comprised of phospholipids high in eicosanoids (e.g., arachidonic acid, docosahexaenoic acid, etc.). These polyunsaturated fats (PUFA) promote membrane fluidity, permeability, and normal cell function. Peroxidation of membrane phospholipids causes release and loss of embedded PUFAs and subsequent loss of membrane flexibility (Catala 2009). Lipid peroxidation can result from interaction with hydroxyl free radicals or activation of the phospholipase A2 or phospholipase C/diacylglycerol lipase pathways (Farooqui and Horrocks 1994). Several factors can exaggerate hydroxyl free radical formation, such as intracellular exposure to heavy metals like iron and copper, which catalyze hydroxyl radical formation. Both catabolic lipase pathways, the phospholipase A2 and phospholipase C/diacylglycerol pathways, are inducible by excess excitatory amino acid receptor binding or excess intracellular levels of DAG (Farooqui and Horrocks 1994, 1998). Following DA depletion in nigrostriatal neurons of PD tissue, protective homeostatic mechanisms respond by alterations in transcriptional upregulation of several excitatory receptor subunits (e.g., glutamatergic) in attempt to compensate for the decreased DA-ergic activity (Mallet et al. 2006; Meurers et al. 2009; Wilson and Kawaguchi 1996). This, in turn, can begin a deleterious feed-forward biochemical cycle: compensatory upregulation of excitatory receptor activity on DA nigrostriatal neurons leads to an increase in activity in the phospholipase A2 and phospholipase C/ diacylglycerol pathways, which gives way to an increase in lipid catabolism and peroxidation, loss of membrane fluidity, destabilization of bioelectric currents (namely Ca2+ balance), and activation of pro-apoptotic pathways, cell loss and aggravation of DA-ergic cell loss.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
49
The brain consumes approximately 20% of body oxygen despite constituting only 2–3% of human body mass (Moreira et al. 2005). Across cell types, brain cells are particularly vulnerable to oxidative stress due to the high level of oxygen consumption and processing. This vulnerability is heightened by the high levels of PUFAs in neural cell membranes in relation to comparatively low levels of antioxidant enzymes (Mariani et al. 2005). Aging is associated with increased oxidative stress due to elevated levels of ROS within the brain accumulating over time, which induces protein misfolding, aggregation, and formation of inclusion bodies (Cardoso et al. 2005). Couple the increase in age-associated oxidative stress with a polymorphic version of the DGK-θ protein to decrease its efficacy— depending on cellular compensatory mechanisms via alternate pathways, over time peroxidation of membrane lipids becomes increasingly likely with an age-associated increase in oxidative stress and functionally dysregulated DAG/PA ratios due to less efficacious DGK-θ enzymatic action. As previously stated, dysregulation of DAG levels can lead to the release of polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) via the phospholipase A2 and phospholipase C/diacylglycerol lipase pathway, decreasing membrane fluidity, loss of membrane integrity, and resultant cell death via any number of pathways, such as Ca2+ dependent and Ca2+ independent apoptosis. Homeostasis of DAG levels is crucial for controlling lipid peroxidation (Farooqui and Horrocks 1998; Nishino et al. 1989). Additionally, the regulation of DAG levels by DGK-θ is important in controlling the activation of protein kinase C (PKC) induced NADPH oxidase activity (Miller et al. 2009). DAG or phorbol esters interact with protein kinase C to induce production of free radicals through activation of NADPH oxidase (Kozikowski et al. 2003; Nishino et al. 1989). Ubiquitous to neural cells, PKC isoforms regulate a wide array of biochemical activity, such as ion channel and receptor balance, neurotransmitter release, and synaptogenesis (Battaini 2001; Catarsi and Drapeau 1997; Wagey et al. 2001). Aberrant PKC enzymatic activity has been noted in several neurodegenerative conditions, such as Alzheimer ’s and PD (Ahlemeyer et al. 2002; Kozikowski et al. 2003; Ran et al. 2003). NADPH-oxidase is upregulated in the substantia nigra of PD patients (Wu et al. 2003). One potential mechanism to connect these lines of evidence involves dysfunctional DGK-θ activity. If DGK-θ fails to efficiently convert DAG to PA and cellular levels of DAG rise, then DAG binding to PKC should increase inducing an increase in the NADPH-oxidase activity. NADPH activity increases ROS and mediates neurotoxicity of alpha-synuclein (Zhang et al. 2005). The aggregate-inducing interaction between ROS and alpha-synuclein is well described. Another potential aggravator are PUFAs, which as
50
Dementia
previously highlighted are exposable to intracellular contents given fluctuating DAG/PA ratios via DGK dysfunction. Sharon et al. (2003) identified a cellular pool of soluble oligomers of alpha-synuclein in normal mesencephalic human brain tissue. Exposure to PUFAs increased amounts of soluble alpha-synuclein oligomers, whereas exposure to saturated fatty acids decreased oligomer levels. In mouse models, soluble oligomers of alphasynuclein were demonstrated to increase with age. This fits with the finding that patients with PD or other Lewy body–associated conditions have higher levels of lipid-interacting soluble oligomers of alpha-synuclein. The process leading to alpha-synuclein aggregation in neurodegenerative disease is likely preceded by PUFAs and soluble stores of alpha-synuclein increasingly interacting over time. Finally, there is a very close biochemical connection between DAG pathways and ceramide synthesis (Liu, Kleine, and Herbert 1999). Ceramide is an important second messenger implicated in numerous signal transduction pathways mediating cell growth, differentiation, inflammatory response, and apoptosis (Cutler and Mattson 2001). DA-ergic cell signaling, DAG, and ceramide biochemical pathways have been demonstrated to interact dynamically (Liu et al. 2003) and emerging pathways in the genetics of Parkinson’s disease implicate ceramide metabolism (Bras et al. 2008). In sum, the largest genome-wide association study to date identified a novel PD susceptibility area with SNPs in the GAK/DGK-θ region on human chromosome 4p16.3 (Pankratz et al. 2009). This is notable because DGK-θ is predicted to be a maternally imprinted gene and our confidence in the accuracy of this prediction is increased because parent-of-origin transmission effects from its locus at 4p16.3 have already been demonstrated. As suggested, DGK-θ could potentially be involved in modulating lipid peroxidation, production of ROS, and maintaining neuronal membrane stability. Further, DGK-θ is important for regulating DAG-related ceramide biosynthetic pathways, which are potentially involved in Parkinson’s disease pathology. Figure 2.3 furnishes an overview of the potential pathways that DGK-θ regulates that relate to PD cellular dysfunction.
COMPLEXITY OF IMPRINTING EFFECTS: GRB10, GROWTH FACTOR RECEPTOR BOUND PROTEIN 10; MATERNAL IMPRINTING IN THE PERIPHERY AND PATERNAL IMPRINTING IN THE BRAIN We now illustrate the flexibility and complexity of imprinting effects on physiologic and PD-related pathophysiologic systems. GRB10 inhibits receptor-bound insulin signaling pathways (Liu and Roth 1995). GRB10
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
51
Figure 2.3
Biochemical pathways associated with the maternally imprinted gene DGKtheta, a complex intracellular signaling molecule implicated in PD susceptibility by a recent genome-wide assoiation study. DGK-θ – diacylglycerol kinase type theta; PKC – protein kinase type C; NADPH oxidase – nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phospate-oxidase; DAG – diacylglycerol; PA – phosphatidic acid; PUFA – polyunsaturated fatty acid; ♀ – female imprinted gene.
binds with high affinity to autophosphorylated insulin receptors bound by insulin. Formation of this GRB10-insulin-insulin receptor complex inhibits insulin-dependent phosphorylative activation of IRS1 and a GTPase activating protein. GRB10 can also inhibit tyrosine kinase activity involved in growth promotion caused by IGF1. Another biochemical activity of GRB10 involves downstream reactions in insulin-mediated pathways. In response to insulin stimulation, GRB10 migrates from the cytosol to the cellular membrane. GRB10 can also bind activated platelet-derived growth factor receptor, epidermal growth factor receptor, and growth hormone (Frantz et al. 1997). Blagitko et al. (2000) demonstrated that GRB10 is imprinted in a tissue-specific manner. Maternal expression predominates in fetal skeletal
52
Dementia
muscle and paternal expression dominates in brain tissue. Charalambous et al. (2003) confirmed that GRB10 is imprinted with the majority of total protein expression belonging to the maternal allele. Blocking the maternal allele caused a 30% increase in both embryonic and placental overgrowth in mice. Maternal GRB10 alleles are growth inhibitors in the periphery capable of suppressing tyrosine kinase mediated growth. The paternally imprinted gene IGF2 is a growth-promoting gene not impacted by downstream effects of GRB10 growth-inhibition. This is consistent with the gene-conflict model of offspring development. GRB10 is a gene of interest to PD research because of its chemical interaction with PARK11. Located at 2q37, it is also known as GIGYF2 (GRB10 Interacting GYF Protein 2). Lautier et al. (2008) demonstrated a genetic association between GIGYF2 mutations and PD in a study of 250 European individual with a family history of PD. Homologous proteins, GIGYF2 and GIGYF1, bind GRB10 through the GYF domain and interact with insulin and IGF signaling, which is vital to brain function. Giovannone et al. (2003) provided evidence that overexpression of GIGYF1 bound to IGF1 receptor complexes formed cytotoxic inclusion bodies. Dysregulation of either GRB10 or GIGYF2 could lead to formation of inclusion bodies in the brain. One potential contributory mechanism could be the formation of cytotoxic inclusion bodies because of over expression of GIGYF2 or GRB10. Because GRB10 is paternally expressed in brain tissue, unrestrained growth promoting effects could lead to formation of inclusion bodies via overload of protein expression across time that overwhelm cellular machinery that handle protein turnover. Reciprocally, a loss of maternal function to restrain growth could lead to excess GIGYF2 or GRB10 binding to IGF receptors and subsequent formation of cytotoxic protein aggregates. With this potential etiological mechanism, PD emerges as an effect of dysregulated growth in brain tissue based on paternal growth promotion outpacing maternal growth restraint. Either a maternal loss-of-function in growth restraint mechanisms or a paternal gain-of-function in growth and protein expression could set up the conditions for accumulation of proteinaceous inclusion bodies, such as Lewy bodies, over time (see Table 2.2). PD ASSOCIATED GENES IN IMPRINTING CENTERS Many regions of the genome are associated with risk for developing PD. In this brief section, we present several “suspect” genes with either known or unknown imprinting status. The proximity of these target areas to imprinting centers increases the likelihood that epigenetic mechanisms influence gene expression.
Gene
NDUFA4
TH
Mitochondrial Function
Catecholamine Metabolism
DA System FOXA2 Building and Longevity DA System LMX1B Building and Longevity Mitochondrial NDUFS4 Function
Relation to PD pathology
11p15.5
1p13.3
5q11.1
9q33.3
20p11.21
Forkhead transcription factor, 2A LIM Homeobox transcription factor 1 beta NADH-ubiquinone oxdore ductase Fe-S protein 4 NADHubiquinone oxidoreductase 1 alpha subcomplex 4 Tyrosine hydroxylase
Chromosome Encoded locus protein
Rate-limiting catalyst in the conversion of L-tyrosine to L-DOPA
Pivotal for Complex I mitochondrial function; demonstrated to bind DJ-1 to preserve mitochondrial function
Plays a role in early development of the notochord; demonstrated to play a role in longevity via regulation of dopaminergic mid-brain neurons Important for the development of mesenchephalic dopaminergic neurons; SNPs associated with PD risk in women Crucial for Complex I mitochondrial function; electron transfer
Putative function
Table 2.2 Imprinted Genes and Predicted Imprinted Genes Associated with PD
(Continued)
Maternally imprinted *
Paternally imprinted
Paternally imprinted
Maternally imprinted
Maternally imprinted
Mode of inheritance
3q21.3
GATA2
Diacylglycerol kinase, theta Growth factor receptor bound protein 10; maternally expressed gene 1
L-amino acid decarboxylase Dopamine β-hydroxylase 5-HT receptor, 2A GATA binding protein 2 Polymorphisms in this 5-HT receptor have been linked to PD susceptibility and phenotype Zinc-finger transcription factor; regulates expression of α-synuclein in substantia nigra and frontal lobes Modulate balance between diacylglyerol (DAG) and phosphatidic acid (PA) Involved in insulin signaling pathways; interacts with park11 /GIGYF2 (GRB10 interacting GYF protein 2)
Enzyme responsible for synthesizing dopamine and serotonin Enzyme responsible for synthesizing dopamine and serotonin
Maternally imprinted Maternally * expression in peripheral tissue and Paternal * expression in brain
Maternally imprinted * Maternally imprinted
Paternally imprinted * Maternally imprinted
* Denotes known imprinted gene; remaining genes are predicted to imprinted by the bioinformatics model from Luedi et al. (2007).
DGKQ 4p16.3 (DGK-θ) GRB10 7p12 (MEG1)
13q14
5-HTR2A
Catecholamine Metabolism Lewy Body Formation
Lewy Body Formation Lewy Body Formation
DDC 7p12 (AAAD) DBH 9q34.2
Catecholamine Metabolism Catecholamine Metabolism
Table 2.2 (Continued)
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
55
11p15.5 Two genes located in this imprinting center are related to PD susceptibility but their imprinting status is not known: DRD4 and CTSD. The dopamine-four-receptor (DRD4) is a metabotropic, G protein-coupled receptor categorized in the D2-like receptor family. This class of receptors all function to inhibit adenylyl cyclase (Oldenhaf et al. 1998). DRD4 is differentially expressed across varied regions of the brain with the prefrontal cortex containing the highest density of receptors that act to inhibit neural firing (Dulawa et al. 1999). Additionally, high expression of DRD4 messenger ribonucleic acid (mRNA) has been detected in the medulla, midbrain, and amygdala (D’Souza et al. 2004). Linkage disequilibrium analysis demonstrates linkage to nearby imprinted genes, such as tyrosine hydroxylase and the Harvey RAS oncogene (Gelernter et al. 1992). DRD4 shows evidence of maternal transmission patterns in some cases of bipolar disorder and paternal patterns in cases of ADHD (Hawi et al. 2005; Muglia et al. 2002). However, no direct tissue-by-tissue assessment of imprinting across varying developmental periods has been completed so the imprinting status of DRD4 is still speculative but with high suspicion based on parent-of-origin effects in neuropsychiatric populations. Ricketts et al. (1998) demonstrated an association between PD and long variants (>6) of the variable number tandem repeats (VNTR) in exon III of DRD4. A recent study of north and south Indian populations revealed an association between DRD4 120-bp duplication markers and PD (Juyal et al. 2006). Both long VNTR variants in exon III and 120-bp duplication makers functionally reduce the activity or expression of the DRD4 receptor. Because of its inhibitory function, loss-of-DRD4 receptor function results in phenotypic disinhibition. Beyond genetic susceptibility to PD, DRD4 polymorphisms have been implicated in sleep attacks in PD. Paus et al. (2004) found an association between VNTRs in exon III and sleep attacks in PD. A forthcoming article by Eisenegger et al. (2009) implicates DRD4 polymorphisms in L-DOPA responsiveness and pathological gambling behavior. Another gene located at 11p15.5 related to PD is cathepsin D (CTSD; Latourelle et al. 2009). Cathepsins are lysosomal proteases, and CTSD is an important lysosomal aspartyl protease comprised of two disulfideconnected polypeptide chains (Hasilik and Neufeld 1980). Cullen et al. (2009) demonstrated the CTSD degrades alpha-synuclein in dopaminergic cells and that mutations in CTSD lead to lysosomal storage dysfunction, alpha-synuclein misprocessing, and alpha-synuclein toxicity.
56
Dementia
15q13 A genome wide study of genetic susceptibility to PD implicated the 15q13 loci, which is the region connected to the imprinting disorders of Prader-Willi and Angelman syndrome (Morison, Ramsay, and Spencer 2005; Srinivasan et al. 2008). Ubiquitin-mediated proteolysis was implicated in this study with loss of function to the maternally imprinted ubiquinating gene (UBEA3) as the most likely molecular target. 1p36 This region of the human genome contains at least 26 imprinted genes, mainly of maternal origin (Luedi et al. 2007; Morison et al. 2005). PD associated genes of interest located in this 1p36 region are PINK1 (PARK6) and DJ-1 (PARK7), and ATP13A2 (PARK9). Additionally, a genome-wide association study assessing the impact of genetics on age-of-onset identified an SNP in the 1p36.32 region coding for the maternally imprinted gene peroxisome biogenesis factor 10, also known as Pex10 (Latourelle et al. 2009; Luedi et al. 2007). While the association did not meet conservative criteria for genome-wide significance, it was still an important predictor of PD susceptibility in certain populations. Pex10 encodes a protein important for importing peroxisomal matrix proteins. Loss of maternal function in Pex10 likely increases oxidative stress within select cellular environments (Warren et al. 1998). X-LINKED GENES AND PD Beside the impact of autosomal DNA on PD susceptibility, several loci on the X chromosome correlate with PD risk. While numerous reports of several X-linked genes exist in the literature on PD susceptibility, Table 2.3 presents an abridged list of the most frequently replicated X-linked loci associated with PD risk. The X-linked genes might help to explain the sex-difference in PD risk. Other explanations for the male to female ratio difference include (1) sex hormone impact on disease susceptibility, (2) differences in male versus female DA network function (e.g., DA receptor density and recycling, DA metabolic rates, etc.), and (3) male versus female behavior differences that lead to differential environmental exposures leading to increased risk of PD. We will suggest another potential explanatory option related to gene imprinting in the following section. Imprinting might be important for explaining parent-of-origin influences on X-linked gene susceptibility loci for any given condition. Murine
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
57
Table 2.3 X-Linked Genes Associated with PD Locus (gene)
Chromosomal Encoded locus protein
GATA1
Xp11.23
MAO-B
Xp11.23
LAMP2
Xq24
PARK12
Xq21-q25
PASD1
Xq28
Putative function
GATA binding Regulates ALAS2 protein 1 and SNCA gene expression in addition to FECH, the catalyst for the final step of heme biosynthesis, which adds iron into protoporphyrin IX Monoamine Mitochondrial oxidase B enzymes responsible for catabolizing biogenic amines (e.g., dopamine) in brain and platelets Lysosomal glycoLysosomalassociated protein receptor membrane necessary for protein 2 protein degradation via chaperone-mediated autophagy; mutant α-synuclein blocks LAMP2 function Genetic linkage Unknown evidence suggene gests this region product is a susceptibility locus for PD PAS domain Associated with containing 1 PD
Mode of inheritance X-linked
X-linked
X-linked
X-linked
X-linked
58
Dementia
models show the genes most important for X-inactivation: X-ist and Tsix are paternally and maternally imprinted, respectively (Marahrens et al. 1997). However, evidence of X-linked imprinting in humans was inconclusive until the past few years. Recently, because of the increased prevalence of gene-imprinting associated diseases in babies born with assisted reproductive technology (ART) therapy, research to support a role for imprinting in humans developed. Kobayashi et al. (2009) present evidence for paternal imprinting of the X-ist gene in humans. Gene imprinting might help to explain X-linked risk of PD based on male or female descent due to the differential inactivation of X chromosome via epigenetic regulation. EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVES In this final section, we situate our findings within a larger theoretical framework that draws on Haig’s well-supported kinship theory of gene imprinting, and on evolutionary approaches to aging. Based on phylogenetic comparisons, genetics of menopause and aging, the free radical theory of aging, intergenerational resource transfer models, models of maternal care and genomic imprinting, and the implication of imprinting in the etiology of numerous neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative diseases, we suggest that aging and longevity are significantly influenced by genetic conflict. To the extent that some aspects of PD pathophysiology are linked with aberrant aging processes, PD too will be influenced by genetic conflict. We suggest age-related aspects of PD are influenced by epigenetic shifts toward loss-of-function and gain-of-function in matrilineal and patrilineal alleles, respectively. Summary of Relevant Aspects of the Kinship Theory of Gene Imprinting The kinship theory of genomic imprinting attributes imprinted expression of a locus to a conflict of interests between alleles of maternal and paternal origin (Burt and Trivers 1998; Haig 1992; Haig 2000a; Haig 2004; Haig and Westoby 1989). Conflicts arise when the expression of gene G in offspring Z exerts fitness consequences for other individuals to whom offspring Z has differential matrilineal and patrilineal coefficients of relatedness (Haig 2004; Normark 2006). That is, levels of expression of gene G in offspring Z come under selective pressure when either greater or lesser expression of G differentially affects matrilineal or patrilineal inclusive fitness. As an example and stated more concretely, genes of paternal
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
59
origin are expected to promote increased demands on mothers throughout pregnancy, whereas genes of maternal origin are predicted to restrain fetal demand on the mother (Haig 2000b; Haig 2004). This “tug-of-war” within the developing embryo is essentially a genetic scuffle over resource withdrawal from the gravid female. Evidence comes from conditions associated with loss of either paternal or maternal imprinting, evidenced in Prader-Willi and Silver-Russell syndrome (both loss of paternal gene expression or maternal uniparental disomy) or in Angelman and Beckwith-Widemann syndrome (both due to loss of maternal gene expression or excess paternal gene function) (Eggermann, Eggermann, and Schonherr 2008; Haig 2010). In addition to fetus-mother struggles over resources, the kinship theory is relevant to all of a given individual’s interactions with asymmetric kin (Haig 1997; Haig 2000b). Table 2.4 outlines the paternal versus maternal allelic effects of known imprinted genes on growth schedules and behavior across varying life stages. The terms madumnal and padumnal are used to denote the allelic influence of maternally and paternally derived alleles within a given individual, respectively (Wilkins and Haig 2003, “Inbreeding”). In an infinitely outbreeding population, all children of a given mother have madumnal alleles perfectly in common (relatedness of 1) whereas, given paternity uncertainty (either due to serial monogamy or polygamous sexual practice), the padumnal alleles among kin of this same mother are asymmetrically related (Relatedness Range: 0–1) (Haig 2004). Gene imprinting arises when equal expression of maternally derived and paternally derived alleles fails to remain evolutionarily stable (Wilkins and Haig 2003, “Inbreeding”). Evolutionary instability and selection can drive preferential allele expression based on parent-of-origin until the allele of lesser expression becomes silenced via mechanisms of nucleic acid methylation, histone acetylation, ncRNA action, etc. In the section to follow we propose a kinship theory of genomic conflict in aging, which represents a provisional attempt to make sense of the reason (1) Parkinson’s disease exists, (2) male susceptibility is higher, and (3) why PD is primarily a condition of aging. Kinship Theory of Genomic Conflict in Aging The kinship theory of gene imprinting predicts intragenomic conflict whenever levels of gene expression exert asymmetric effects on the fitness of patrilineal and matrilineal kin. Based on models of inbreeding (Wilkins and Haig 2003a; Haig 1999), we derive the source of conflict in aging from the differential rate in change of coefficients of relatedness (padumnal
60
Dementia
Table 2.4 Overview of Parent-of-Origin Gene Strategies to Increase Respective Inclusive Fitness by Developmental Stages Life stage
Madumnal alleles
Prenatal [~9 months]
Retard growth schedules Promote fetal overgrowth Decrease placental blood Increase placental blood flow by elevated blood flow by reduced blood pressure to lessen nutri- pressure to promote nutrient transfer to fetus ent transfer to fetus Hasten partuition Delay partuition
Postnatal and Preweaning [~0–3 years]
Lessen intensity suckling Heighten suckling intensity Stronger appetite for Lowered appetite for supplemental foods supplemental foods Earlier weaning Prolong weaning Shorten lactational Lengthen lactational amenorrhea amenorrhea Decrease demanding Increase demanding behavior behavior
Postnatal, postweaning, and preadrenarche [~3–6 years]
Accelerate past “selfcentered,” no sharing behavior Greater acceptance of responsibility
Adrenarche, prepubescence, and pregonadarche [~6–12 years]
Decrease behaviors of Increase behaviors of social reciprocity and social reciprocity and increase in food stealing food sharing Attenuated appetite and Hyperphagia and increase slowed growth in growth rate Continued reliance on Shift from feeding from maternal “local” feeding “local” feeding pot to “communal” feeding pot pot
Pubescence and early adulthood [> 13+ years] Adulthood and postreproductive aging [~25–75 years]
Quickened shift from nonreproductive consumer to reproductive producer ?
Padumnal alleles
Prolong “self-centered,” no sharing behavior Reticence to accept responsibility
Tarried shift from nonreproductive consumer to reproductive producer ?
Note: Evidence comes from study of syndromes resulting from loss- or gain- of paternal or maternal gene function in Prader-Willi, Silver-Russell, Angelman’s, Beckwith-Widemann, and Temple syndromes.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
61
versus madumnal) as a function of female reproductive life spans. Modeling of empirical data on aging in humans (from recent and ancestral hunter-gatherer groups) supports our claim that intergenerational transfers predict low offspring mortality especially as female fertility declines with age (Lee 2003). To increase inclusive fitness, madumnal allele expression favors life extension beyond reproductive capacity. We suggest that PD results when epigenetic mechanisms controlling aging become dysregulated. Source of Gene Conflict in Aging Because ancestral populations did not represent infinitely outbred, genetically unrelated kin, the genetic relatedness differed by group, that is, inbreeding to some degree was occurring in all reproductive acts (Wilkins and Haig 2003b). For example, if a female mates with a related male her two alleles may have different probabilities of being present in the spermatozoa that fertilize her oocyte. Geographic structure and higher male reproductive variance imply that earlier matings by reproductively viable females tend to produce kin that are more padumnally related compared to madumnally related. In time, if a mother disperses to a new locale, older potential mates with high patrilineal relatedness die off or the group splits, then the average madumnal relatedness increases in kin bred from later stages of a given females reproductive lifespan—thus, madumnal inclusive fitness is maximized. Reciprocally, padumnal relatedness is maximized in kin bred from earlier female reproductive stages. So, inbreeding and higher male reproductive variance leads to differentially favorable padumnal inclusive fitness with maximal resource demands incurred to mothers of such kin (Haig 1999). The formalization of the model (Wilkins and Haig 2003b) is summarized below. Let rm1 and rp1 represent the average relatedness of current offspring for a female’s madumnal and padumnal alleles, respectively, and let rm2 and rp2 represent the average relatedness of future offspring for a female’s madumnal and padumnal alleles, respectively. The most favorable allocation of maternal resources, from the “perspective” of an allele, is governed by the ratio (V) of current relatedness to mean future relatedness, for madumnal (Vm = rm1/rm2) and padumnal alleles (Vp = rp1/rp2). In unbounded outbreeding population, Vm will always equal Vp (e.g., rm1 = rm2 = rp1 = rp2 = ½). Mating conditions in ancestral populations, however, were not under conditions of complete symmetry. Asymmetry arose, and consequently evolutionary selective processes, when populations exhibited size-boundedness based on the environmental carrying capacity of a given geographic locale, with male/female differences in reproductive
62
Dementia
variance, and with inbreeding and/or incest rates changing over the course of a female’s reproductive life. Resultantly, the rate of change in the probability of relatedness (V) differs for madumnal and padumnal alleles. Wilkins and Haig (2003b) suppose that the age-related decline in rm and rp could be modeled as an exponential decay process changing in time toward (1 + F)/2, where F is a female’s own degree of inbreeding. We find an exponential decay process to be reasonable because evidence suggests that maternal compared to paternal dispersal was likely more common in ancestral populations (Selelstad, Minch, and Cavalli-Sforza 1998). Figure 2.4 demonstrates the differential decay process between madumnal and padumal alleles, which selects for different time preferences for investment. In relation to the genetic control of aging, we will make the case that madumnal alleles will promote longevity, especially in maternal grandmothers, in order to transfer the cost of maternal investment “now” to aged-others “later”. Phylogenetic Comparisons Interbirth intervals, longevity and postreproductive life histories in humans stand in stark contrast to our most phylogenetically related species, pan troglodytes and pan paniscus (chimpanzees and bonobos, respectively). Both species from the pan genus have approximate life spans of 40 years in the wild with reproductive cycles every 6 to 8 years (Tarou et al. 2002). On average both chimpanzee and bonobo mothers can have 5–6 offspring total throughout their reproductive life. A well-supported human female can reproduce every 2–3 years over the course of a 30-year reproductive life span (~age 16 to 46), which potentially results in 10 offspring precluding multiple births. This strategy is only successful with resource transfers from “mothers to others” (preferably to those with high coefficients of madumnal relatedness). Compared to the phylogenetically related great apes, then, humans exhibit a prolonged life after reproductive viability, an extended juvenile stage of dependence, and support of resource provision by older postreproductive individuals and/or reproductively active male resource contributors (Kaplan et al. 2000). Intergenerational Transfers Human ontogenetic development is marked by a series of passages through differing patterns of resource transfer. Resource transfer occurs from placenta to embryo, breast to baby, and foraging and food pots to child. Measures of “resources” include food, warming (group thermoregulation via burning brown adipose; see Haig 2007), fanning, guarding, carrying,
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
63
teaching, and leading. Modeling of empirical data regarding offspring mortality rates with the aforementioned resource-type provisioning from modern-day and hunter-gatherer populations demonstrates that life histories are shaped by intergenerational transfer of resources provided by post-reproductive individuals (Lee 2003). The most optimized strategy, then, for investment payoff is the intergenerational transfer effect. This explains the necessity of postreproductive survivorship and why juvenile mortality declines with age. In other words, a three-generation model of resource transfer is the only plausible explanatory device. Madumnal gene interests in longevity promote inclusive fitness by transferring the costs of resource provision for offspring to grandparents, especially grandmothers (or any other aged individuals with high madumnal relatedness coefficients). Further, relatedness asymmetries, based on the model of Wilkins and Haig (2003b) predict that reproduction by mothers at times later in their reproductive life favors madumnal inclusive fitness. Offspring from partuition later in a mother ’s reproductive lifespan will escape high mortality rates if proper provisioning of Figure 2.4
Average Relatedness 0.76
0.72
y
0.68
Average paternal relatedness curve
0.64
Average maternal related curve 0.6
0.56
Time, fraction of female’s reproductive life
0.52 0
x 0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7
0.8
0.9
1
A depiction of rate of decay of madumnal and padumnal allele relatedness across a given female’s reproductive lifespan. Note that both values decrease as the female’s mating pool includes fewer related males due to death and dispersal. Relatedness decay asymptotically approaches (1 + F)/2 given F, the female’s inbreeding coefficient (adapted from Wilkins & Haig, 2003).
64
Dementia
resources is available by contributors to both the “local” pot (from kin with high madumnal relatedness) and “community” pot (kin with either unknown or low relatedness to madumnal alleles). Nonparental helpers are absolutely necessary for offspring survival—for example, 11 people on average help to raise a single child in the African Ituri Forest hunter-gather society and in other instances kin relationships influence the degree to which a helper contributes to the local (increased madumnal relatedness) compared to the community resource pot (decreased madumnal relatedness) (Ivey 2000; Pashos and McBurney 2008). We suggest longevity promoted by madumnal alleles provides the occasion for grandmothers (of maternal descent most probabilistically) to provide care and food for the local pot, and grandfathers that contribute to the “community” pool. Due to paternity uncertainty, grandfathers are increasingly less likely to identify kin of higher padumnal relatedness, so contribution to the community reserves remains the most likely “average” scenario (Haig 2010). Genomic Conflict over Aging If madumnal inclusive fitness improves with later-stage reproductive efforts, then why did not selective process merely extend the reproductive lives of human females? In fact, regression models from empirical data on rates of follicular attrition predict that human female reproductive capacities should extend to 70 years (Cant and Johnstone 2008). However, a rapid drop-off in fertility occurs after the fourth decade of life largely under genetic and perhaps epigenetic control (Liu et al. 2010; Murabito et al. 2005; van Asselt et al. 2004; Voorhuis et al. 2010). Formal modeling of human reproductive life cycles shows how menopause and prolonged nonreproductive life stages decreases reproductive competition among females and boosts matrilineal inclusive fitness. The genetics underlying this process of “physiological decay in fertility” was codified in a common primate ancestral species (Thompson et al. 2007) so the genetically programmed life-extension beyond reproductive viability is a distinctly Homo feature that needs explaining. If (1) offspring from early female reproductive life cycles favors padumnal inclusive fitness and offspring from late reproductive efforts enhances madumnal inclusive fitness, (2) menopause is genetically controlled (if not epigenetically) to extinguish conditions high in maternal competitiveness, (3) madumnal alleles favor shorter interbirth intervals and padumnal alleles favor longer interbirth intervals, (4) offspring viability (especially from later reproductive female stages) requires intergenerational transfers for success, then our kinship theory of gene conflict in aging predicts
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
65
(a) matrilineal control over enhancing longevity, (b) longevity distinctly benefiting madumnal inclusive fitness, and (c) counter strategies and antagonizing effects of aging beyond reproductive viability deriving from padumnal alleles. We propose that PD (and perhaps other neurodegenerative conditions) can result when epigenetic dysmodulation of the alleles involved in this intragenomic conflict over aging become biochemically salient to phenotypic expression. Matrilineal genetics tend to be in control of longevity, based on the most prevalent theory of aging in mammals, the free radical theory of aging (Harman 1956) and telomere-length. Evidence for the free radical theory compiled over the decades show that (1) mitochondrial production of ROS and/or damage to mtDNA increase with age, (2) introduction of chemically uncoupled or aged mitochondria into young cells leads to cellular degeneration, (3) overexpression of enzymatic controllers of ROS reduces mtDNA damage and ROS thus extending lifespan, and (4) caloric restraint reduces mitochondrial ROS production and mtDNA mutation and thus prolongs cellular life (for a review see Benz and Yau 2008; DePinho 2000). Epigenetics are implicated in the modulation of free radical production and control (Allen and Balin 1989; Guarente 2006; Hitchler and Domann 2007; Imai et al. 2000; Orr et al. 2005; Sohal and Allen 1985; Stover et al. 2000). Another mechanism involves the connection between telomere length and aging. Two of the major regulators are dyskerin (DKC1, at X28 maternally controlled) and telomerase reverse transcriptase (TERT), which is located at 5p15.33—a site flanked by 10 predicted maternally imprinted genes (Calado and Young 2009; Luedi et al. 2007). So two major longevity associated systems exhibit maternal control—via mtDNA, X-linked, and epigenetic mechanisms. Numerous genes have been associated with longevity, including, apolipoprotein epsilon (APOE, De Benedictis et al. 1997; Kervinen et al. 1994), TH (De Benedictis et al. 1998), and DDC (loss of paternal function, DeLuca et al. 2001, 2003). Variations in APOE are of interest to the present discussion because of its deep connection to the pathophysiology of neurodegenerative diseases of aging, such as Alzheimer ’s and PD (Bu 2009; Gallardo, Schluter, and Sudhof 2008; Saunders et al. 1993). And as previously discussed variations in the maternally imprinted TH and paternally imprinted DDC are associated with PD risk. Because, TH and DDC impact resource conflict at prenatal and early postnatal stages, it is interesting that an impact on aging is present. Once a given locus has been silenced, new functions acquired by that locus that maximize inclusive fitness of the controlling genome (madumnal or padumnal) can emerge as pleiotropic effects (Haig 2000a).
66
Dementia
Given that life extension is predominately controlled by maternally controlled gene systems, how and why might these mechanisms favor greater female versus male life expectancy? Difference in life span between males and females is a complex phenomenon because several possible environmental, epigenetic, and genetic overlapping factors can impact phenotypic outcomes. Controlling for environmental factors, quantitative genetic analyses have revealed that the genetic architecture of aging varies in males and females (Fox et al. 2006; Lai et al. 2007; Veumeulen, Bijlsma, and Loeschcke 2008). Gene imprinting, sex-linked genetics, and maternally controlled mtDNA appear to all contribute to both aging and the gender difference in longevity. Asymmetric inheritance of mtDNA from maternal lineages in humans precludes the possibility for paternal optimization processes to engage natural selective processes to increase inclusive fitness (Tower 2006). A recent study assessed the impact of X-linked genes on aging in Drosophila by analysis of sex-specific levels of transcript variation (Wayne et al. 2007). Variation in males resulted mainly from additive allele interaction, while in females allele expression was largely nonadditive and subject more to epistatic control, especially through X-associated gene regulation. As mentioned above, life span can be extended by dietary restriction (e.g., lower mitochondrial energy metabolism, thus less ROS over time). Paternally imprinted genes can lower life span with gain-of-function growth factor expression or extend life span when loss-of-function mutations ensue. For example, C. elegans, Drosophila, and murine models demonstrate that loss-of-function in insulin (paternally imprinted at 11p15) and IGF1 signaling increases life span (Chandrashekar and Bartke 2003; Partridge, Gems, and Withers 2005). Curiously, insulin-like growth factor-binding protein, acid-labile subunit (IGFALS at 16p13.3), which binds IGF,1 shows a monoallelic pattern of expression suggestive of imprinting (Sano et al. 2001). Also, elderly men exhibit increased rates of metabolism, caloric consumption, and levels of sympathetic nervous system activity, which all tend to accelerate the effects of aging (Poehlman et al. 1997). As predicted earlier, if madumnal inclusive fitness increases with longevity promotion and the creation of intergenerational transfer resource providers for later-stage partuition, then padumnal line counterstrategies to enhance inclusive fitness ought to also be apparent. We suggest that the reproductive male preference for women in younger stages of reproductive histories is such a counterstrategy because this maximizes the padumnal relatedness inherent (through inbreeding, higher male reproductive variance, and incest) in the maternal genome. This is in fact the case that human males (Buss 2006) sexual preference in “short-term investments” are young fertile females in ripe ovulatory cycles—even at a subliminal
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
67
level perhaps communicated by pheromones (Miller, Tybur, and Jordan 2007). It is not at all obvious or given that human males should desire young females. To illustrate, bonobo males prefer mating with older, more experienced females (Small 2002). These matriarchal-based bonobo social societies are madumnally inbred, so the rate in change of relatedness asymmetries does not follow the same course as in human reproductive life spans, thus, the age-preferred sexually appetitive behavior differs. PD as a Disease of Evolutionary Conflict over Aging Our kinship theory of gene conflict in aging predicts that alleles to affect the aging process exhibit asymmetric fitness consequences for matrilineal and patrilineal kin. Dysregulation leading to loss of function of matrilinecontrolled longevity-promoting allele expression will increase PD susceptibility if the downstream biochemical phenotype involves mitochondrial dysfunction, increase in ROS, increased protein aggregation, or pro-apoptoic pathway activation. We made an extended case for the involvement of imprinted genes on PD susceptibility. The conflict over mitochondrial function is fundamental to the case that PD risk is impacted by intragenomic conflict. The seven maternally controlled subunits of complex I (via mtDNA) and one X-linked gene are juxtaposed with the at least two paternally imprinted nuclear DNA subunits (NDUFS4 and NDUFA4) pivotal for energy production. Further we demonstrated a genetic struggle over building and maintaining a dopaminergic diencephalon/midbrain, regulating behavior and longevity via the catecholaminergic system, and protein growth and restraint. We showed that females benefit most in the struggle over longevity because of the X-linked and epigenetic gene control of aging. Our kinship theory of aging predicts that this is the case because female aged helpers (especially with high madumnal relatedness to kin) are prerequisite for madumnal inclusive fitness via intergenerational transfers, which enable reproductive females to reproduce more frequently. This leaves males more susceptible to acceleration in aging either by design or as a side effect of the evolved gene system regulation over longevity. This scenario taken in consideration with the vulnerable functional haploidy that imprinted genetics involves, presents us with a theoretical tool to understand why PD even exists, and why aged males are most susceptible. CONCLUSION We situated our findings within a larger theoretical framework, the kinship theory of gene imprinting, which has successfully explained the phenotypic
68
Dementia
expression of dozens of imprinted genotypes. We provisionally extend the theory to include the domain of aging as a ground for intragenomic conflict. Based on phylogenetic comparisons, genetics of menopause and aging, free radical theory of aging, resource transfer models, models of maternal care and genomic imprinting, and the implication of imprinting in the etiology of numerous neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative diseases, we suggest that not only is aging and longevity subject to the influence of gene conflict, but that PD results when the epigenetic struggle between matrilineal and patrilineal inclusive fitness becomes dysregulated—namely shifts toward loss of function and gain of function in matrilineal and patrilineal alleles, respectively. Thus, while still retaining its identity as a heterogeneous condition with complex etiopathogenesis, PD can also be studied within an evolutionary perspective for clues to evolution of aging itself. That is, PD exists as a uniquely human disease because of dysregulation in the evolutionarily recent struggle to maintain an evolutionarily stable strategy to maximize inclusive fitness of both padumnal and madumnal gene lineages.
REFERENCES Abdolmaleky, H. M., S. Thiagalingam, and M. Wilcox. 2005. Genetics and epigenetics in major psychiatric disorders: Dilemmas, achievements, applications, and future scope. American Journal of Pharmacogenomics 5: 149–160. Ahlemeyer, B., S. Kolker, Y. Zhu, G. F. Hoffman, and J. Krieglstein. 2002. Increase in glutamate-induced neurotoxicity by activated astrocytes involves stimulation of protein kinase C. Journal of Neurochemistry 82: 504–515. Alavian, K. N., C. Scholz, and H. H. Simon. 2008. Transcriptional regulation of mesenchephalic dopaminergic neurons: The full circle of life and death. Movement Disorders 23 (3): 319–328. Allen, R. G., and A. K. Balin. 1989. Oxidative influence on development and differentiation: An overview of a free radical theory of development. Free Radical Biological Medicine 6: 631–661. Ang, S. L., and J. Rossant. 1994. HNF-3 beta is essential for node and notochord formation in mouse development. Cell 78: 561–574. Anvret, M., M. Nordenskjold, L. Stolpe, L. Johansson, and K. Brondum-Nielsen. 1991. Molecular analysis of 4p deletion associated with Wolf-Hirschhorn syndrome moving the “critical segment” towards the telomere. Human Genetics 86: 481–483. Athanassiadou, A., G. Voutsinas, L. Psiouri, E. Leroy, M. H. Polymeropoulus, A. Illias, G. M. Maniatis, and T. Papapetropoulos. 1999. Genetic analysis of families with Parkinson disease that carry the Ala53Thr mutation in the gene encoding alpha-synuclein. American Journal of Human Genetics 65: 555–558.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
69
Bassett, S. S., D. Avramopoulos, and D. Fallin. 2002. Evidence for parent of origin effect in late-onset Alzheimer ’s disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics 114 (6): 679–686. Battaini, F. 2001. Protein kinase C isoforms as therapeutic targets in nervous system disease states. Pharmacology Research 44: 353–361. Benz, C. B., and C. Yau. 2008. Ageing, oxidative stress and cancer: Paradigms in parallax. Nature Reviews Cancer 8 (11): 875–879. Bergman, O., A. Hakansson, L. Westberg, A. C. Belin, O. Sydow, L. Olson, B. Holmberg, et al. 2009. Do polymorphisms in transcription factors LMX1A and LMX1B influence the risk of Parkinson’s disease? Journal of Neural Transmission 116 (3): 333–338. Bernheimer, H., W. Berkmayer, O. Hornykiewicz, K. Jellinger, and F. Seitelberger. 1973. Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington. Journal of Neurological Science 20: 415–455. Bertram, L., D. Blacker, K. Mullin, D. Keeney, J. Jones, S. Basu, S. Yhu, et al. 2000. Evidence for genetic linkage of Alzheimer ’s disease to chromosome 10q. Science 290: 2302–2303. Betarbet, R., R. M. Canet-Aviles, T. B. Sherer, P. G. Mastroberardino, C. McLendon, J. H. Kim, S. Lund, et al. 2006. Intersecting pathways to neurodegeneration in Parkinson’s disease: Effects of the pesticide rotenone on DJ-1, alphasynuclein, and the ubiquitin-proteasome system. Neurobiology of Disease 22: 404–420. Blagitko, N., S. Mergenthaler, U. Schulz, H. A. Wollmann, W. Craigen, T. Eggermann, H. H. Ropers, and V. M. Kalscheur. 2000. Human GRB10 is imprinted and expressed from the paternal and maternal allele in a highly tissue- and isoform- specific fashion. Human Molecular Genetics 9: 1587–1595. Bonifati, V. 2006. Parkinson’s disease: LRRK2-G2019S mutation: Opening a novel era in Parkinson’s disease genetics. European Journal Human Genetics 14: 1061–1062. Borglum, A. D., G. Kirov, N. Craddock, O. Mors, W. Muir, V. Murray, I. McKee, et al. 2003. Possible parent-of-origin effect of dopa decarboxylase in susceptibility to bipolar affective disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics B 117: 18–22. Bostantjopolou, S., Z. Katsarou, A. Papadimitriou, V. Veletza, G. Hatzigeorgiou, and A. Lees. 2001. Clinical features of parkinsonian patients with the alphasynuclein (G209A) mutation. Movement Disorders 16: 1007–1013. Bras, J., A. Singleton, M. R. Cookson, and J. Hardy. 2008. Emerging pathways in genetic Parkinson’s disease: Potential role of ceramide metabolism in Lewy body disease. FEBS Journal 275: 5767–5773. Brewer, S., W. Feng, J. Huang, S. Sullivan, and T. Williams. 2004. Wnt1-Cremediated deletion of AP-2α causes multiple neural crest-related defects. Developmental Biology 267 (1): 135–152. Bu, G. 2009. Apolipoprotein E and its receptors in Alzheimer ’s disease: Pathways, pathogenesis and therapy. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10: 333–344.
70
Dementia
Budde, S. M., L. P. van den Heuvel, A. J. Janssen, R. J. Smeets, C. A. Buskens, L. DeMeirleir, R. Van Coster. 2000. Combined enzymatic complex I and III deficiency associated with mutations in the nuclear encoded NDUFS4 gene. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 275 (1): 63–68. Buettner, V. L., J. A. Longmate, M. E. Barish, J. R. Mann, and J. Singer-Sam. 2004. Analysis of imprinting in mice with uniparental duplication of proximal chromosome 7 and 15 by use of a custom oligonucleotide microarray. Mammalian Genome 15 (3): 199–209. Bunzel, R., I. Blumcke, S. Cichon, S. Normann, J. Schramm, P. Propping, and M. M. Nothen. 1998. Polymorphic imprinting of the serotonin-2A (5-HT2A) receptor gene in human adult brain. Brain Research and Molecular Brain Research 59 (1): 90–92. Burt A., and R. Trivers. 1998. Genetic conflicts in genomic imprinting. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 265: 2293–2397. Burt, A., and R. Trivers. 2006. Genes in conflict. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Buss, D. 2006. Strategies of human mating. Psychological Topics 2: 239–260. Byford, M., C. Brayne, I. McKeith, M. Chatfield, P. Ince, and F. Matthews. 2009. Lewy bodies and neuronal loss in subcortical areas and disability in nondemented older people: A population neuropathological cohort study. BMC Geriatrics 15: 9–22. Calado, R. T., and N. S. Young. 2009. Telomere diseases. New England Journal of Medicine 361 (2): 2353–2365. Cant, M. A., and R. A. Johnstone. 2008. Reproductive conflict and the separation of reproductive generations in humans. PNAS 105 (14): 5332–5336. Cardoso, S. M., P. I. Moreira, P. Agostinho, C. Pereira, and C. R. Oliveira. 2005. Neurodegenerative pathways in Parkinson’s disease: Therapeutic strategies. Current Drug Targets: CNS Neurological Disorders 4: 405–419. Catala, A. 2009. Lipid peroxidation of membrane phospholipids generates hydroxy-alkenals and oxidized phospholipids active in physiological and/ or pathological conditions. Chemistry and Physics of Lipids 157 (1): 1–11. Catarsi, S., and P. Drapeau. 1997. Requirement for tyrosine phosphatase during serotonergic neuromodulation by protein kinase C. Journal of Neuroscience Research 17: 5792–5797. Chandrashekar, V., and A. Bartke. 2003. The role of insulin-like growth factor-1 in neuroendocrine function and the consequent effects on sexual maturation: Inferences from animal models. Reproductive Biology 3: 7–28. Charalambous, M., F. M. Smith, W. R. Bennett, T. E. Crew, F. Mackenzie, and A. Ward. 2003. Disruption of the imprinted GRB10 gene leads to disproportionate overgrowth by an Igf2-independent mechanism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100: 8292–8297. Chu, C. T. 2009. Tickled PINK1: Mitochondrial homeostasis and autophagy in recessive Parkinsonism. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta, doi: 10.1016/j. bbadis.2009.06.012.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
71
Crespi, B. 2008. Genomic imprinting in the development and evolution of psychotic spectrum conditions. Biological Reviews 83: 441–493. Cullen, V., M. Lindfors, J. Ng, A. Paetau, E. Swinton, P. Kolodziej, H. Boston, et al. 2009. Cathepsin D expression level affects alpha-synuclein processing, aggregation, and toxicity in vivo. Molecular Brain 2 (5): 1–17. Cutler, R. G., and M. P. Mattson. 2001. Sphingomyelin and ceramide as regulators of development and lifespan. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 122: 895–908. Dagda, R. K., S. J. Cherra, S. M. Kulich, A. Tandon, D. Park, and C. T. Chu. 2009. Loss of PINK1 function promotes mitophagy through effects on oxidative stress and mitochondrial fission. Journal of Biological Chemistry 284 (20): 13843–13855. Dahl, H. H. 1998. Getting to the nucleus of mitochondrial disorders: Identification of respiratory chain-enzyme genes causing Leigh syndrome. American Journal of Human Genetics 63: 1594–1597. D’Amato, R. J., R. M. Zweig, P. J. Whitehouse, G. L. Wenk, H. S. Singer, R. Mayeux, D. L. Price, and S. H. Snyder. 1987. Aminergic system in Alzheimer ’s and Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology 22: 229–236. Damberg, M., M. Eller, M. Tonissaar, L. Oreland, and J. Harro. 2001. Levels of transcription factors AP-2α and AP-2β in the brainstem are correlated to monamine turnover in the rat forebrain. Neuroscience Letters 313: 102–104. Dauer, W., and S. Przedborski. 2003. Parkinson’s disease: Mechanisms and models. Neuron 39: 889–909. David, R. B., M. R. Gomez, and H. Okazaki. 1970. Necrotizing encephalomyelopathy (Leigh). Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 12: 436–445. Davies, W., A. R. Isles, and L. S. Wilkinson. 2001. Imprinted genes and mental dysfunction. Annals of Medicine 33 (6): 428–436. Davies, W., A. R. Isles, and L. S. Wilkinson. 2005. Imprinted gene expression in the brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review 29: 421–430. Day, T., and R. Bonduriansky. 2004. Intralocus sexual conflict can drive the evolution of genomic imprinting. Genetics 167: 1537–1546. De Benedictis, G., E. Falcone, G. Rose, R. Ruffolo, P. Spadafora, G. Baggio, S. Bertolini, et al. 1997. DNA multiallelic systems reveal gene/longevity associations not detected by diallelic systems: The APOB Locus. Human Genetics 99: 312–318. De Benedictis, G., L. Carotenuto, G. Carrieri, M. De Luca, E. Falcone, G. Rose, et al. 1998. Gene/longevity association studies at four autosomal loci (REN, THO, PARP, SOD2). European Journal Human Genetics 6: 534–541. DeLuca, M., G. Rose, M. Bonafe, S. Garasato, V. Greco, B. S. Weir, C. Franceschi, and G. De Benedictis. 2001. Sex-specific longevity associations defined by tyrosine hydroxylase-insulin-insulin growth factor 2 haplotypes on the 11p15.5 chromosomal region. Experimental Gerontology 36 (10): 1663–1671.
72
Dementia
DeLuca, M., N. V. Roshina, G. L. Geiger-Thornsberry, R. F. Lyman, E. G. Pasyukova, and T. F. Mackay. 2003. Dopa decarboxylase (Ddc) affects variation in Drosophila longevity. Nature Genetics 34 (4): 358–359. DePinho, R. A. 2000. The age of cancer. Nature 408: 248–255. Devine, M. J., and P. A. Lewis. 2008. Emerging pathways in genetic Parkinson’s disease: Tangles, Lewy bodies and LRRK2. FEBS Journal 275: 5748–5757. D’Souza, U. M., C. Russ, E. Tahir, J. Mill, P. McGuffin, P. Asherson, and I. W. Craig. 2004. Functional effects of a tandem duplication polymorphism in the 5 prime flanking region of the DRD4 gene. Biological Psychiatry 56: 691–697. Du, Y. L., B. D. Wilcox, M. Teitler, and J. J. Jeffrey. 1994. Isolation and characterization of the rat 5-hydroxytriptamine type 2 receptor promoter: constitutive and inducible activity in myometrial smooth muscle cells. Molecular Pharmacology 45: 1125–1131. Dulawa, S. C., D. K. Grandy, M. J. Low, M. P. Paulus, and M. A. Geyer. 1999. Dopamine D4 receptor-knock-out mice exhibit reduced exploration of novel stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience 19: 9550–9556. Eckert, D., S. Buhl, S. Weber, R. Jager, and H. Schorle. 2005. The AP-2 family of transcription factors. Genome Biology 6 (13): 246. Eggermann, T., K. Eggermann, and N. Schonherr. 2008. Growth retardation versus overgrowth: Silver-Russell syndrome is genetically opposite to BeckwithWidemann syndrome. Trends in Genetics 24: 195–204. Eisenegger, C., D. Knoch, R. P. Ebstein, L. R. R. Gianotti, P. S. Sandor, and E. Fehr. 2009. Dopamine receptor d4 polymorphism predicts the effect of L-DOPA on gambling behavior. Biological Psychiatry, doi: 101016/j.biopsych.2009.09.021. Eliot, M., and E. R. Maher. 1994. Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome. Journal of Medical Genetics 31: 560–564. Elstner, M., C. M. Morris, K. Heim, P. Lichtner, A. Bender, D. Mehta, C. Schulte, M. Sharma, et al. 2009. Single-cell expression profiling of dopaminergic neurons combined with association analysis identifies pyridoxal kinase as Parkinson’s disease gene. Annals of Neurology 66: 792–798. Emahazion, T., A. Beskow, U. Gyllensten, and A. J. Brookes. 1998. Intron based radiation hybrid mapping of 15 complex I genes of the human electron transport chain. Cytogenetics and Cell Genetics 82: 115–119. Epstein, D. J., A. P. McMahon, and A. L. Joyner. 1999. Regionalization of sonic hedgehog transcription along the anteroposterior axis of the mouse central nervous system is regulated by Hnf3-dependent and -independent mechanisms. Development 126: 281–292. Farooqui, A. A., and L. A. Horrocks. 1994. Excitotoxicity and neurological disorders: Involvement of membrane phospholipids. International Review of Neurobiology 36: 267–323. Farooqui, A. A., and L. A. Horrocks. 1998. Lipid peroxides in the free radical pathophysiology of brain diseases. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology 18 (6): 599–608.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
73
Feng, J., F. Bussiere, and S. Hekimi. 2001. Mitochondrial electron transport is a key determinant of life span in Caenorhabditis elegans. Developmental Cell 1: 633–644. Flint, J. 1992. Implications of genomic imprinting for psychiatric genetics. Psychological Medicine 22: 5–10. Fox, C. W., K. L. Scheibly, W. G. Wallin, L. J. Hitchcock, R. C. Stillwell, and B. P. Smith. 2006. The genetic architecture of life span and mortality rates: Gender and species differences in inbreeding load of two seed-feeding beetles. Genetics 174: 763–773. Francis, P. T., and E. K. Perry. 2007. Cholinergic and other neurotransmitter mechanisms in Parkinson’s disease, Parkinson’s disease dementia, and dementia with Lewy bodies. Movement Disorders (Suppl 17): S351–S357. Frantz, J. D., S. Giorgetti-Peraldi, E. A. Ottinger, and S. E. Shoelson. 1997. Human GRB-IR-beta/GRB10: Splice variants of an insulin and growth factor receptor-binding protein with PH and SH2 domains. Journal of Biological Chemistry 272: 2659–2667. Freed, W. J., J. Chen, C. M. Backman, C. M. Schwartz, T. Vazin, J. Cai, C. E. Spivak, C. R. Lupica, M. S. Rao, and X. Zeng. 2008. Gene expression profile of neuronal progenitor cells derived from hESCs: Activation of chromosome 11p15.5 and comparison to human dopaminergic neurons. PLoS One 1: 1–12. Fuchs, J., J. C. Mueller, P. Lichtner, C. Schulte, M. Munz, D. Berg, U. Wullner, M. Sharma, and T. Gasser. 2009. The transcription factor PITX3 is associated with sporadic Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging 30: 731–738. Fukuda, Y., M. Koga, M. Arai, E. Noguchi, T. Ohtsuki, Y. Horiuchi, H. Ishiguro, et al. 2006. Monoallelic and unequal allelic expression of the HTR2A gene in human brain and peripheral lymphocytes. Biological Psychiatry 60 (12): 1331–1335. Gallardo, G., O. M. Schluter, and T. C. Sudhof. 2008. A molecular pathway of neurodegeneration linking alpha-synuclein to ApoE and A-beta peptides. Nature Neuroscience 11: 301–308. Gaweda-Walerych, K., A. Maruszak, K. Safranow, M. Bialecka, G. KlodowskaDuda, K. Czyzewki, J. Slawek, et al. 2008. Mitochondrial DNA haplogroups and subhaplogroups are associated with Parkinson’s disease risk in a Polish PD cohort. Journal of Neural Transmission 115: 1521–1526. Gelernter, J., J. L. Kennedy, H. H. Van Tol, O. Civelli, and K. Kidd. 1992. The D4 dopamine receptor (DRD4) maps to distal 11p close to HRAS. Genomics 13: 208–210. Giasson, B. I., J. E. Duda, I. V. J. Murray, Q. Chen, J. M. Souza, H. I. Hurtig, H. Ischiropoulos, J. Q. Trojanowski, and V. M. Lee. 2000. Oxidative damage linked to neurodegeneration by selective alpha-synuclein nitration in synucleinopathy lesions. Science 290: 985–989. Gieger, C., L. Geistlinger, E. Altmaier, M. Hrabe de Angelis, F. Kronenberg, T. Meitinger, H.-W. Mewes, et al. 2008. Genetics meets metabolomics: A genomewide association study of metabolite profiles in human serum. PLoS Genet 4 (11): e1000282. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000282.
74
Dementia
Giles, R. E., H. Blanc, H. M. Cann, and D. C. Wallace. 1980. Maternal inheritance of human mitochondrial DNA. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 77 (11): 6715–6719. Giovannone, B., E. Lee, L. Laviola, F. Giorgino, K. A. Cleveland, and R. J. Smith. 2003. Two novel proteins that are linked to insulin-like growth factor (IGF-1) receptors by the GRB10 adaptor and modulate IGF-1 Signaling. Journal of Biological Chemistry 278 (34): 31564–31573. Girault, J.-A., and P. Greengard. 2004. The neurobiology of dopamine signaling. Archives of Neurology 61: 641–644. Goos, L. M., and I. Silverman. 2006. The inheritance of cognitive skills: Does genomic imprinting play a role? Journal of Neurogenetics 20: 19–40. Gowers, W. 1888. Diseases of the nervous system. Philadelphia: P. Blakiston, Son and Company. Greco, D., E. Zellmer, Z. Zhang, and E. Lewis. 1995. Transcription factor AP-2 regulates expression of the dopamineβ-hydroxylase gene. Journal of Neurochemistry 65: 510–516. Greer, E. L., and A. Brunet. 2005. FOXO transcription factors at the interface between longevity and tumor suppression. Oncogene 24: 7410–7425. Grima, B., A. Lamouroux, C. Boni, J.-F. Julien, F. Javoy-Agid, and J. Mallet. 1987. A single human gene encodes multiple tyrosine hydroxylases with different predicted functional characteristics. Nature 326: 707–711. Grunblatt, E., S. Mandel, J. Jacob-Hirsch, S. Zeligson, N. Amariglo, G. Rechavi, J. Li, R. Ravid, W. Roggendorf, P. Rieserer, and M. B. Youdim. 2004. Gene expression profiling of parkinsonian substantia nigra pars compacta: Alterations in ubiquitin-proteasome, heat shock protein, iron and oxidative stress regulated proteins, cell adhesion/cellular matrix and vesicle trafficking genes. Journal of Neural Transmission 111: 1543–1573. Guarente, L. 2006. Sirtuins as potential targets for metabolic syndrome. Nature 444: 868–74. Hahn, S. L., M. Hahn, U. J. Kang, and T. H. Joh. 1993. Structure of the rat aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase gene: Evidence for an alternative promoter usage. Journal Neurochemistry 60: 1058–1064. Haig, D. 1992. Genomic imprinting and the theory of parent-offspring conflict. Seminal Developmental Biology 3: 153–160. Haig, D. 1997. Parental antagonism, relatedness asymmetries and genomic imprinting. Proceedings for the Royal Society of London, Series B 264: 1657–1662. Haig, D. 1999. Asymmetric relations: Internal conflicts and the horror of incest. Evolution and Human Behavior 20: 83–98. Haig, D. 2000a. The kinship theory of genomic imprinting. Annual Review of Ecological Systems 31: 9–32. Haig, D. 2000b. Genomic imprinting, sex-biased dispersal, and social dispersal. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 907: 149–163. Haig, D. 2004. Genomic imprinting and kinship: How good is the evidence? Annual Review of Genetics 38: 553–585.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
75
Haig, D. 2007. Huddling: Brown fat, genomic imprinting and the warm inner glow. Current Biology 18: 172–174. Haig, D. 2010. Transfers and transitions: Parent-offspring conflict, genomic imprinting, and the evolution of human life history. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 (1): 1731–1735. Haig, D., and M. Westoby. 1989. Parent-specific gene expression and the triploid endosperm. American Nature 134: 147–155. Hardy, J., C. Huaiban, M. R. Cookson, K. Gwinn-Hardy, and A. Singleton. 2006. Genetics of Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonism. Annals of Neurology 60: 389–398. Harman, D. 1956. Aging: A theory based on free radical and radiation chemistry. Journal of Gerontology 11: 298–300. Hasilik, A., and E. F. Neufeld. 1980. Biosynthesis of lysosomal enzymes in fibroblasts: Synthesis as precursors of higher molecular weight. Journal of Biological Chemistry 255 (10): 4937–4945. Haslam, R. J., H. B. Koide, and B. A. Hemmings. 1993. Pleckstrin domain homology. Nature 363 (6427): 309–310. Hawi, Z., R. Segurado, J. Conroy, K. Sheehan, N. Lowe, A. Kirley, D. Shields, M. Fitzgerald, L. Gallagher, and M. Gill. 2005. Preferential transmission of paternal alleles at risk genes in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Human Genetics 77 (6): 958–965. Hayashi, T., C. Ishimori, K. Takahashi-Niki, T. Taira, Y.-C. Kim, H. Maita, C. Maita, H. Ariga, and S. M. M. Iguchi-Ariga. 2009. DJ-1 binds mitochondrial complex I and maintains its activity. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications, doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2009.10.025 Healy, D., P. Abou-Sleiman, T. Ozawa, A. J. Lees, K. Bhatia, K. R. Ahmadi, U. Wullner, et al. 2004. A functional polymorphism regulating dopamine beta-hydroxylase influences against Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology 55: 443–446. Hekimi, S., and L. Guarente. 2003. Genetics and the specificity of the aging process. Science 299: 1351–1356. Hemberger, M., and U. Zechner. 2004. Genetic and genomic approaches to study placental development. Cytogenetics and Genome Research 105: 257–269. Hensch, T., H.-L. Wargelius, U. Herold, A. Strobel, L. Oreland, and B. Brocke. 2008. Electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of polymorphisms in the transcription factor AP-2β coding gene. Neuroscience Letters 436: 67–71. Hirschhorn, K., H. L. Cooper, and I. L. Firschein. 1965. Deletion of short arms of chromosome 4-5 in a child with defects of midline fusion. Humangenetik 1: 479–482. Hitchler, M. J., and F. R. Domann. 2007. An epigenetic perspective on the free radical theory of development. Free Radical Biology and Medicine 43: 1023–1036. Ho, C. C. Y., H. J. Rideout, E. Ribe, C. M. Troy, and W. Dauer. 2009. The Parkinson’s disease protein leucine-rich repeat kinase 2 transduces death signals via
76
Dementia
Fas-associated protein with death domain and caspase-8 in a cellular model of neurodegeneration. Journal of Neuroscience 29 (4): 1011–1116. Houssa, B., D. Schaap, J. van der Wal, K. Goto, H. Kondo, A. Yamakawa, M. Shibata, T. Takenawa, and W. J. van Blitterswik. 1997. Cloning of a novel human diacylglycerol kinase (DGKu) containing three cysteine-rich domains, a proline-rich region, and a pleckstrin homology domain with an overlapping Ras-associating domain, Journal of Biological Chemistry 272: 10422–10428. Huang, H. S., A. Matevossian, C. Whittle, S. Y. Kim, A. Schumacher, S. P. Baker, and S. Akbarian. 2007. Prefrontal dysfunction in schizophrenia involves mixed-lineage leukemia 1-regulated histone methylation at GABAergic gene promoters. Neurobiology of Disease 27 (42): 11254–11262. Hughes, A. J., S. E. Daniel, L. Kilford, and A. J. Lees. 1992. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: A clinicopathological study of 100 cases. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55: 181–184. Hyland, K., R. A. Surtees, C. Rodeck, and P. T. Clayton. 1992. Aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase deficiency: Clinical features, diagnosis, and treatment of a new inborn error of neurotransmitter amine synthesis. Neurology 42: 1980–1988. Ichinose, H., Y. Kurosawa, K. Titani, K. Fujita, and T. Nagatsu. 1989. Isolation and characterization of a cDNA clone encoding human aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase. Biochemistry and Biophysics Research Communication 164: 1024–1030. Imai, S., C. M. Armstrong, M. Kaeberlein, and L. Guarente. 2000. Transcriptional silencing and longevity protein Sir2 is an NAD-dependent histone deactylase. Nature 402: 795–800. Ishikawa, S., T. Taira, T. Niki, K. Takahashi-Niki, H, Laita, H. Ariga, and S. M. M. Iguchi-Ariga. 2009. Oxidative status of DJ-1-dependent activation of dopamine synthesis through interaction of tyrosine hydroxylase and 4-dihydroxyl-L-phenylalanine (L-DOPA) decarboxylase with DJ-1. Journal of Biological Chemistry 284 (42): 28832–28844. Ivey, P. K. 2000. Cooperative reproduction in Ituri Forest hunter-gatherers: Who cares for Efe infants? Current Anthropology 41 (5): 856–866. Jakes, R., M. G. Spillantini, and M. Goedert. 1994. Identification of two distinct synucleins from human brain. FEBS Letters 345: 27–32. Juyal, R. C., M. Das, S. Punia, M. Behari, G. Nainwal, S. Singh, P. V. Swaminath, S. T. Govindappa, S. Jayaram, U. B. Muthane, and B. K. Thelma. 2006. Genetic susceptibility to Parkinson’s disease among South and North Indians: I. Role of polymorphisms in dopamine receptor and transporter genes and association of DRD4 120-bp duplication marker. Neurogenetics 7: 223–229. Kaplan, H., K. Hill, J. Lancaster, and A. M. Hurtado. 2000. A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 9 (4): 156–185.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
77
Kervinen, K., M. J. Savolainen, J. Salokannel, A. Hynninen, J. Heikkinen, C. Ehnholm, M. J. Koistinen, and Y. A. Kesaniemi. 1994. Apolipoprotien E and B polymorphisms-longevity factors assessed in nonagenarians. Atherosclerosis 105: 89–95. Kim, H. S., S. J. Hong, M. S. LeDoux, and K. S. Kim. 2001. Regulation of the tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine beta-hydroxylase genes by the transcription factor AP-2. Journal of Neurochemistry 76: 280–294. Kim, K. S., J. J. Kim, J. C. M. Deckers, and M. F. Rothschild. 2004. Polar overdominant inheritance of a DLK1 polymorphism is associated with growth and fatness in pigs. Mammalian Genome 15: 552–559. Kishino, T. 2006. Imprinting in neurons. Cytogenetics and Genome Research 113: 209–214. Kittappa, R., W. W. Chang, R. B. Awatramani, and R. D. G. McKay. 2007. The foxa2 gene controls the birth and spontaneous degeneration of dopamine neurons in old age. PLoS Biology 5 (12): 2875–2884. Knudson, A. 1971. Mutation and cancer: Statistical study of retinoblastoma. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 68 (4): 820–823. Kobayashi, K., Y. Kurosawa, K. Fujita, and T. Nagatsu. 1989. Human dopamine beta-hydroxylase gene: Two mRNA types having different 3’-terminal regions are produced through alternative polyadenylation. Nucleic Acids Research 17 (3): 1089–1102. Kobayashi, H., H. Hiura, R. M. John, A. Sato, E. Otsu, N. Kobayashi, R. Suzuki, et al. 2009. DNA methylation errors at imprinted loci after assisted conception originate in the parental sperm. European Journal of Human Genetics 17: 1582–1591. Kontopoulos, E., J. D. Parvin, and M. B. Feany. 2006. Alpha-synuclein acts in the nucleus to inhibit histone acetylaion and promote neurotoxicity. Human Molecular Genetics 15: 3012–3023. Kozikowski, A. P., I. Nowak, P. A. Petukhov, R. Etcheberrigaray, A. Mohamed, M. Tan, N. Lewin, H. Hennings, L. L. Pearce, and P. M. Blumberg. 2003. New amide-bearing benzolactam-based protein kinase C modulators induced enhanced secretion of the amyloid precursor protein metabolite s APPα. Journal of Medical Chemistry 46: 364–373. Kratz, C. P., D. Steinemann, C. M. Miemeyer, B. Schlegelberger, E. Koscielniak, U. Kontny, and M. Zenke. 2007. Uniparental disomy at chromosome 11p15.5 followed by HRAS mutations in embryonal rhabdomyosarcoma: Lessons from Costello syndrome. Human Molecular Genetics 16 (4): 374–379. Kruger, R., W. Kuhn, T. Muller, D. Woitalla, M. Graeber, S. Kosel, H. Przuntek, J. T. Epplen, L. Schols, and O. Riess. 1998. Ala30Pro mutation in the gene encoding alpha-synuclein in Parkinson’s disease. Nature Genetics 18: 106–108. Kwon, S. H., J. C. Park, S. Ramachandran, S. D. Cha, K. Y. Kwon, J. K. Park, J. W. Park, I. Bae, and C. H. Cho. 2008. Loss of cyclin g1 expression in
78
Dementia
human uterin leiomyoma cells induces apoptosis. Reproductive Science 15 (4): 400–410. Lai, C. Q., L. D. Parnell, R. F. Lyman, J. M. Ordovas, and T. F. Mackay. 2007. Candidate gene affecting Drosophila life span identified by integrating microarray gene expression anaylsis and QTL mapping. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 128: 237–249. Lamb, J. A., G. Barnby, E. Bonora, N. Sykes, E. Bacchelli, F. Blasi, E. Maestrini, et al. 2005. Analysis of IMGSAC autism susceptibility loci: Evidence for sex limited and parent of origin specific effects. Journal of Medical Genetics 42: 132–137. Latourelle, J. C., N. Pankratz, A. Dumitiu, J. B. Wilk, S. Goldwurm, G. Pezzoli, C. B. Mariani, et al. 2009. Genomewide association study for onset age in Parkinson disease. BMC Medical Genetics 10: 98. Lautier, C., S. Goldwurm, A. Durr, B. Giovannone, W. G. Tsiaras, G. Pezzoli, A. Brice, and R. J. Smith. 2008. Mutations in the GIGYF2 (TNRC15) gene at the PARK11 locus in familial Parkinson disease. American Journal of Human Genetics 82: 822–833. Lazarou, M., M. McKenzie, A. Ohtake, D. R. Thorburn, and M. T. Ryan. 2007. Analysis of the assembly profiles for mitochondrial- and nuclear-DNAencoded subunits into Complex I. Molecular and Cellular Biology 27 (12): 4228–4237. Lee, M.-E., D. H. Temizer, J. A. Clifford, and T. Quertermous. 1991. Cloning of the GATA-binding protein that regulates endothelin-1 gene expression in endothelial cells. Journal of Biological Chemistry 266: 16188–16192. Lee, R. 2003. Rethinking the evolutionary theory of aging: Transfers, not births, shape senescence in social species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100 (16): 9637–9642. LeWitt, P. 2008. Levodopa for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease. NEJM 359: 2468–2476. Lin, W., E. Metzakopian, Y. E. Mavromatakis, N. Gao, N. Balaskas, H. Sasaki, J. Briscoe, et al. 2009. Foxa1 and foxa2 function both upstream of and cooperatively with Lmx1a and Lmx1b in a feedforward loop promoting mesodiencephalic dopaminergic neuron development. Developmental Biology 333: 386–396. Litvan, I., G. Halliday, M. Hallett, C. G. Goetz, W. Rocca, C. Duyckaerts, Y. Ben-Shlomo, et al. 2007. The etiopathogenesis of Parkinson’s disease and suggestions for future research. Part I. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 66: 251–257. Liu, F., and R. A. Roth. 1995. Grb-IR: A SH2-domain-containing protein that binds to the insulin receptor and inhibits its function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92: 10287–10291. Liu, G., M. H. Ghahremani, B. Banihashemi, and P. R. Albert. 2003. Diacylglycerol and ceramide formation induced by dopamine D2S receptors via Gbetasubunits in Balb/c-3T3 cells. American Journal of Physiology and Cellular Physiology 284: 640–648.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
79
Liu, G., L. Kleine, and R. L. Herbert. 1999. Advances in the signal transduction of ceramide and related sphingolipids. Critical Review of Clinical Laboratory Science 36: 511–573. Liu, P., Y. Lu, R. R. Recker, H.-W. Deng, and V. Dvornyk. 2010. Association analyses suggest multiple interaction effects of the methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase polymorphisms on timing of menarache and natural menopause in white women. Menopause: The Journal of the North American Menopause Society 17 (1): 185–190. Liu, S., I. Ninan, I. Antonova, F. Battaglia, F. Trinchese, A. Narasanna, N. Kolodilov, W. Dauer, R. D. Hawkins, and O. Arancio. 2004. Alpha-synuclein produces a long lasting increase in neurotransmitter release. EMBO Journal 23: 4506–4516. Lo, H. S., Z. Wang, Y. Hu, H. H. Yang, S. Gere, K. H. Buetow, and M. P. Lee. 2003. Allelic variation in gene expression is common in the human genome. Genome Research 13 (8): 1855–1862. Lotharius, J., and P. Brundin. 2002. Impaired dopamine storage resulting from alpha-synuclein mutations may contribute to the pathogenesis of Parkinson’s disease. Human Molecular Genetics 11: 2395–2407. Ludecke, B., B. Dworniczak, and K. Bartholome. 1995. A point mutation in the tyrosine hydroxylase gene associated with Segawa’s syndrome. Human Genetics 95: 123–125. Luedi, P. P., F. S. Dietrich, J. R. Weidman, J. M. Bosko, R. L. Jirtle, and A. J. Hartemink. 2007. Computational and experimental identification of novel human imprinted genes. Genome Research 17: 1723–1730. Luoma, P., A. Melberg, J. O’Rinne, J. A. Kaukonen, N. N. Nupponen, R. M. Chalmers, A. Oldfors, et al. 2004. Parkinsonism, premature menopause, and mitochondrial DNA polymerase gamma mutations: Clinical and molecular genetic study. Lancet 364: 875–882. Ma, G., M. Roth, J. Groskopf, F. Y. Tsai, S. H. Orkin, F. Grosveld, J. D. Engel, and D. I. Linzer. 1997. GATA-2 and GATA-3 regulate trophoblast-specific gene expression in vivo. Development 124: 907–914. Mallet, N., B. Ballion, C. LeMoine, and F. Gonon. 2006. Cortical inputs and GABA interneurons imbalance projection neurons in the striatum of parkinsonian rats. Journal of Neuroscience 26: 3875–3884. Marahrens, Y., B. Panning, J. Dausman, W. Strauss, and R. Jaenisch. 1997. Xist-deficient mice are defective in dosage compensation but not spermatogenesis. Genes and Development 11: 156–166. Mariani, E., M. C. Polidori, A. Cherubini, and P. Mecocci. 2005. Oxidative stress in brain aging, neurodegenerative and vascular diseases: An overview. Journal Chromatography B: Analytical, Technological, and Biomedical Life Sciences 827: 65–75. Marin, I. 2006. The Parkinson’s disease gene LRRK2: Evolutionary and structural insights. Molecular Biology and Evolution 23: 2423–2433. Mayeux, R., J. H. Lee, S. N. Romas, D. Mayo, V. Santana, J. Williamson, A. Clappa, et al. 2002. Chromosome-12 mapping of late-onset Alzheimer disease among Caribbean Hispanics. American Journal of Human Genetics 70: 237–243.
80
Dementia
McMahon, M., and E. Sabban. 1992. Regulation of expression of dopamineβ hydroxylase in PC12 cells by glucocorticoids and cyclic AMP analogues. Journal of Neurochemistry 59: 2040–2204. McNamara, P., R. Durso, and E. Harris. 2007. “Machiavellianism” and frontal dysfunction: Evidence from Parkinson’s disease (PD). Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 12 (4): 285–300. McNamara, P., R. Durso, and E. Harris. 2008. Alterations of the sense of self and personality in Parkinson’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23 (1): 79–84. Menheniott, T. R., K. Wodfine, R. Schulz, A. J. Wood, D. Monk, A. S. Giraud, H. S. Baldwin, G. E. Moore, and R. J. Oakey. 2008. Genomic imprinting of dopa decarboxylase in heart and reciprocal allelic expression with neighboring Grb10. Molecular and Cellular Biology 28 (1): 386–396. Merida, I., A. Avila-Flores, and E. Merino. 2008. Diacylglycerol kinases: At the hub of cell signaling. Biochemistry Journal 409: 1–18. Meurers, B. H., G. Dziewczapolski, T. Shi, A. Bittner, R. Kamme, and C. W. Shults. 2009. Dopamine depletion induces distinct compensatory gene expression changes in DARPP-32 signal transduction cascades of striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience 29 (21): 6828–6839. Miller, G., J. M. Tybur, and B. D. Jordan. 2007. Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: economic evidence for human estrus. Evolution and Human Behavior 28: 375–381. Miller, R. L., M. James-Kracke, G. S. Sun, and A. Y. Sun. 2009. Oxidative and inflammatory pathways in Parkinson’s disease. Neurochemistry Res. 34: 55–65. Moreira, P. I., S. L. Siedlak, G. Aliev, X. Zhu, A. D. Cash, M. A. Smith, and G. Perry. 2005. Oxidative stress mechanisms and potential therapeutics in Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neural Transmission 112: 921–932. Morison, I. M., J. P. Ramsay, and H. G. Spencer. 2005. A census of mammalian imprinting. Trends in Genetics 21 (8): 457–465. Moser, M., J. Ruschoff, and R. Buetnner. 1997. Comparative analysis of AP-2α and AP-2β gene expression during murine embryogenesis. Developmental Dynamics 1208: 115–124. Moser, M., A. Pscherer, C. Roth, J. Becker, G. Mucher, K. Zerres, C. Dixkens, et al. 1997. Enhanced apoptotic cell death of renal epithelium cells in mice lacking transcription factor AP-β. Genes and Development 11: 1938–1948. Muglia, P., A. Petronis, E. Mundo, S. Lander, T. Cate, and J. L. Kennedy. 2002. Dopamine D4 receptor and tyrosine hydroxylase genes in bipolar disorder: Evidence for a role of DRD4. Molecular Psychiatry 7 (8): 860–866. Murabito, J. M., Q. Yang, C. Fox, P. W. Wilson, and L. A. Cupples. 2005. Heritability of age at natural menopause in the Framingham Heart Study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 90: 3427–3430. Murphy, M. P. 2009. How mitochondria produce reactive oxygen species. Biochemistry Journal 417 (1): 1–13.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
81
Naf, D., L. A. Wilson, R. A. Bergstrom, R. S. Smith, N. C. Goodwin, A. Verkerk, G. J. van Ommen, S. L. Ackerman, W. N. Frankel, and J. C. Schimenti. 2001. Mouse models for the Wolf-Hirschhorn deletion syndrome. Human. Molecular Genetics 10: 91–98. Nagatsu, T., and H. Ichinose. 1991. Comparative studies on the structure of human tyrosine hydroxylase with those of the enzyme of various mammals. Comparative Biochememistry and Physiology 98: 203–210. Nishino, N., N. Kitamura, T. Hashimoto, and C. Tanaka. 1993 Transmembrane signaling systems in the brain of patients with Parkinson’s disease. Review Neuroscience 4 (2): 213–222. Nishino, N., N. Kitamura, T. Hashimoto, and C. Tanaka. 1989. Phorbol ester binding sites in human brain: Characterization, regional distribution, agecorrelation, and alterations in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience 1 (1): 19–26. Normark, B. B. 2006. Maternal kin groups and the origins of asymmetric genetic systems-genomic imprinting, haplodiploidy, and parthenogenesis. International Journal of Organic Evolution 60 (4): 631–642. Oldenhof, J., R. Vickery, M. Anafi, J. Oak, A. Ray, O. Schoots, T. Pawson, M. von Zastrow, and H. Van Tol. 1998. SH3-binding domains in the dopamine D4 receptor. Biochemistry 37: 15726–15736. Orr, W. C., S. N. Radyuk, L. Prabhudesai, D. Toroser, J. J. Benes, J. M. Luchak, R. J. Mockett, I. Rebrin, J. G. Hubbard, and R. S. Sohal. 2005. Overexpression of glutamate-cystein ligase extends life span in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Biological Chemistry 280: 37331–37338. Ottman, R., J. F. Annegers, W. A. Hanser, and L. T. Kurland. 1988. Higher risk of seizures in offspring of mothers than of fathers with epilepsy. American Journal of Medical Genetics 43: 257–264. Outeiro, T. F., E. Kontopoulos, S. M. Altman, I. Kufareva, K. E. Strathearn, A. M. Amore, C. B. Volk, et al. 2007. Sirtuin 2 inhibitors rescue alpha-synucleinmediated toxicity in models of Parkinson’s disease. Science 317: 516– 519. Padmanabhan, J., D. S. Park, L. A. Greene, and M. L. Shelanski. 1999. Role of cell cycle regulatory proteins in cerebellar granule neuron apoptosis. Journal of Neuroscience 19 (20): 8747–8756. Pankratz, N., J. B. Wilk, J. C. Latourelle, A. L. DeStefano, C. Halter, E. W. Pugh, K. F. Doheny, et al. 2009. Genomewide association study for susceptibility genes contributing to familial Parkinson disease. Human Genetics 124: 593–605. Papa, S., V. Petruzzella, S. Sasso, A. M. Sardanelli, A. Iuso, D. Panelli, R. Vitale, et al. 2009. Pathogenetic mechanisms in hereditary dysfunctions of complex I of the respiratory chain in neurological diseases. Biochimica Biophysica Acta 1787 (5): 502–517. Parkinson, J. 1817. An essay on the shaking palsy. London: Sherwood, Neely and Jones.
82
Dementia
Partridge, L., D. Gems, and D. J. Whithers. 2005. Sex and death: What is the connection? Cell 120: 461–472. Pashos, A., and D. H. McBurney. 2008. Kin relationships and the caregiving of grandparents, aunts, and uncles. Human Nature 19 (3): 311–330. Paus, S., G. Seeger, H. Brecht, J. Koster, M. El-Faddagh, M. Nothen, T. Klockgether, and U. Wullner. 2004. Association study of dopamine D2, D3, D4 receptor and serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms with sleep attacks in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 19 (6): 705–707. Poehlman, E. T., M. J. Toth, P. A. Ades, and J. Calles-Escandon. 1997. Gender differences in resting metabolic rate and noradrenaline kinetics in older individuals. European Journal of Clinical Investigations 27: 23–28. Pollard, K. S., D. Serre, X. Wang, H. Tao, E. Grundberg, T. J. Hudson, A. G. Clark, and K. Frazer. 2008. A genome-wide approach to identifying novel-imprinted genes. Human Genetics 122: 625–634. Polymeropoulos, M. H., J. J. Higgins, L. I. Golbe, W. G. Johnson, S. E. Ide, G. Di Iorio, G. Sanges, et al. 1996. Mapping of a gene for Parkinson’s disease to chromosome 4q21-23. Science 274: 1197–1199. Polymeropoulos, M. H., C. Lavedan, E. Leroy, S. E. Ide, A. Dehejia, A. Dutra, B. Pike, et al. 1997. Mutation in the alpha-synuclein gene identified in families with Parkinson’s disease. Science 299: 2045–2047. Pons, R., B. Ford, C. A. Chiriboga, P. T. Clayton, V. Hinton, K. Hyland, R. Sharma, and D. C. De Vivo. 2004. Aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase deficiency: Clinical features, treatment, and prognosis. Neurology 62: 1058–1065. Pyle, A., T. Foltynie, W. Tiangyou, C. Lambert, S. M. Keers, L. M. Allcock, J. Davison, et al. 2005. Mitochondrial DNA haplotype cluster UKJT reduces the risk of PD. Annals of Neurology 57: 564–567. Ran, X., H.-H. Miao, F.-S. Sheu, and D. Yan. 2003. Structural and dynamic characterization of a neuron-specific protein kinase C substrate, neurogranin. Biochemistry 42: 5143–5150. Ricketts, M., R. Hammer, P. Manowitz, F. Feng, J. Sage, R. Di Paola, and M. A. Menza. 1998. Association of long variants of the dopamine D4 receptor exon 3 repeat polymorphism with Parkinson’s disease. Clinical Genetics 54: 33–38. Royo, H., and J. Cavaille. 2008. Non-coding RNAs in imprinted gene clusters. Biology Cell 100: 149–166. Ross, O. A., A. T. Braithwaite, L. M. Skipper, J. Kachergus, M. M. Hulihan, F. A. Middleton, K. Nishioka, et al. 2008. Genomic investigations of alpha-synuclein multiplication and parkinsonism. Annals of Neurology 63: 743–750. Ruf, N., S. Bahring, D. Galetzka, G. Pliushch, F. C. Luft, P. Nurnberg, T. Haaf, G. Kelsey, and U. Zechner. 2007. Sequence-based bioinformatic prediction and QUASEP identify genomic imprinting of the KCNK9 potassium channel gene in mouse and human. Human Molecular Genetics 16 (21): 2591–2599. Rush, R. A., and L. D. Geffen. 1980. Dopamine beta-hydroxylase in health and disease. Critical Reviews of Clinical Laboratory Science 12 (3): 241–277.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
83
Saha, S., M. D. Guillily, A. Ferree, J. Lanceta, D. Chan, J. Ghosh, C. H. Hsu, et al. 2009. LRRK2 modulates vulnerability to mitochondrial dysfunction in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neurobiology of Disease 29 (29): 9210–9218. Sakane, F., S. Imai, M. Kai, S. Yasuda, and H. Kanoh. 2007. Diacylglycerol kinases: Why so many of them? Biochimica Biophysica Acta 1771: 793–806. Sandovici, I., M. Leppert, P. R. Hawk, A. Suarez, Y. Linares, and C. Sapienze. 2003. Familial aggregation of abnormal methylation of parental alleles at the IGF2/H19 and IGF2R differentially methylated regions. Human Molecular Genetics 12 (13): 1569–1578. Sano, Y., T. Shimada, H. Nakashima, R. H. Nicholson, J. F. Eliason, T. A. Kocarek, and M. S. H. Ko. 2001. Random monoallelic expression of three genes clustered within 60 kb of mouse t complex genomic DNA. Genome Research 11: 1833–1841. Sasaki, H., and B. L. Hogan. 1994. HNF-3 beta as a regulator of floor plate development. Cell 76: 103–115. Saunders, A. M., W. J. Strittmatter, D. Schmechel, P. H. St. George-Hyslop, M. A. Pericak-Vance, S. H. Joo, B. L. Rosi, et al. 1993. Association of apolipoprotein E allele E4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 43: 1467. Scherzer, C., J. Grass, Z. Liao, I. Pepiviani, B. Zheng, A. C. Eklund, P. A. Ney, et al. 2008. GATA transcription factors directly regulate the Parkinson’s diseaselinked gene alpha-synuclein. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 (31): 10907–10912. Selelstad, M. T., E. Minch, and L. L. Cavalli-Sforza. 1998. Genetic evidence for a higher female migration rate in humans. Nature Genetics 20: 278–280. Shadrina, M., T. Nikopensius, P. Slominsky, S. Illarioshkin, G. Bagyeva, E. Markova, I. Ivanova-Smolenskala, A. Kurg, S. Limborska, and A. Metspalu. 2008. Association study of sporadic Parkinson’s disease genetic risk factors in patients from Russia by APEX technology. Neuroscience Letters 405: 212–216. Sharon, R., I. Bar-Joseph, M. Frosch, D. Walsh, J. Hamilton, and D. Selkoe. 2003. The formation of highly soluble oligomers of alpha-synuclein is regulated by fatty acids and enhanced Parkinson’s disease. Neuron 37 (4): 583–595. Shimura, H., N. Hattori, S. Kubo, Y. Mizuno, S. Asakawa, S. Minoshima, N. Shimizu, et al. 2000. Familial Parkinson disease gene product, parkin, is a ubiquitinprotein ligase. Nature Genetics 25: 302–305. Skulachev, V. P. 1998. Cytochrome c in the apoptotic and antioxidant cascades. FEBS Letters 423 (3): 275–280. Small, M. F. 2002. Introduction to part 3. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, ed. C. Boesch, G. Hohmann, and L. F. Marchant, 153–155. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Smidt, M. P., C. H. Asbreuk, J. J. Cox, H. Chen, R. L. Johnson, and J. P. Burbach. 2000. A second independent pathway for development of mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons requires Lmx1b. Nature Neuroscience 3 (4): 337–341.
84
Dementia
Smidt, M. P., and J. P. Burbach. 2007. How to make a mesodiencephalic dopaminergic neuron. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8 (1): 21–32. Sohal, R. S., and R. G. Allen. 1985. Relationship between metabolic rate, free radicals, differentiation and aging: A unified theory. In Molecular biology of aging, basic life sciences, ed. A. D. Woodhead, A. D. Blackett, and A. Hollander, 35: 75–104. New York: Plenum. Srinivasan, B. S., J. Doostzadeh, F. Absalan, S. Mohandessi, R. Jalili, S. Bigdeli, J. Wang, et al. 2008. Whole genome survey of coding SNPs reveals a reproducible determinant of Parkinson disease. Human Mutation 30 (2): 228–238. Standaert, D. G., and I. Cantuti-Castelvetri. 2008. Sex differences in Parkinsons disease. In Sex differences in the brain: From genes to behavior, ed. J. B. Becker, K. J. Berkley, N. Geary, E. Hampson, J. P. Herman, and E. A. Young, 455–464. New York: Oxford University Press. Stover, S. K., G. A. Gushansky, J. J. Salmen, and C. S. Gardiner. 2000. Regulation of gamma-glutamte-cysteine ligase expression by oxidative stress in the mouse preimplantation embryo. Toxicology and Applied Pharmacology 168: 153–159. Sun, M., J. C. Latourelle, G. F. Wooten, M. F. Lew, C. Klein, H. A. Shill, L. I. Golbe, et al. 2006. Influence of heterozygosity for parkin mutation on onset age in familial Parkinson’s disease: The GenePD study. Annals of Neurology 63 (6): 826–832. Sutherland, G., G. Mellick, J. Newman, K. L. Double, J. Stevens, L. Lee, D. Rowe, R. Silburn, and G. M. Halliday. 2008. Haplotype analysis of the IGF2-INS-TH gene cluster in Parkinson’s disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 147B: 495–499. Swaans, R. J. M., P. Rondot, W. O. Renier, L. P. W. J. van den Heuvel, G. Steenbergen-Spanjers, and R. A. Wevers. 2000. Four novel mutations in the tyrosine hydroxylase gene in patients with infantile parkinsonism. Annals of Human Genetics 64: 25–31. Swerdlow, R. H., J. K. Parks, J. N. Davis, D. S. Cassarino, P. A. Trimmer, L. J. Currie, J. Dougherty, et al. 1998. Matrilineal inheritance of complex I dysfunction in multigenerational Parkinson’s disease family. Annals of Neurology 44: 873–881. Taira, T., Y. Saito, T. Niki, S. M. Iguchi-Ariga, K. Takahashi, and H. Ariga. 2004. DJ-1 has a role I antioxidative stress to prevent cell death. EMBO Report 5: 430. Tarou, L. R., M. A. Bloomsmith, M. P. Hoff, J. M. Erwin, and T. L. Maple. 2002. The behavior of aged great apes. In Interdisciplinary topics in gerontology, ed. J. M. Erwin and P. R. Hof, 31: 209–231. Basel: Karger. Tehranian, R., S. Montoya, A. Van Laar, T. G. Hastings, and R. G. Perez. 2006. Alpha-synuclein inhibits aromatic amino decarboxylase activity in dopaminergic cells. Journal of Neurochemistry 99: 1188–1196.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
85
Thies, U., E. Back, G. Wolff, T. Schroeder-Kurth, H.-D. Hager, and K. Schroder. 1992. Clinical, cytogenetic and molecular investigations in three patients with Wolf-Hirschhorn syndrome. Clinical Genetics 42: 201–205. Thompson, M. E., J. H. Jones, A. E. Pusey, S. Brewer-Marsden, J. Goodall, D. Marsden, T. Matsuzawa, et al. 2007. Aging and fertility patterns in wild chimpanzees provide insights into the evolution of menopause. Current Biology 17: 2150–2156. Toiber, D., D. S. Greenberg, and H. Soreq. 2009. Pro-apototic protein-protein interactions of the extended N-AChE terminus. Journal of Neural Transmission, June 16. E-pub ahead of print, PMID: 19533292. Tong, Q., G. Dalgin, H. Xu, C.-N. Ting, J. M. Leiden, and G. S. Hotamisligil. 2000. Function of GATA transcription factors in preadipocyte-adipocyte transition. Science 290: 134–138. Tothova, Z., and D. G. Gilliland. 2007. FoxO transcription factors and stem cell homeostasis: Insights from the hemtopoietic system. Cell Stem Cell 1: 140–152. Tower, J. 2006. Sex-specific regulation of aging and apoptosis. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development 127: 705–718. Tsai, F.-Y., G. Keller, F. C. Kuo, M. Weiss, J. Chen, M. Rosenblatt, F. W. Alt, and S. H. Orkin. 1994. An early haematopoietic defect in mice lacking the transcription factor GATA2. Nature 371: 221–226. Tu-Sekine, B., and D. M. Raben. 2009. Regulation of DGK-θ. Journal of Cellular Physiology 220: 548–552. Ubeda, F., and J. F. Wilkins. 2008. Imprinted gene and human disease: An evolutionary perspective. In Genomic Imprinting, 101–113. New York: Springer. Ugalde, C., R. J. R. J. Janssen, L. P. van den Heuvel, J. A. M. Smeitink, and L. G. J. Nijtmans. 2004. Differences in assembly or stability of complex I and other mitochondrial OXPHOS complexes in inherited complex I deficiency. Human Molecular Genetics 13 (6): 659–667. Urdinguio, R. G., J. Sanchez-Mut, and M. Esteller. 2009. Epigenetic mechanisms in neurological diseases: genes, syndromes, and therapies. Lancet Neurology 8: 1056–1072. Valente, E. M., P. M. Abou-Sleiman, V. Caputo, M. M. Muqit, K. Harvey, S. Gispert, Z. Ali, et al. 2004. Hereditary early-onset Parkinson’s disease caused by mutations in PINK1. Science 304 (5674): 1158–1160. Van Asselt, K. M., H. S. Kok, H. Putter, C. C. Wijmenga, P. H. M. Peeters, Y. T. van der Schouw, D. E. Grobbee, E. R. te Velde, S. Mosselman, and P. L. Pearson. 2004. Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative trait loci influencing variation in human menopausal age. American Journal of Human Genetics 74: 444–453. Van den Heuvel, L., W. Ruitenbeek, R. Smeets, Z. Gelman-Kohan, O. Elpeleg, J. Loeffen, F. Trijbels, E. Mariman, D. de Bruijn, and J. Smeitink. 1998. Demonstration of a new pathogenic mutation in human complex I deficiency: A
86
Dementia
5-bp duplication in the nuclear gene encoding the 18-kD (AQDQ) subunit. American Journal of Human Genetics 62: 262–268. Veumeulen, C. J., R. Bijlsma, and V. Loeschcke. 2008. A major QTL affects temperature sensitive adult lethality and inbreeding depression in life span in Drosophila melanogaster. BMC Evolutionary Biology 8: 297. Voorhuis, M., N. C. Onland-Moret, Y. T. van der Schouw, B. C. J. M. Fauser, and F. J. Broekmans. 2010. Human studies on genetics of the age at natural menopause: A systematic review. Human Reproduction Update, doi: 10.1093/ humupd/dmp055. Wagey, R., J. Hu, S. L. Pelech, L. A. Raymond, and C. Krieger. 2001. Modulation of NMDA-mediated excitotoxicity by protein kinase C. Journal of Neurochemistry 78: 715–726. Warren, D. S., J. C. Morrell, H. W. Moser, D. Valle, and S. J. Gould. 1998. Identification of PEX10, the gene defective in complementation group 7 of the peroxisome-biogenesis disorders. American Journal of Human Genetics 63 (2): 347–359. Wayne, M. L., M. Telonis-Scott, L. M. Bono, L. Harshman, A. Kopp, S. V. Nuzhdin, and L. M. McIntyre. 2007. Simple mode of inheritance of transcriptional variation in male Drosophila melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 18577–18582. Weinstein, L. S. 2001. The role of tissue-specific imprinting as a source of phylogenetic heterogeneity in human disease. Biological Psychiatry 50 (12): 927–931. Whittington, J., A. Holland, T. Webb, J. Butler, D. Clarke, and H. Boer. 2004. Cognitive abilities and genotype in a population-based sample of people with Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities Research 48 (pt. 2): 172–187. Wild, P., and I. Dikic. 2010. Mitochondria get a Parkin’ ticket. Nature Cell Biology 12 (2): 104–106. Wilkins, J. F., and D. Haig. 2003a. What good is genomic imprinting: The function of parent-specific gene expression. Nature Reviews Genetics 4: 1–10. Wilkins, J. F., and D. Haig. 2003b. Inbreeding, maternal care, and genomic imprinting. Journal of Theoretical Biology 221: 559–564. Wilkinson, L. S., W. Davies, and A. R. Isles. 2007. Genomic imprinting effects on brain development and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 832–843. Williams-Gray, C. H., T. Foltynie, C. E. G. Brayne, T. W. Robbins, and R. A. Barker. 2007. Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s disease cohort. Brain 130 (7): 1787–1798. Wilson, C. J., and Y. Kawaguchi. 1996. The origins of two-state spontaneous membrane potential fluctuations of neostriatal spiny neurons. Journal of Neuroscience 16: 2397–2410. Wolf, J. B., J. M. Cheverud, C. Roseman, and R. Hager. 2008. Genome-wide analysis reveals a complex pattern of genomic imprinting in mice. PLoS Genetics 4 (6): e1000091.
Epigenetic and Gene Imprinting Effects in Parkinson’s Disease
87
Wolf, U., H. Reinwein, R. Porsch, R. Schroter, and H. Baitsch. 1965. Defizienz an den kurzen Armen eines Chromosoms Nr. 4. Humangenetik 1: 397–413. Wood, A. J., and R. J. Oakey. 2006. Genomic imprinting in mammals: Emerging themes and established theories. PLoS Genetics 2: 1677–1685. Wu, D. C., P. Teismann, K. Tieu, M. Vila, V. Jackson-Lewis, H. Ischiropoulos, and S. Przedborski. 2003. NADPH oxidase mediates oxidative stress in the 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1, 2, 3, 6-tetrahydropyridine model of Parkinson’s disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100: 6145–6150. Xu, J., S. Y. Kao, F. J. S. Lee., W. Song, L. W. Jin, and B. A. Yankner. 2002. Dopaminedependent neurotoxicity of alpha synuclein: A mechanism for selective neurodegeneration in Parkinson’s disease. Nature Medicine 8: 6000–6006. Zabetian, C. P., G. M. Anderson, S. G. Buxbaum, R. C. Elston, H. Ichinose, T. Nagatsu, K. S. Kim, et al. 2001. A quantitative-trait analysis of human plasma-dopamine beta-hydroxylase activity: Evidence for a major functional polymorphism at the DBH locus. American Journal of Human Genetics 68: 515–522. Zhang, W., T. Wang, Z. Pei, D. S. Miller, X. Wu, M. L. Block, B. Wilson. 2005. Aggregated alpha-synuclein activates microglia: A process leading to disease progression in Parkinson’s disease. FASEB Journal 19: 533–542. Zhou, Q.-Y., C. J. Quaife, and R. D. Palmiter. 1995. Targeted disruption of the tyrosine hydroxylase gene reveals that catecholamines are required for mouse fetal development. Nature 374: 640–643. Zulian, S. E., M. G. Ilincheta de Boschero, and N. M. Giusto. 2006. Insulin promotes diacylglycerol kinase activation by different mechanisms in rat cerebral cortex synaptosomes. Journal of Neuroscience Research 84 (5): 1012–1019.
Chapter 3
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction: A Redoubtable Trio in Alzheimer’s Disease Pathogenesis Sónia C. Correia, Renato X. Santos, Cristina Carvalho, Susana Cardoso, and Paula I. Moreira
Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) represents the most common form of dementia among people age 65 and older, affecting more than 35 million people worldwide and representing 50–56% of cases at autopsy and in clinical series. Clinically, AD is characterized by a progressive cognitive deterioration, together with impairments in behavior, language, and visuospatial skills, culminating in the premature death of the individual typically 3–9 years after diagnosis (Querfurth and LaFerla 2010, 329). The great majority of AD cases are sporadic in origin with a late onset, while a small proportion (< 1%) has genetic origin and involves mutations in amyloid β protein precursor (AβPP) and presenilins 1 and 2 (PS1 and PS2), leading to autosomal dominant familial AD with an early onset. Additionally, the allelic abnormalities of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene on chromosome 19 are responsible for both anticipated onset and increase in severity of inherited and sporadic AD (Rocchi et al. 2003, 1). Neuropathologically, AD has as main hallmarks the selective neuronal and synaptic loss, the deposition of
90
Dementia
extracellular senile plaques, mainly composed of amyloid-β (Aβ) peptide and the presence of intracellular neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) containing hyperphosphorylated tau protein (Selkoe 2001, 75; Moreira et al. 2006, 97; Moreira, Santos, and Oliveira 2007, 1621; Moreira et al. 2009, 741). Aβ peptide consists of 39–43 amino acid residues, derived from the proteolytic cleavage of AβPP by the β- and γ-secretases (Uemura, Kuzuya and Shimohama 2004, 1). The most common resulting fragments are either 40 or 42 amino acids in length (Aβ1-40 and Aβ1-42). Over the last decades many efforts have been made to uncover the molecular mechanisms underlying the pathogenesis of AD with several hypothesis being proposed to answer to one of the most exciting question of the actuality: What is the trigger/early event(s) of AD? In 2004, the “mitochondrial cascade hypothesis” emerged to explain many of the biochemical, genetic and pathological features of sporadic AD (Swerdlow and Khan 2004, 8). This hypothesis postulates that (1) inheritance determines mitochondrial baseline function and durability; (2) mitochondrial durability influences how mitochondria change with age; and (3) when mitochondrial alterations reaches a threshold, AD histopathology and symptoms ensue (Swerdlow and Khan 2009, 308). In addition, mitochondria have been shown to be targets of the deleterious effects of Aβ (LaFerla, Green, and Oddo 2007, 499), potential sites of Aβ production (Hansson et al. 2004, 654; Keil et al. 2004, 50310) and triggers of the disease (Nunomura et al. 2001, 759; Pratico et al. 2001, 4183; Hauptmann et al. 2009, 1574), which provide stronger evidence supporting the “mitochondrial cascade hypothesis.” Thus, it is plausible that mitochondrial-dependent pathogenic mechanisms have a central stage in the onset and progression of AD (Figure 3.1). Oxidative stress has also been implicated in the pathogenesis of AD occurring prior to the onset of symptoms, the oxidative changes being pervasive throughout the body and detected peripherally (Ghanbari et al. 2004, 41; Moreira, Harris, et al. 2007, 195; Perry et al. 2003, 552) and associated with the vulnerable regions of the brain affected in disease (Nunomura et al. 1999, 1959; Nunomura et al. 2001, 759). The complex nature and genesis of oxidative damage in AD could be the result of mitochondrial abnormalities that can trigger oxidative stress. Interestingly, also disturbances in insulin metabolism, especially insulin resistance, have been suggested to be involved in AD, attributing a role to the disruption of insulin signaling in AD pathophysiology (Moreira, Santos, et al. 2007, 1621; Cardoso et al. 2009, 483). In light of this evidence, the present review is devoted to discuss the current knowledge concerning the role of oxidative stress, mitochondria, and insulin signaling deregulation in the onset and progression of AD.
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction
91
Figure 3.1
Mitochondrial dysfunction in Alzheimer ’s disease. Mitochondrial dysfunction is intimately involved in the pathogenesis of AD. Amyloid β peptide (Aβ) has been documented to impair the activity of respiratory chain complex IV, leading to increased reactive oxygen species (ROS) levels. Indeed, Aβ peptide was shown to be imported into mitochondria via the translocase of the outer membrane (TOM) import machinery and localized to mitochondrial cristae, thus promoting mitochondrial dysfunction and oxidative damage. Furthermore, Aβ also interacts with cyclophilin D, a critical molecule involved in mitochondrial permeability transition pore (MPTP) formation and cell death. The opening of MPTP leads to the release of pro-apoptotic factors such as cytochrome c (Cyt c) to the citosol, and consequently to the induction of the apoptotic cell death. Additionally, mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) mutations have also been implicated in mitochondrial dysfunction that occurs in AD. ADP- adenosine diphosphate; Cyt c- cytochrome c; IM- inner membrane; IMSintermembrane space of mitochondria; NAD+- oxidized nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide; NADH- reduced nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide; H+- proton; OM- mitochondrial outer membrane.
Unraveling the mechanisms involved in the etiopathogenesis of AD could provide new insights that can be translated to potential pharmacological interventions aimed to treat this neurodegenerative disease.
92
Dementia
OXIDATIVE STRESS AND MITOCHONDRIAL DYSFUNCTION AS TRIGGERS OF NEURODEGENERATION IN ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE Mitochondria are ubiquitous and dynamic organelles that house many crucial cellular processes in eukaryotic organisms being considered “gatekeepers of life and death.” Major functions of mitochondria include the production of over 90% of cellular ATP through the tricarboxylic acid cycle (TCA) cycle and oxidative phosphorylation, regulation of intracellular calcium (Ca2+) and redox signaling and the arbitration of apoptosis (Green and Kroemer 2004, 626; Beal 2005, 495; Mattson, Gleichmann, and Cheng 2008, 748). Therefore, the importance of mitochondria for neuronal function and survival is notorious since neurons are cells with extremely high energy demands, mitochondrial oxidative phosphorylation being essential for neurons to meet their high energy requirements. In line with this, neurons are very vulnerable to bioenergetic crisis and dysfunction of mitochondrial machinery (Murphy, Fiskum, and Beal 1999, 231; Moreira et al. 2009, 741). Indeed, dysfunction of mitochondrial energy metabolism culminates in ATP production and Ca2+ buffering impairment, and exacerbated generation of reactive oxygen species (ROS) (Beal 2005, 495). ROS, in turn, cause cell membranes damage through lipid peroxidation and accelerates the high mutation rate of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA). Additionally, accumulation of mtDNA mutations enhances oxidative damage, causes energy crisis and increases ROS production, in a vicious cycle (Petrozzi et al. 2007, 87). Moreover, the brain is especially prone to oxidative stress-induced damage due to its high levels of polyunsaturated fatty acids, high oxygen consumption, high content in transition metals and poor antioxidant defenses (Nunomura et al. 2006, 82323). The next subsections explore the role of oxidative stress in AD as well as the contribution of mitochondrial malfunctions to the pathophysiology of the disease. Oxidative Stress: A Critical Player in Alzheimer ’s Disease For a long time, oxidative stress was defined as the imbalance between the formation of ROS and the antioxidant defense mechanisms. Meanwhile, a new concept of oxidative stress emerged to account for two different mechanistic outcomes, macromolecular damage and disruption of thiol redox circuits, which lead to aberrant cell signaling and dysfunctional redox control (Jones 2006, 1865). Increased oxidative stress has been observed in age and age-related neurodegenerative diseases, mitochondria being both targets and sources of ROS (Lin and Beal 2006, 787). In
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction
93
fact, accumulating evidence demonstrates that oxidative damage marked by high levels of lipid, protein, and nucleic acid oxidation is increased in vulnerable neurons in AD (Castellani et al. 2001, 175; Nunomura et al. 1999, 1959; Nunomura et al. 2001, 759; Smith et al. 1997, 2653; Straface et al. 2005, 2759). Nucleic acid oxidation is marked by increased levels of 8-hydroxy-2-deoxyguanosine (8-OHdG) and 8-hydroxyguanosine (8-OHG) (Nunomura et al. 1999, 1959; Nunomura et al. 2001, 759). Protein oxidation is marked by elevated levels of protein carbonyl and widespread nitration of tyrosine residues in the susceptible neurons (Smith et al. 1996, 120; Smith et al. 1997, 2653). Lipid peroxidation is marked by higher levels of thiobarbituric acid reactive substances (TBARS), malondialdehyde (MDA), 4-hydroxy-2-nonenal (HNE), isoprostanes and altered phospholipid composition (Sayre et al. 1997, 2092). Also, modifications to sugars are observed via increased glycoxidation and glycation (Smith et al. 1994, 5710; Smith et al. 1995, 172) that are responsible for the formation of advanced glycation endproducts (AGEs) such as Nε-(carboxymethyl) lysine (CML), pentosidine and pyralline. Additionally, it has been proposed that oxidative stress precedes all the other pathological hallmarks of AD pathogenesis. Indeed, the secretion and deposition of Aβ within vulnerable AD neurons have been suggested to be compensatory mechanisms developed by cells to protect themselves against oxidative damage (Hayashi et al. 2007, 1552; Nakamura et al. 2007, 12737; Smith et al. 2002, 1194). Accordingly, Aβ was demonstrated to follow the appearance of oxidative stress markers in AD (Petersen et al. 2007, 143) and it was shown that this peptide protects lipoproteins from oxidation in cerebrospinal fluid and plasma (Atwood et al. 1998, 12817; Atwood et al. 2003, 249–266; Cuajungco et al. 2000, 19439; Kontush et al. 2001, 119). Similarly, in AβPP transgenic mouse models of AD (Tg2576), it was also observed that oxidative stress appears before than Aβ deposition (Pratico et al. 2001, 4183; Smith et al. 1998, 2212). In light of this evidence, Zhu and colleagues proposed the “Two-Hit hypothesis,” which postulates that the early and progressive oxidative damage to neurons elicits a compensatory response such that the cell can exist in the overly oxidizing environment. Furthermore, this “oxidative steady state,” with the initial purpose to afford protection, makes the cell more vulnerable to additional insults, such as Aβ deposition and NFT formation (Zhu et al. 2001, 39; Zhu et al. 2004, 219; Zhu et al. 2007, 494). Cellular oxidative damage also promotes cell cycle aberration and tau hyperphosphorylation, leading to the NFT formation (Castegna et al. 2002, 1524; Castegna et al. 2003, 1394; Lee et al. 2004, 1; Lee et al. 2005, 164; Mark
94
Dementia
et al. 1997, 255). Consequently, damaged cells succumb to the degenerative process, or exist in a dysfunctional state, the ultimate manifestation of which is the cognitive decline and dementia descriptive of AD. Along with increased oxidative damage, impaired antioxidant defenses have also been proposed to be prominent features of AD (Smith et al. 1997, 2653; Straface et al. 2005, 2759). Indeed, decreased activities of the antioxidant enzymes copper/zinc superoxide dismutase (Cu/ZnSOD) and catalase (CAT) were found in the frontal and temporal cortex of AD patients (Marcus et al. 1998, 40). The total antioxidant capacity was also significantly decreased in AD as well as in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) but not in patients with vascular dementia (Straface et al. 2005, 2759), being shown a negative correlation between the total antioxidant capacity and disease duration, in AD patients (Guidi et al. 2006, 262). It has also been proposed that oxidative stress-mediated neuronal loss could be initiated by a decline in glutathione (GSH), which acts as a scavenger of free radicals and is the most abundant thiol-reducing agent in mammalian tissues (Bains and Shaw 1997, 335). In fact, altered GSH levels were observed in specific regions of the central nervous system of AD patients (Gu et al. 1998, 24). Similarly, it was found a reduced GSH content in lymphoblasts carrying AβPP, PS1, and PS2 gene mutations when compared to controls (Cecchi et al. 1999, 152). More recently, it has been reported that erythrocytes of AD and MCI patients present a decrease in GSH levels and GSH/ GSSG ratio compared to age-matched control subjects (Bermejo et al. 2008, 162). Accordingly, a study from our laboratory showed low levels of GSH in the triple transgenic model of AD (3xTg-AD), accompanied by a decrease in vitamin E levels and high levels of lipid peroxidation (Resende et al. 2008, 2051). Mitochondrial abnormalities have also been implicated in the etiopathogenesis of AD, which can be triggered by oxidative disturbances. Compelling evidence demonstrates that AD patients present reduced metabolic activity, which is believed to be the result of oxidative damage to vital mitochondrial components (Aksenov et al. 1998, 151; Aliev et al. 2003, 209–238; Anderson, Cummings, and Cotman 1994, 286; Hirai et al. 2001, 3017). As mentioned above, besides being essential ATPproducing organelles, mitochondria are also one of the major intracellular sources of potentially pathogenic ROS, including hydrogen peroxide (H2O2), hydroxyl (HO–) and superoxide (O2–), particularly in highly metabolically active organs such as the brain (Wallace 1999, 1482). Excessive mitochondrial ROS generation damages several cellular targets including mitochondrial components themselves (lipids, proteins, and DNA) (Moreira et al. 2009, 741). Indeed, mitochondrial ROS induce mutations
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction
95
in the mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA), which in turn impair the oxidative phosphorylation system. This impairment results in an exacerbation of ROS generation, promoting the augment of the number of mtDNA mutations in a vicious positive feedback cycle (Fukui and Moraes 2008, 251). Also the lack of histones in mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) and diminished capacity for DNA repair render mitochondria an easy target to oxidative stress events (Moreira et al. 2009, 741). Overall, oxidative stress-induced cellular damage has been long recognized as a culprit in degenerative processes that occur in AD. The next subsection is devoted to explore the intimate connection between mitochondrial impairment and oxidative stress. Mitochondrial Anomalies and Oxidative Stress: Side by Side in Alzheimer ’s Disease Accumulating data from in vitro, in vivo and human studies argue that mitochondrial dysfunction and bioenergetics failure are early events implicated in AD pathogenesis (Moreira et al. 2010, 2) (see Figure 3.1). Impaired activities of the three key TCA enzyme complexes, pyruvate dehydrogenase (PDH), isocitrate dehydrogenase, and α-ketoglutarate dehydrogenase (KGDH) have been documented in postmortem AD brain and fibroblasts from AD patients (Huang et al. 2003, 309; Bubber et al. 2005, 695). Data from our laboratory also demonstrated that the levels of both PDH and KGDH are decreased in AD brains (Moreira et al. 2007, “Autophagocytosis of mitochondria,” 525). Furthermore, Bubber and collaborators (2005, 695) tested whether impairments in TCA cycle enzymes correlate with disability in AD brains. The authors observed that all the changes in TCA cycle activities (specifically that of PDH complex) correlated with the clinical state, suggesting a coordinated mitochondrial alteration (Bubber et al. 2005, 695). These enzymes are known to be highly susceptible to oxidative modification and are altered by exposure to a range of pro-oxidants (Tretter and Adam-Vizi 2000, 8972). In addition, a decline in respiratory chain complexes I, III, and IV activities was found in platelets and lymphocytes from AD patients and postmortem AD brain tissue (Kish et al. 1992, 776; Parker et al. 1994, 1086; Bosetti et al. 2002, 371; Valla et al. 2006, 323), further emphasizing that mitochondrial abnormalities are present at the earliest symptomatic stages of the disease. Similarly, in vitro studies demonstrated that pheochromocytoma cells (PC12) exposed to Aβ1-40 and Aβ25-35 present mitochondrial dysfunction characterized by the inhibition of complexes I, III, and IV of the mitochondrial respiratory chain (Pereira, Santos and Oliveira 1998, 1749).
96
Dementia
More recently, Fattoretti and collaborators (2009), in order to establish a link between AD and mitochondrial dysfunction, investigated succinic dehydrogenase (SDH) (mitochondrial respiratory complex II) activity in mitochondria of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons obtained from 3xTg-AD mice. The authors observed a decreased density (number of mitochondria/μm3 of cytoplasm) of SDH-positive mitochondria in 3xTg-AD mice. Data from our laboratory also revealed that AD fibroblasts present high levels of oxidative stress and apoptotic markers when compared with young and age-matched controls. Moreover, AD-type changes could be generated in control fibroblasts using N-methylprotoporphyrin to inhibit cytochrome c oxidase (COX) assembly, which indicates that the observed oxidative damage was associated with mitochondrial dysfunction. Additionally, the effects promoted by the N-methylprotoporphyrine were reversed or attenuated by lipoic acid and N-acetyl cysteine (Moreira, Harris, et al. 2007, 195). Overall, these findings suggest that mitochondria are important in oxidative damage that occurs in AD and that antioxidant therapies may be promising. mtDNA mutations have also been implicated in mitochondrial dysfunction in the pathogenesis of AD (see Figure 3.1). For instance, 20 point mutations were detected in the mitochondrial-encoded cytochrome c oxidase subunits I, II, and III genes in AD patients (Hamblet et al. 2006, 398). Qiu and collaborators (2001, 261) also identified two missense mutations in the mtDNA of COX in a patient with AD. Further, a high aggregate burden of somatic mtDNA mutations was observed in postmortem brain tissue from AD patients (Lin et al. 2002, 133; Coskun, Beal and Wallace, 2004, 10726). Accumulating evidence also indicates that Aβ and AβPP could directly target mitochondria. For instance, Aβ was found to impair cellular respiration, energy production, and mitochondrial electron chain complexes activity in human neuroblastoma cells (Rhein, Baysang, et al. 2009, 1063). Moreover, cultured neurons isolated from Tg mice that overexpress a mutant form of AβPP and Aβ-binding alcohol dehydrogenase (ABAD) (Tg mAβPP/ABAD) display spontaneous generation of H2O2 and O2–, decreased ATP, release of cytochrome c and induction of caspase 3-like activity followed by DNA fragmentation and loss of cell viability. Furthermore, generation of ROS is associated with dysfunction at the level of COX (Takuma et al. 2005, 597). Similarly, Crouch and colleagues (2005, 672) found that Aβ1-42 can disrupt mitochondrial COX activity in a sequenceand conformation-dependent manner. In an in vitro study designed to explore the effect of the AβPP Swedish double mutation (K670M/N671L) on oxidative stress-induced cell death mechanisms in PC12 cells, increased activity of caspase 3 was observed due to an enhanced activation of both
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction
97
intrinsic and extrinsic apoptotic pathways, including activation of the JNK pathway. Moreover, apoptosis was attenuated by SP600125, a JNK inhibitor, through protection of mitochondrial dysfunction and reduction of caspase 9 activity (Marques et al. 2003, 28294). These findings corroborate the hypothesis that the massive neurodegeneration at an early age in familial AD patients could be a result of an increased vulnerability of neurons through the activation of different apoptotic pathways as a consequence of elevated levels of oxidative stress. In addition, mitochondrial dysfunction was also linked to the accumulation of full-length and carboxy-terminally truncated AβPP across mitochondrial import channels in brain tissue from AD patients. The authors observed that this accumulation of AβPP inhibited the entrance of nuclear-encoded COX subunits IV and Vb proteins, which was associated with decreased cytochrome c oxidase activity and increased H2O2 levels (Devi et al. 2006, 9057). Similarly, Anandatheerthavarada and colleagues (2003) reported an accumulation of full-length AβPP in the mitochondrial compartment in a transmembrane-arrested form that impaired mitochondrial functionality and energy metabolism. Also, a progressive accumulation of Aβ monomers and oligomers was detected within the mitochondria of both transgenic mice overexpressing mutant AβPP and postmortem brain from AD patients (Caspersen et al. 2005, 2040; Crouch et al. 2005, 672; Devi et al. 2006, 9057; Manczak et al. 2006, 1437). A direct link between Aβ-induced toxicity and mitochondrial dysfunction in AD pathology has been suggested by the interaction between mitochondrial Aβ and ABAD (Yan and Stern 2005, 161; Lustbader et al. 2004, 448). Moreover, this interaction was found to induce mitochondrial failure via changes in mitochondrial membrane permeability and a reduction in the activities of enzymes involved in mitochondrial respiration (Lustbader et al. 2004, 448). More recently, Hansson Petersen and collaborators (2008, 13145) showed that Aβ peptide is imported into mitochondria via the translocase of the outer membrane (TOM) import machinery and localized to mitochondrial cristae (see Figure 3.1). Thus, it has been proposed that Aβ species transport to mitochondria cause mitochondrial dysfunction and oxidative damage, and consequently damage neurons both structurally and functionally (Caspersen et al. 2005, 2040; Crouch et al. 2005, 672; Devi et al. 2006, 9057; Manczak et al. 2006, 1437; Hansson Petersen et al. 2008, 13145). Previous studies from our laboratory also reported an increased susceptibility to mitochondrial permeability transition pore (MPTP) induction promoted by Aβ peptides (Moreira et al. 2001, 789; Moreira et al. 2002, 257) (Figure 3.1). In accordance, it was provided a plausible mechanism underlying Aβ-induced mitochondrial dysfunction, in which Aβ interacts with cyclophilin D, a critical
98
Dementia
molecule involved in MPTP formation and cell death. Du and collaborators (2008) showed that the interaction of cyclophilin D with mitochondrial Aβ potentiates mitochondrial, neuronal and synaptic stress. Conversely, cyclophilin D ablation protects neurons from Aβ-induced MPTP formation and the resultant mitochondrial and cellular stresses. Additionally, cyclophilin D deficiency substantially improves learning and memory and synaptic function in an AD mouse model and alleviates Aβ-mediated reduction of long-term potentiation (LTP) (Du et al. 2008, 1097). Another study reported that the presequence protease (PreP) is responsible for the degradation of the accumulated Aβ in mitochondria, further supporting the association of Aβ with mitochondria and mitochondrial dysfunction in AD (Falkevall et al. 2006, 29096). However, the key role of mitochondria in AD pathogenesis was recently highlighted, as well as the close interplay of this organelle with the two main pathological features of the disease. Rhein, Song, and collaborators (2009, 20057) demonstrated that Aβ and tau synergistically impair mitochondrial function and energy homeostasis in 3xTg-AD mice. Accordingly, a previous study demonstrated that Tg mice overexpressing the P301L mutant human tau protein present alterations of metabolism-related proteins including mitochondrial respiratory chain complexes, antioxidant enzymes and synaptic proteins that are associated with increased oxidative stress. Moreover, mitochondria from these Tg mice displayed increased vulnerability toward Aβ insult, which reinforce a possible synergistic action of tau and Aβ pathology on the mitochondria. The authors also suggest that tau pathology involves a mitochondrial and oxidative stress disorder possibly distinct from that caused by Aβ (David et al. 2005, 23802). These findings may contribute to a better understanding of the biochemical pathways underlying mitochondrial dysfunction in AD and may help lead to the development of novel mitochondrial-targeted therapeutic strategies. Ultrastructural alterations in mitochondrial morphology such as reduced size and broken internal membrane cristae were also documented in brains from AD patients (Hirai et al. 2001, 3017; Baloyannis 2006, 119). One reasonable explanation for these observations could be the increased mitochondrial autophagy found in AD (Moreira et al. 2007, “Autophagocytosis of mitochondria,” 525; Moreira et al. 2007, “Increased autophagic degradation,” 614). Another consequence of Aβ on mitochondria is the induction of dynamic changes, including mitochondrial fission/fusion perturbations. Wang and collaborators (2008, “Dynamin,” 470) reported abnormal mitochondrial fission and fusion in fibroblasts from sporadic AD patients, marked by lower levels of dynamin-related protein 1 (Drp1), a key regulator of mitochondrial fission. The authors also observed that AD fibroblasts display elongated
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction
99
mitochondria which form collapsed perinuclear networks (Wang et al. 2008, “Dynamin,” 470; Wang et al. 2009, “Role of abnormal mitochondrial dynamics,” 153). Accordingly, AβPP overexpression in M17 neuroblastoma cells resulted in predominantly fragmented mitochondria, decreased Drp1 and optic atrophy protein 1 (OPA1) levels, and a defect in neuronal differentiation (Wang et al. 2008, “Amyloid-beta overproduction,” 19318). Moreover, reduced expression levels of Drp1, OPA1, mitofusin (Mfn)1 and 2 and increased mitochondria fission protein 1 (Fis1) levels were found in hippocampal tissues from AD patients compared with age-matched controls (Wang et al. 2009, “Impaired balance,” 9090). These results suggest that AD is characterized by mitochondrial fission/fusion imbalance, and consequently mitochondrial fragmentation and abnormal distribution, which potentiates mitochondrial and neuronal dysfunction in this neurodegenerative disease. BRAIN GLUCOSE TRANSPORT AND METABOLISM AND INSULIN SIGNALING DEREGULATION IN ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE Glucose is the main source of energy required for normal brain function. Since neurons are incapable to synthesize or store glucose, they are dependent on glucose transport across the blood-brain barrier (BBB), which is mediated by glucose transporters (GLUTs) (Scheepers, Joost, and Schurmann 2004, 364). An impairment of glucose metabolism in the brain of AD patients has been observed by positron emission tomography (PET) imaging studies (Azari et al. 1993, 438; Small et al. 1996, 70; Davis et al. 1997, 4526). Moreover, this impairment seems to be a cause, rather than a consequence, of neurodegeneration in AD (Hoyer 2004, 541). Insulin-mediated neuronal insulin receptor (IR) and/or insulin-like growth factor-1 receptor (IGF-1R) activation (Kahn et al. 1993, 291; Noh et al. 1999, 263) underlies a complex and important role in the regulation of brain metabolism (Gasparini and Xu 2003, 404; Santos, Pereira, and Carvalho 1999, 33; Shah and Hausman 1993, 151; Yang, Raizada, and Fellows 1981, 1050), neuronal growth and differentiation (Schechter et al. 1998, 270; Gasparini and Xu 2003, 404; Plitzko, Rumpel, and Gottmann 2001, 1412), or neuromodulation (Gasparini and Xu 2003, 404; Kremerskothen et al. 2002, 153; Shuaib et al. 1995, 809; Vilchis and Salceda 1996, 1167). Originally, the brain was believed to be an insulin-insensitive organ. However, biochemical evidence for the presence of insulin and IRs in the brain and expression of insulin-sensitive GLUT-4 in neurons confirm the idea that the brain is in fact a target organ for insulin (El Messari et al.
100
Dementia
2002, 225). Indeed, IRs are widely expressed throughout the brain in both neurons and glia (Wozniak et al. 1993, 1), with the highest levels in the olfactory bulb, cerebral cortex, hippocampus, cerebellum, and hypothalamus (Havrankova, Roth, and Brownstein 1978, 636; Van Houten et al. 1979, 666). In the adult brain, insulin derives primarily from its synthesis in pancreatic β-cells, being transported by cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) into the brain. This transport occurs mainly via a carrier-mediated, saturable, regulatable, and temperature-sensitive active process (Erol 2008, 241; Salkovic-Petrisic and Hoyer 2007, 217; Banks 2004, 5; Burns et al. 2007, 1094). Additionally, previous studies showed that insulin could be synthesized de novo in the brain. This idea was confirmed by the observation of the existence of preproinsulin I and II mRNA within rat fetal brain and in cultured neurons, and insulin immunoreactivity in the endoplasmatic reticulum (ER), Golgi apparatus, cytoplasm, axon, dendrites and synapses of neuronal cells (Adamo, Raizada, and LeRoith 1989, 71; Craft et al. 1996, 123; Schechter et al. 1996, 16; Schechter et al. 1998, 270; Zhao et al. 1999, 34893). This was further supported by the high levels of insulin detected in brain extracts (Havrankova et al. 1979, 636), the presence of insulin in immature nerve cell bodies (Schechter et al. 1992, 27; Schechter et al. 1996, 16), the observation that, despite peripherally injected, insulin can enter the CSF rapidly (Freude et al. 2005, 3343), and that less than 1% of the hormone crosses the BBB in dogs and rodents (Banks and Kastin 1998, 883). Hoyer (2004, 135) suggested that the impaired glucose utilization observed in AD brains is a consequence of diminished glucose breakdown in brain tissue, caused by a disturbance in the control of glucose utilization at the level of insulin signal transduction. Furthermore, it has been shown that type 2 diabetes mellitus is a risk factor for AD and that AD patients have a higher risk to develop type 2 diabetes (Cole and Frautschy 2007, 10; Moreira et al. 2009, 741). More recently, it was proposed that AD can be an “insulin-resistant brain state” or even a “type 3 diabetes” (Rivera et al. 2005, 247; Steen et al. 2005, 63; Craft et al. 1998, 164). Indeed, it was observed an age- and AD-related decrease in insulin mRNA and protein levels (Lester-Coll et al. 2006, 13; Rivera et al. 2005, 247; Steen et al. 2005, 63), IR and IGF-1R expression (Frolich et al. 1999, 290; Moloney et al. 2010, 224), insulin receptor substrate-1 (IRS-1) and IRS-2 levels and phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase (PI3-K) and extracellular-regulated kinase 1/2 (ERK1/2) activities. Furthermore, AD patients show increased fasting plasma insulin levels, decreased CSF insulin levels, and/or decreased CSF/plasma insulin ratio, besides increased Aβ levels (Watson and Craft 2004, 97), suggesting a decrease in insulin clearance, which may provoke
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 101
an elevation of plasma Aβ levels (Li and Holscher 2007, 384). It was also found that insulin modulates AβPP processing both in vivo and in vitro. Insulin has also been proposed to increase the extracellular concentration of Aβ by two independent mechanisms: stimulation of Aβ secretion by the enhancement of its trafficking from the ER and trans-Golgi network, the main site for Aβ generation, to the plasma membrane, which significantly reduces the intracellular concentration of Aβ derivatives (Aβ40 and Aβ42); or inhibition of extracellular degradation of Aβ by insulin-degrading enzyme (IDE), a metalloprotease enzyme responsible for insulin degradation and is also the main soluble Aβ degrading enzyme at neutral pH (Gasparini et al. 2001, 2561). This last hypothesis is supported by (1) a decrease in IDE activity and mRNA and protein levels in the AD brain; (2) impaired brain Aβ and insulin degradation in knockout mice lacking IDE (Frolich et al. 1999, 290; Hong and Lee 1997, 19547; Lucas et al. 2001, 27); (3) increased IDE immunoreactivity around senile plaques; and (4) enhanced IDE activity in IDE and AβPP double transgenic mice associated with a decrease in Aβ and prevention of AD (Leissring et al. 2003, 1087). Meanwhile, brain IR does not desensitize, thus IDE may constitute a negative feedback loop that controls insulin action (van der Heide, Ramakers, and Smidt 2006, 205; Zhao et al. 2004, 71). Since AD has been recognized as an “insulin-resistant brain state,” the intracerebroventricular (icv) injection of diabetogenic streptozotocin (STZ) has been shown to produce neurochemical and brain glucose metabolism changes, as well as long-term and progressive deficit in learning, memory, and cognitive behavior, that resemble those found in the brain of patients with AD (Grünblatt et al. 2007, 757; Salkovic-Petrisic and Hoyer 2007, 217). It was found that icvSTZ administration promotes a significant decrease in IRs expression in cortex and hippocampus, insulin-1 mRNA in hippocampus, insulin-2 mRNA in cortex and a significant increase of tau phosphorylation in the hippocampus, these alterations being associated with the impairment of memory and learning. These findings suggest that alterations of neuronal insulin signaling severely affect learning and memory processes. Additionally, icvSTZ administration was shown to induce brain atrophy, mainly due to neuronal and oligodendroglial cell loss mediated by apoptosis, mitochondrial dysfunction, neuroinflammation, and oxidative stress (Lester-Coll et al. 2006, 13). Furthermore, icvSTZ administration causes abnormalities in brain glucose metabolism, including reduction of glucose utilization in 17 of 35 brain areas (Duelli et al. 1994, 737) and decreased activities of glycolytic enzymes, leading to a decline in the levels of energy-rich compounds, ATP and creatine phosphate (Lannert and
102
Dementia
Hoyer 1998, 1199). De la Monte and collaborators (2006, 89) also reported an increase of AβPP and acetylcholinesterase expression, GSK-3β activity, phospho-tau and ubiquitin levels and decreased expression of choline acetyltransferase in icvSTZ-treated animals. Collectively, these findings support the idea that dysfunctional insulin signaling is critically involved in the pathogenesis of AD. Disturbance of tau phosphorylation seems to be another mechanism by which insulin is implicated in AD pathology. Indeed, insulin has been shown to activate the major kinases involved in tau phosphorylation, including glycogen synthase kinase 3β (GSK-3β), ERK1/2 and cyclindependent kinase 5 (Cdk-5) (Li and Hölscher 2007, 384; de la Monte and Wands 2005, 23802; van der Heide, Ramakers, and Smidt 2006, 205). Conversely, it has been reported that insulin and IGF-1 also inhibit abnormal tau hyperphosphorylation by stimulating Akt-induced phosphorylation/ inactivation of GSK-3β in both human and animal neurons (Li and Holscher 2007, 384; de la Monte and Wands 2005, 23802; Moloney et al. 2010, 224; Hong and Lee 1997, 19547). Accordingly, it was previously shown that insulin and IGF-1 reduce tau phosphorylation promoting its binding to microtubules by inhibition of GSK-3β via the PI3-K pathway (Ho et al. 2004, 902). In primary cortical neurons it was also observed that insulin or IGF-1 transiently increases phosphorylation of specific tau residues by activation of GSK-3β (Lesort and Johnson 2000, 305). Thus, disturbed insulin and/or IGF-1 signaling pathways could potentiate abnormal tau hyperphosphorylation leading to NFT formation (Cheng et al. 2005, 5086). Furthermore, hyperphosphorylated tau fails to be transported into axons, accumulating and aggregating into NFTs in neuronal perikarya, which promote mitochondrial dysfunction, oxidative stress, apoptotic or necrotic death (de la Monte and Wands 2005, 45). FINAL REMARKS Oxidative stress and mitochondrial abnormalities have been proposed to play a central role in the pathogenesis of AD. Mitochondria are pivotal in controlling cell survival and death, since they generate the majority of cellular ATP, buffer intracellular Ca2+, integrate apoptotic signaling pathways and represent one of the major sources of pathogenic ROS. Thus, perturbations in the physiological functions of these organelles inevitably culminate in oxidative damage and disturbed mitochondrial and neuronal function. Oxidative stress, in turn, potentiates mitochondrial and neuronal dysfunction in a vicious cycle. Converging evidence also posits that impaired brain glucose metabolism and abnormalities in the insulin
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 103
signaling cascade are intimately involved in the onset and establishment of AD pathology. As such, a detailed understanding of the involvement of oxidative stress, mitochondrial anomalies, impaired brain glucose metabolism, and dysfunctional insulin signaling will be of paramount importance in the context of AD. REFERENCES Adamo, M., M. K. Raizada, and D. LeRoith. 1989. Insulin and insulin-like growth factor receptors in the nervous system. Mol Neurobiol 3 (1–2): 71–100. Aksenov, M. Y., H. M. Tucker, P. Nair, M. V. Aksenova, D. A. Butterfield, S. Estus, and W. R. Markesbery. 1998. The expression of key oxidative stress-handling genes in different brain regions in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Mol Neurosci 11 (2): 151–164. Aliev, G., M. A. Smith, M. E. Obrenovich, J. C. de la Torre, and G. Perry. 2003. Role of vascular hypoperfusion-induced oxidative stress and mitochondria failure in the pathogenesis of Azheimer disease. Neurotox Res 5 (7): 491–504. Anandatheerthavarada, H. K., G. Biswas, M. A. Robin, and N. G. Avadhani. 2003. Mitochondrial targeting and a novel transmembrane arrest of Alzheimer ’s amyloid precursor protein impairs mitochondrial function in neuronal cells. J Cell Biol 161 (1): 41–54. Anderson, A. J., B. J. Cummings, and C. W. Cotman. 1994. Increased immunoreactivity for jun- and fos-related proteins in Alzheimer ’s disease: Association with pathology. Exp Neurol 125 (2): 286–295. Atwood, C. S., R. D. Moir, X. Huang, R. C. Scarpa, N. M. Bacarra, D. M. Romano, M. A. Hartshorn, R. E. Tanzi, and A. I. Bush. 1998. Dramatic aggregation of Alzheimer Abeta by Cu(Ii) is induced by conditions representing physiological acidosis. J Biol Chem 273 (21): 12817–12826. Atwood, C. S., M. A. Smith, R. N. Martins, R. E. Tanzi, A. E. Roher, A. I. Bush, and G. Perry. 2003. Neuroinflammation: Mechanisms and management. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. Azari, N. P., K. D. Pettigrew, M. B. Schapiro, J. V. Haxby, C. L. Grady, P. Pietrini, J. A. Salerno, L. L. Heston, S. I. Rapoport, and B. Horwitz. 1993. Early detection of Alzheimer ’s disease: A statistical approach using positron emission tomographic data. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 13 (3): 438–447. Bains, J. S., and C. A. Shaw. 1997. Neurodegenerative disorders in humans: The role of glutathione in oxidative stress-mediated neuronal death. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 25 (3): 335–358. Baloyannis, S. J. 2006. Mitochondrial alterations in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 9 (2): 119–126. Banks, W. A. 2004. The source of cerebral insulin. Eur J Pharmacol 490 (1–3): 5–12. Banks, W. A., and A. J. Kastin. 1998. Differential permeability of the blood-brain barrier to two pancreatic peptides: Insulin and amylin. Peptides 19 (5): 883–889.
104
Dementia
Beal, M. F. 2005. Mitochondria take center stage in aging and neurodegeneration. Ann Neurol 58 (4): 495–505. Bermejo, P., S. Martin-Aragon, J. Benedi, C. Susin, E. Felici, P. Gil, J. M. Ribera, and A. M. Villar. 2008. Peripheral levels of glutathione and protein oxidation as markers in the development of Alzheimer ’s disease from mild cognitive impairment. Free Radic Res 42 (2): 162–170. Bosetti, F., F. Brizzi, S. Barogi, M. Mancuso, G. Siciliano, E. A. Tendi, L. Murri, S. I. Rapoport, and G. Solaini. 2002. Cytochrome c oxidase and mitochondrial F1F0-ATPase (ATP synthase) activities in platelets and brain from patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 23 (3): 371–376. Bubber, P., V. Haroutunian, G. Fisch, J. P. Blass, and G. E. Gibson. 2005. Mitochondrial abnormalities in Alzheimer brain: Mechanistic implications. Ann Neurol 57: 695–703. Burns, J. M., J. E. Donnelly, H. S. Anderson, M. S. Mayo, L. Spencer-Gardner, G. Thomas, B. B. Cronk, et al. 2007. Peripheral insulin and brain structure in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology 69 (11): 1094–1104. Cardoso, S., S. Correia, R. X. Santos, C. Carvalho, M. S. Santos, C. R. Oliveira, G. Perry, M. A. Smith, X. Zhu, and P. I. Moreira. 2009. Insulin is a two-edged knife on the brain. J Alzheimers Dis 18 (3): 483–507. Caspersen, C., N. Wang, J. Yao, A. Sosunov, X. Chen, J. W. Lustbader, H. W. Xu, D. Stern, G. McKhann, and S. D. Yan. 2005. Mitochondrial Abeta: A potential focal point for neuronal metabolic dysfunction in Alzheimer ’s disease. Faseb J 19 (14): 2040–2041. Castegna, A., M. Aksenov, V. Thongboonkerd, J. B. Klein, W. M. Pierce, R. Booze, W. R. Markesbery, and D. A. Butterfield. 2002. Proteomic identification of oxidatively modified proteins in Alzheimer ’s disease brain. Part 2: Dihydropyrimidinase-related Protein 2, Alpha-Enolase and Heat Shock Cognate 71. J Neurochem 82 (6): 1524–1532. Castegna, A., V. Thongboonkerd, J. B. Klein, B. Lynn, W. R. Markesbery, and D. A. Butterfield. 2003. Proteomic identification of nitrated proteins in Alzheimer ’s disease Brain. J Neurochem 85 (6): 1394–1401. Castellani, R. J., P. L. Harris, L. M. Sayre, J. Fujii, N. Taniguchi, M. P. Vitek, H. Founds, C. S. Atwood, G. Perry, and M. A. Smith. 2001. Active glycation in neurofibrillary pathology of Alzheimer disease: N(epsilon)-(carboxymethyl) lysine and hexitol-lysine. Free Radic Biol Med 31 (2): 175–180. Cecchi, C., S. Latorraca, S. Sorbi, T. Iantomasi, F. Favilli, M. T. Vincenzini, and G. Liguri. 1999. Gluthatione level is altered in lymphoblasts from patients with familial Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurosci Lett 275 (2): 152–154. Cheng, C. M., V. Tseng, J. Wang, D. Wang, L. Matyakhina, and C. A. Bondy. 2005. Tau is hyperphosphorylated in the insulin-like growth factor-I null brain. Endocrinology 146 (12): 5086–5091. Cole, G. M., and S. A. Frautschy. 2007. The role of insulin and neurotrophic factor signaling in brain aging and Alzheimer ’s disease. Exp Gerontol 42 (1–2): 10–21.
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 105 Coskun, P. E., M. F. Beal, and D. C. Wallace. 2004. Alzheimer ’s brains harbor somatic mtDNA control-region mutations that suppress mitochondrial transcription and replication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101 (29): 10726–10731. Craft, S., J. Newcomer, S. Kanne, S. Dagogo-Jack, P. Cryer, Y. Sheline, J. Luby, A. Dagogo-Jack, and A. Alderson. 1996. Memory improvement following induced hyperinsulinemia in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 17 (1): 123–130. Craft, S., E. Peskind, M. W. Schwartz, G. D. Schellenberg, M. Raskind, and D. Porte Jr. 1998. Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma insulin levels in Alzheimer ’s disease: Relationship to severity of dementia and apolipoprotein E genotype. Neurology 50 (1): 164–168. Crouch, P. J., R. Blake, J. A. Duce, G. D. Ciccotosto, Q. X. Li, K. J. Barnham, C. C. Curtain, et al. 2005. Copper-dependent inhibition of human cytochrome C oxidase by a dimeric conformer of amyloid-beta1-42. J Neurosci 25 (3): 672–679. Cuajungco, M. P., L. E. Goldstein, A. Nunomura, M. A. Smith, J. T. Lim, C. S. Atwood, X. Huang, Y. W. Farrag, G. Perry, and A. I. Bush. 2000. Evidence that the beta-amyloid plaques of Alzheimer ’s disease represent the redox-silencing and entombment of Abeta by zinc. J Biol Chem 275 (26): 19439–19442. David, D. C., S. Hauptmann, I. Scherping, K. Schuessel, U. Keil, P. Rizzu, R. Ravid, et al. 2005. Proteomic and functional analyses reveal a mitochondrial dysfunction in P301l tau transgenic mice. J Biol Chem 280 (2): 23802–23814. Davis, R. E., S. Miller, C. Herrnstadt, S. S. Ghosh, E. Fahy, L. A. Shinobu, D. Galasko, et al. 1997. Mutations in mitochondrial cytochrome C oxidase genes segregate with late-onset Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94 (9): 4526–4531. de la Monte, S. M., M. Tong, N. Lester-Coll, M. Plater Jr., and J. R. Wands. 2006. Therapeutic rescue of neurodegeneration in experimental type 3 diabetes: Relevance to Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 10 (1): 89–109. de la Monte, S. M., and J. R. Wands. 2005. Review of insulin and insulin-like growth factor expression, signaling, and malfunction in the central nervous system: Relevance to Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 7 (1): 45–61. Devi, L., B. M. Prabhu, D. F. Galati, N. G. Avadhani, and H. K. Anandatheerthavarada. 2006. Accumulation of amyloid precursor protein in the mitochondrial import channels of human Alzheimer ’s disease brain is associated with mitochondrial dysfunction. J Neurosci 26 (35): 9057–9068. Du, H., L. Guo, F. Fang, D. Chen, A. A. Sosunov, G. M. McKhann, Y. Yan, et al. 2008. Cyclophilin D deficiency attenuates mitochondrial and neuronal perturbation and ameliorates learning and memory in Alzheimer ’s disease. Nat Med 14 (10): 1097–1105. Duelli, R., H. Schrock, W. Kuschinsky, and S. Hoyer. 1994. Intracerebroventricular injection of streptozotocin induces discrete local changes in cerebral glucose utilization in rats. Int J Dev Neurosci 12 (8): 737–743.
106
Dementia
El Messari, S., A. Ait-Ikhlef, D. H. Ambroise, L. Penicaud, and M. Arluison. 2002. Expression of insulin-responsive glucose transporter GLUT4 mRNA in the rat brain and spinal cord: An in situ hybridization study. J Chem Neuroanat 24 (4): 225–242. Erol, A. 2008. An integrated and unifying hypothesis for the metabolic basis of sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 13 (3): 241–253. Falkevall, A., N. Alikhani, S. Bhushan, P. F. Pavlov, K. Busch, K. A. Johnson, T. Eneqvist, L. Tjernberg, M. Ankarcrona, and E. Glaser. 2006. Degradation of the amyloid beta-protein by the novel mitochondrial peptidasome, PreP. J Biol Chem 281 (39): 29096–290104. Fattoretti, P., M. Balietti, T. Casoli, B. Giorgetti, G. Di Stefano, C. Bertoni-Freddari, F. Lattanzio, and S. L. Sensi. 2009. Decreased numeric density of succinic dehydrogenase-positive mitochondria in Ca1 pyramidal neurons of 3xtg-Ad mice. Rejuvenation Res 13 (2–3): 144–147. Freude, S., L. Plum, J. Schnitker, U. Leeser, M. Udelhoven, W. Krone, J. C. Bruning, and M. Schubert. 2005. Peripheral hyperinsulinemia promotes tau phosphorylation in vivo. Diabetes 54 (12): 3343–3348. Frolich, L., D. Blum-Degen, P. Riederer, and S. Hoyer. 1999. A disturbance in the neuronal insulin receptor signal transduction in sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 893: 290–293. Fukui, H., and C. T. Moraes. 2008. The mitochondrial impairment, oxidative stress and neurodegeneration connection: Reality or just an attractive hypothesis? Trends Neurosci 31 (5): 251–256. Gasparini, L., G. K. Gouras, R. Wang, R. S. Gross, M. F. Beal, P. Greengard, and H. Xu. 2001. Stimulation of beta-amyloid precursor protein trafficking by insulin reduces intraneuronal beta-amyloid and requires mitogen-activated protein kinase signaling. J Neurosci 21 (8): 2561–2570. Gasparini, L., and H. Xu. 2003. Potential roles of insulin and Igf-1 in Alzheimer ’s disease. Trends Neurosci 26 (8): 404–406. Ghanbari, H. A., K. Ghanbari, P. L. Harris, P. K. Jones, Z. Kubat, R. J. Castellani, B. L. Wolozin, M. A. Smith, and G. Perry. 2004. Oxidative damage in cultured human olfactory neurons from Alzheimer ’s disease patients. Aging Cell 3 (1): 41–44. Green, D. R., and G. Kroemer. 2004. The pathophysiology of mitochondrial cell death. Science 305 (5684): 626–629. Grünblatt, E., M. Salkovic-Petrisic, J. Osmanovic, P. Riederer, and S. Hoyer. 2007. Brain insulin system dysfunction in streptozotocin intracerebroventricularly treated rats generates hyperphosphorylated tau protein. J Neurochem 101 (3): 757–770. Gu, M., A. D. Owen, S. E. Toffa, J. M. Cooper, D. T. Dexter, P. Jenner, C. D. Marsden, and A. H. Schapira. 1998. Mitochondrial function, GSH, and iron in neurodegeneration and Lewy Body diseases. J Neurol Sci 158 (1): 24–29. Guidi, I., D. Galimberti, S. Lonati, C. Novembrino, F. Bamonti, M. Tiriticco, C. Fenoglio, et al. 2006. Oxidative imbalance in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 27 (2): 262–269.
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 107 Hamblet, N. S., B. Ragland, M. Ali, B. Conyers, and F. J. Castora. 2006. Mutations in mitochondrial-encoded cytochrome C oxidase subunits I, II, and III genes detected in Alzheimer ’s disease using single-strand conformation polymorphism. Electrophoresis 27 (2): 398–408. Hansson, C. A., S. Frykman, M. R. Farmery, L. O. Tjernberg, C. Nilsberth, S. E. Pursglove, A. Ito, et al. 2004. Nicastrin, presenilin, Aph-1, and Pen-2 form active gamma-secretase complexes in mitochondria. J Biol Chem 279 (49): 51654–51660. Hansson Petersen, C. A., N. Alikhani, H. Behbahani, B. Wiehager, P. F. Pavlov, I. Alafuzoff, V. Leinonen, et al. 2008. The amyloid beta-peptide is imported into mitochondria via the TOM import machinery and localized to mitochondrial cristae. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105 (35): 13145–13150. Hauptmann, S., I. Scherping, S. Drose, U. Brandt, K. L. Schulz, M. Jendrach, K. Leuner, A. Eckert, and W. E. Muller. 2009. Mitochondrial dysfunction: An early event in Alzheimer pathology accumulates with age in ad transgenic mice. Neurobiol Aging 30 (10): 1574–1586. Havrankova, J., J. Roth, and M. Brownstein. 1978. Insulin receptors are widely distributed in the central nervous system of the rat. Nature 272: 827–829. Havrankova, J., J. Roth, and M. J. Brownstein. 1979. Concentrations of insulin and insulin receptors in the brain are independent of peripheral insulin levels. Studies of obese and streptozotocin-treated rodents. J Clin Invest 64 (2): 636–642. Hayashi, T., N. Shishido, K. Nakayama, A. Nunomura, M. A. Smith, G. Perry, and M. Nakamura. 2007. Lipid peroxidation and 4-hydroxy-2-nonenal formation by copper ion bound to amyloid-beta peptide. Free Radic Biol Med 43 (11): 1552–1559. Hirai, K., G. Aliev, A. Nunomura, H. Fujioka, R. L. Russell, C. S. Atwood, A. B. Johnson, et al. 2001. Mitochondrial abnormalities in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 21 (9): 3017–3023. Ho, L., W. Qin, P. N. Pompl, Z. Xiang, J. Wang, Z. Zhao, Y. Peng, et al. 2004. Dietinduced insulin resistance promotes amyloidosis in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. Faseb J 18 (7): 902–904. Hong, M., and V. M. Lee. 1997. Insulin and insulin-like growth factor-1 regulate tau phosphorylation in cultured human neurons. J Biol Chem 272 (31): 19547–19553. Hoyer, S. 2004. Causes and consequences of disturbances of cerebral glucose metabolism in sporadic Alzheimer disease: Therapeutic implications. Adv Exp Med Biol 541: 135–152. Huang, H. M., H. C. Ou, H. Xu, H. L. Chen, C. Fowler, and G. E. Gibson. 2003. Inhibition of alpha-ketoglutarate dehydrogenase complex promotes cytochrome C release from mitochondria, caspase-3 activation, and necrotic cell death. J Neurosci Res 74 (2): 309–317. Jones, D. P. 2006. Redefining oxidative stress. Antioxid Redox Signal 8 (9–10): 1865–1879.
108
Dementia
Kahn, C. R., M. F. White, S. E. Shoelson, J. M. Backer, E. Araki, B. Cheatham, P. Csermely, et al. 1993. The insulin receptor and its substrate: Molecular determinants of early events in insulin action. Recent Prog Horm Res 48: 291–339. Keil, U., A. Bonert, C. A. Marques, I. Scherping, J. Weyermann, J. B. Strosznajder, F. Muller-Spahn, et al. 2004. Amyloid beta-induced changes in nitric oxide production and mitochondrial activity lead to apoptosis. J Biol Chem 279 (48): 50310–50320. Kish, S. J., C. Bergeron, A. Rajput, S. Dozic, F. Mastrogiacomo, L. J. Chang, J. M. Wilson, L. M. DiStefano, and J. N. Nobrega. 1992. Brain cytochrome oxidase in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurochem 59 (2): 776–779. Kontush, A., C. Berndt, W. Weber, V. Akopyan, S. Arlt, S. Schippling, and U. Beisiegel. 2001. Amyloid-beta is an antioxidant for lipoproteins in cerebrospinal fluid and plasma. Free Radic Biol Med 30 (1): 119–128. Kremerskothen, J., D. Wendholt, I. Teber, and A. Barnekow. 2002. Insulin-induced expression of the activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated gene (arc) in human neuroblastoma cells requires P21(Ras), mitogen-activated protein kinase/extracellular regulated kinase and Src tyrosine kinases but is protein kinase C-independent. Neurosci Lett 321 (3): 153–156. LaFerla, F. M., K. N. Green, and S. Oddo. 2007. Intracellular amyloid-beta in Alzheimer ’s disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 8 (7): 499–509. Lannert, H., and S. Hoyer. 1998. Intracerebroventricular administration of streptozotocin causes long-term diminutions in learning and memory abilities and in cerebral energy metabolism in adult rats. Behav Neurosci 112 (5): 1199–1208. Lee, H. G., G. Casadesus, X. Zhu, A. Takeda, G. Perry, and M. A. Smith. 2004. Challenging the amyloid cascade hypothesis: Senile plaques and amyloid-beta as protective adaptations to Alzheimer disease. Ann NY Acad Sci 1019: 1–4. Lee, H. G., G. Perry, P. I. Moreira, M. R. Garrett, Q. Liu, X. Zhu, A. Takeda, A. Nunomura, and M. A. Smith. 2005. Tau phosphorylation in Alzheimer ’s disease: Pathogen or protector? Trends Mol Med 11 (4): 164–169. Leissring, M. A., W. Farris, A. Y. Chang, D. M. Walsh, X. Wu, X. Sun, M. P. Frosch, and D. J. Selkoe. 2003. Enhanced proteolysis of beta-amyloid in app transgenic mice prevents plaque formation, secondary pathology, and premature death. Neuron 40 (6): 1087–1093. Lesort, M., and G. V. Johnson. 2000. Insulin-like growth factor-1 and insulin mediate transient site-selective increases in tau phosphorylation in primary cortical neurons. Neuroscience 99 (2): 305–316. Lester-Coll, N., E. J. Rivera, S. J. Soscia, K. Doiron, J. R. Wands, and S. M. de la Monte. 2006. Intracerebral streptozotocin model of type 3 diabetes: Relevance to sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 9 (1): 13–33. Li, L., and C. HÖlscher. 2007. Common pathological processes in Alzheimer disease and type 2 diabetes: A review. Brain Res Rev 56 (2): 384–402.
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 109 Lin, M. T., and M. F. Beal. 2006. Mitochondrial dysfunction and oxidative stress in neurodegenerative diseases. Nature 443 (7113): 787–795. Lin, M. T., D. K. Simon, C. H. Ahn, L. M. Kim, and M. F. Beal. 2002. High sggregate burden of somatic mtDNA point mutations in aging and Alzheimer ’s disease brain. Hum Mol Genet 11 (2): 133–145. Lucas, J. J., F. Hernandez, P. Gomez-Ramos, M. A. Moran, R. Hen, and J. Avila. 2001. Decreased nuclear beta-catenin, tau hyperphosphorylation and neurodegeneration in GSK-3beta conditional transgenic mice. Embo J 20 (1–2): 27–39. Lustbader, J. W., M. Cirilli, C. Lin, H. W. Xu, K. Takuma, N. Wang, C. Caspersen, et al. 2004. Abad directly links Abeta to mitochondrial toxicity in Alzheimer ’s disease. Science 304 (5669): 448–452. Manczak, M., T. S. Anekonda, E. Henson, B. S. Park, J. Quinn, and P. H. Reddy. 2006. Mitochondria are a direct site of a beta accumulation in Alzheimer ’s disease neurons: Implications for free radical generation and oxidative damage in disease progression. Hum Mol Genet 15 (9): 1437–1449. Marcus, D. L., C. Thomas, C. Rodriguez, K. Simberkoff, J. S. Tsai, J. A. Strafaci, and M. L. Freedman. 1998. Increased peroxidation and reduced antioxidant enzyme activity in Alzheimer ’s disease. Exp Neurol 150: 40–44. Mark, R. J., M. A. Lovell, W. R. Markesbery, K. Uchida, and M. P. Mattson. 1997. A role for 4-hydroxynonenal, an aldehydic product of lipid peroxidation, in disruption of ion homeostasis and neuronal death induced by amyloid beta-peptide. J Neurochem 68 (1): 255–264. Marques, C. A., U. Keil, A. Bonert, B. Steiner, C. Haass, W. E. Muller, and A. Eckert. 2003. Neurotoxic mechanisms caused by the Alzheimer ’s disease-linked Swedish amyloid precursor protein mutation: Oxidative stress, caspases, and the JNK pathway. J Biol Chem 278 (30): 28294–28302. Mattson, M. P., M. Gleichmann, and A. Cheng. 2008. Mitochondria in neuroplasticity and neurological disorders. Neuron 60 (5): 748–766. Moloney, A. M., R. J. Griffin, S. Timmons, R. O’Connor, R. Ravid, and C. O’Neill. 2010. Defects in IGF-1 receptor, insulin receptor and IRS-1/2 in Alzheimer ’s disease indicate possible resistance to IGF-1 and insulin signaling. Neurobiol Aging 31 (2): 224–243. Moreira, P. I., C. Carvalho, X. Zhu, M. A. Smith, and G. Perry. 2010. Mitochondrial dysfunction is a trigger of Alzheimer ’s disease pathophysiology. Biochim Biophys Acta 1802 (1): 2–10. Moreira, P. I., A. I. Duarte, M. S. Santos, A. C. Rego, and C. R. Oliveira. 2009. An integrative view of the role of oxidative stress, mitochondria, and insulin in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 16 (4): 741–761. Moreira, P. I., P. L. Harris, X. Zhu, M. S. Santos, C. R. Oliveira, M. A. Smith, and G. Perry. 2007. Lipoic acid and N-acetyl cysteine decrease mitochondrial-related oxidative stress in Alzheimer disease patient fibroblasts. J Alzheimers Dis 12 (2): 195–206.
110
Dementia
Moreira, P. I., K. Honda, X. Zhu, A. Nunomura, G. Casadesus, M. A. Smith, and G. Perry. 2006. Brain and hrawn: Parallels in oxidative strength. Neurology 66 (2) (Suppl 1): S97–101. Moreira, P. I., M. S. Santos, A. Moreno, and C. Oliveira. 2001. Amyloid beta-peptide promotes permeability transition pore in brain mitochondria. Biosci Rep 21 (6): 789–800. Moreira, P. I., M. S. Santos, A. Moreno, A. C. Rego, and C. Oliveira. 2002. Effect of amyloid beta-peptide on permeability transition pore: A comparative study. J Neurosci Res 69 (2): 257–267. Moreira, P. I., M. S. Santos, and C. R. Oliveira. 2007. Alzheimer ’s disease: A lesson from mitochondrial dysfunction. Antioxid Redox Signal 9 (10): 1621–1630. Moreira, P. I., S. L. Siedlak, X. Wang, M. S. Santos, C. R. Oliveira, M. Tabaton, A. Nunomura, et al. 2007. Autophagocytosis of mitochondria is prominent in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 66 (6): 525–532. Moreira, P. I., S. L. Siedlak, X. Wang, M. S. Santos, C. R. Oliveira, M. Tabaton, A. Nunomura, et al. 2007. Increased autophagic degradation of mitochondria in Alzheimer disease. Autophagy 3 (6): 614–615. Murphy, A. N., G. Fiskum, and M. F. Beal. 1999. Mitochondria in neurodegeneration: Bioenergetic function in cell life and death. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 19 (3): 231–245. Nakamura, M., N. Shishido, A. Nunomura, M. A. Smith, G. Perry, Y. Hayashi, K. Nakayama, and T. Hayashi. 2007. Three histidine residues of amyloidbeta peptide control the redox activity of copper and iron. Biochemistry 46 (44): 12737–12743. Noh, K. M., J. C. Lee, Y. H. Ahn, S. H. Hong, and J. Y. Koh. 1999. Insulin-induced oxidative neuronal injury in cortical culture: Mediation by induced N-methylD-aspartate receptors. IUBMB Life 48 (3): 263–269. Nunomura, A., K. Honda, A. Takeda, K. Hirai, X. Zhu, M. A. Smith, and G. Perry. 2006. Oxidative damage to RNA in neurodegenerative diseases. J Biomed Biotechnol 2006 (3): 82323. Nunomura, A., G. Perry, G. Aliev, K. Hirai, A. Takeda, E. K. Balraj, P. K. Jones, et al. 2001. Oxidative damage is the earliest event in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 60 (8): 759–767. Nunomura, A., G. Perry, M. A. Pappolla, R. Wade, K. Hirai, S. Chiba, and M. A. Smith. 1999. RNA oxidation is a prominent feature of vulnerable neurons in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 19 (6): 1959–1964. Parker, W. D., Jr., N. J. Mahr, C. M. Filley, J. K. Parks, D. Hughes, D. A. Young, and C. M. Cullum. 1994. Reduced platelet cytochrome C oxidase activity in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 44 (6): 1086–1090. Pereira, C., M. S. Santos, and C. Oliveira. 1998. Mitochondrial function impairment induced by amyloid beta-peptide on Pc12 cells. Neuroreport 9 (8): 1749–1755. Perry, G., R. J. Castellani, M. A. Smith, P. L. Harris, Z. Kubat, K. Ghanbari, P. K. Jones, et al. 2003. Oxidative damage in the olfactory system in Alzheimer ’s disease. Acta Neuropathol 106 (6): 552–556.
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 111 Petersen, R. B., A. Nunomura, H. G. Lee, G. Casadesus, G. Perry, M. A. Smith, and X. Zhu. 2007. Signal transduction cascades associated with oxidative stress in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 11 (2): 143–152. Petrozzi, L., G. Ricci, N. J. Giglioli, G. Siciliano, and M. Mancuso. 2007. Mitochondria and neurodegeneration. Biosci Rep 27 (1–3): 87–104. Plitzko, D., S. Rumpel, and K. Gottmann. 2001. Insulin promotes functional induction of silent synapses in differentiating rat neocortical neurons. Eur J Neurosci 14 (8): 1412–1415. Pratico, D., K. Uryu, S. Leight, J. Q. Trojanoswki, and V. M. Lee. 2001. Increased lipid peroxidation precedes amyloid plaque formation in an animal model of Alzheimer amyloidosis. J Neurosci 21 (12): 4183–4187. Qiu, X., Y. Chen, and M. Zhou. 2001. Two point mutations in mitochondrial DNA of cytochrome C oxidase coexist with normal mtDNA in a patient with Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain Res 893 (1–2): 261–263. Querfurth, H. W., and F. M. LaFerla. 2010. Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 362 (4): 329–344. Resende, R., P. I. Moreira, T. Proenca, A. Deshpande, J. Busciglio, C. Pereira, and C. R. Oliveira. 2008. Brain oxidative stress in a triple-transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer disease. Free Radic Biol Med 44 (12): 2051–2057. Rhein, V., G. Baysang, S. Rao, F. Meier, A. Bonert, F. Muller-Spahn, and A. Eckert. 2009. Amyloid-beta leads to impaired cellular respiration, energy production and mitochondrial electron chain complex activities in human neuroblastoma cells. Cell Mol Neurobiol 29 (6–7): 1063–1071. Rhein, V., X. Song, A. Wiesner, L. M. Ittner, G. Baysang, F. Meier, L. Ozmen, et al. 2009. Amyloid-beta and tau synergistically impair the oxidative phosphorylation system in triple transgenic Alzheimer ’s disease mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106 (47): 20057–20062. Rivera, E. J., A. Goldin, N. Fulmer, R. Tavares, J. R. Wands, and S. M. de la Monte. 2005. Insulin and insulin-like growth factor expression and function deteriorate with progression of Alzheimer ’s disease: Link to brain reductions in acetylcholine. J Alzheimers Dis 8 (3): 247–268. Rocchi, A., S. Pellegrini, G. Siciliano, and L. Murri. 2003. Causative and susceptibility genes for Alzheimer ’s disease: A review. Brain Res Bull 61 (1): 1–24. Salkovic-Petrisic, M., and S. Hoyer. 2007. Central insulin resistance as a trigger for sporadic Alzheimer-like pathology: An experimental approach. J Neural Transm Suppl (72): 217–233. Sankar, R., S. Thamotharan, D. Shin, K. H. Moley, and S. U. Devaskar. 2002. Insulin-responsive glucose transporters-GLUT8 and GLUT4 are expressed in the developing mammalian brain. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 107 (2): 157–165. Santos, M. S., E. M. Pereira, and A. P. Carvaho. 1999. Stimulation of immunoreactive insulin release by glucose in rat brain synaptosomes. Neurochem Res 24 (1): 33–36. Sayre, L. M., D. A. Zelasko, P. L. Harris, G. Perry, R. G. Salomon, and M. A. Smith. 1997. 4-Hydroxynonenal-derived advanced lipid peroxidation end products are increased in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurochem 68 (5): 2092–2097.
112
Dementia
Schechter, R., D. Beju, T. Gaffney, F. Schaefer, and L. Whetsell. 1996. Preproinsulin I and II mRNAs and insulin electron microscopic immunoreaction are present within the rat fetal nervous system. Brain Res 736 (1–2): 16–27. Schechter, R., J. Whitmire, L. Holtzclaw, M. George, R. Harlow, and S. U. Devaskar. 1992. Developmental regulation of insulin in the mammalian central nervous system. Brain Res 582 (1): 27–37. Schechter, R., T. Yanovitch, M. Abboud, G. Johnson III, and J. Gaskins. 1998. Effects of brain endogenous insulin on neurofilament and MAPK in fetal rat neuron cell cultures. Brain Res 808 (2): 270–278. Scheepers, A., H. G. Joost, and A. Schurmann. 2004. The glucose transporter families SGLT and GLUT: Molecular basis of normal and aberrant function. JPEN J Parenter Enteral Nutr 28 (5): 364–371. Selkoe, D. J. 2001. Alzheimer ’s disease results from the cerebral accumulation and cytotoxicity of amyloid beta-protein. J Alzheimers Dis 3 (1): 75–80. Shah, B. H., and R. E. Hausman. 1993. Effect of insulin on gabaergic development in the embryonic chick retina. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 72 (2): 151–158. Shuaib, A., M. S. Ijaz, T. Waqar, C. Voll, R. Kanthan, H. Miyashita, and L. Liu. 1995. Insulin elevates hippocampal GABA levels during ischemia. This is independent of its hypoglycemic effect. Neuroscience 67 (4): 809–814. Small, G. W., S. Komo, A. La Rue, S. Saxena, M. E. Phelps, J. C. Mazziotta, A. M. Saunders, J. L. Haines, M. A. Pericak-Vance, and A. D. Roses. 1996. Early detection of Alzheimer ’s disease by combining apolipoprotein E and neuroimaging. Ann NY Acad Sci 802: 70–78. Smith, M. A., G. Casadesus, J. A. Joseph, and G. Perry. 2002. Amyloid-beta and tau serve antioxidant functions in the aging and Alzheimer brain. Free Radic Biol Med 33 (9): 1194–1199. Smith, M. A., K. Hirai, K. Hsiao, M. A. Pappolla, P. L. Harris, S. L. Siedlak, M. Tabaton, and G. Perry. 1998. Amyloid-beta deposition in Alzheimer transgenic mice is associated with oxidative stress. J Neurochem 70 (5): 2212–2215. Smith, M. A., G. Perry, P. L. Richey, L. M. Sayre, V. E. Anderson, M. F. Beal, and N. Kowall. 1996. Oxidative damage in Alzheimer ’s. Nature 382 (6587): 120–121. Smith, M. A., P. L. Richey Harris, L. M. Sayre, J. S. Beckman, and G. Perry. 1997. Widespread peroxynitrite-mediated damage in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 17 (8): 2653–2657. Smith, M. A., L. M. Sayre, V. M. Monnier, and G. Perry. 1995. Radical ageing in Alzheimer ’s disease. Trends Neurosci 18 (4): 172–176. Smith, M. A., S. Taneda, P. L. Richey, S. Miyata, S. D. Yan, D. Stern, L. M. Sayre, V. M. Monnier, and G. Perry. 1994. Advanced Maillard reaction end products are associated with Alzheimer disease pathology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91 (12): 5710–5714. Steen, E., B. M. Terry, E. J. Rivera, J. L. Cannon, T. R. Neely, R. Tavares, X. J. Xu, J. R. Wands, and S. M. de la Monte. 2005. Impaired insulin and insulin-like
Oxidative Stress, Mitochondrial and Insulin Signaling Dysfunction 113 growth factor expression and signaling mechanisms in Alzheimer ’s disease—Is this type 3 diabetes? J Alzheimers Dis 7 (1): 63–80. Straface, E., P. Matarrese, L. Gambardella, R. Vona, A. Sgadari, M. C. Silveri, and W. Malorni. 2005. Oxidative imbalance and cathepsin D changes as peripheral blood biomarkers of Alzheimer disease: A pilot study. FEBS Lett 579 (13): 2759–2766. Swerdlow, R. H., and S. M. Khan. 2004. A mitochondrial cascade hypothesis for sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease. Med Hypotheses 63 (1): 8–20. Swerdlow, R. H., and S. M. Khan. 2009. The Alzheimer ’s disease mitochondrial cascade hypothesis: An update. Exp Neurol 218 (2): 308–315. Takuma, K., J. Yao, J. Huang, H. Xu, X. Chen, J. Luddy, A. C. Trillat, D. M. Stern, O. Arancio, and S. S. Yan. 2005. Abad enhances Abeta-induced cell stress via mitochondrial dysfunction. Faseb J 19 (6): 597–598. Tretter, L., and V. Adam-Vizi. 2000. Inhibition of Krebs cycle enzymes by hydrogen peroxide: A key role of [alpha]-ketoglutarate dehydrogenase in limiting NADH production under oxidative stress. J Neurosci 20 (24 (2000): 8972–8979. Uemura, K., A. Kuzuya, and S. Shimohama. 2004. Protein trafficking and Alzheimer ’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 1 (1): 1–10. Valla, J., L. Schneider, T. Niedzielko, K. D. Coon, R. Caselli, M. N. Sabbagh, G. L. Ahern, et al. 2006. Impaired platelet mitochondrial activity in Alzheimer ’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Mitochondrion 6 (6): 323–330. van der Heide, L. P., G. M. Ramakers, and M. P. Smidt. 2006. Insulin signaling in the central nervous system: Learning to survive. Prog Neurobiol 79 (4): 205–221. Van Houten, M., B. I. Posner, B. M. Kopriwa, and J. R. Brawer. 1979. Insulin-binding sites in the rat brain: in vivo localization to the circumventricular organs by quantitative radioautography. Endocrinology 105: 666–673. Vilchis, C., and R. Salceda. 1996. Effect of diabetes on levels and uptake of putative amino acid neurotransmitters in rat retina and retinal pigment epithelium. Neurochem Res 21 (10): 1167–1171. Wallace, D. C. 1999. Mitochondrial diseases in man and mouse. Science 283 (5407): 1482–1488. Wang, X., B. Su, H. Fujioka, and X. Zhu. 2008. Dynamin-like protein 1 reduction underlies mitochondrial morphology and distribution abnormalities in fibroblasts from sporadic Alzheimer ’s disease patients. Am J Pathol 173 (2): 470–482. Wang, X., B. Su, H. G. Lee, X. Li, G. Perry, M. A. Smith, and X. Zhu. 2009. Impaired balance of mitochondrial fission and fusion in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 29 (28): 9090–9103. Wang, X., B. Su, S. L. Siedlak, P. I. Moreira, H. Fujioka, Y. Wang, G. Casadesus, and X. Zhu. 2008. Amyloid-beta overproduction causes abnormal mitochondrial dynamics via differential modulation of mitochondrial fission/fusion proteins. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105 (49): 19318–19323. Wang, X., B. Su, L. Zheng, G. Perry, M. A. Smith, and X. Zhu. 2009. The role of abnormal mitochondrial dynamics in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurochem 109 (Suppl 1): 153–159.
114
Dementia
Watson, G. S., and S. Craft. 2004. Modulation of memory by insulin and glucose: Neuropsychological observations in Alzheimer ’s disease. Eur J Pharmacol 490 (1–3): 97–113. Wozniak, M., B. Rydzewski, S. P. Baker, and M. K. Raizada. 1993. The cellular and physiological actions of insulin in the central nervous system. Neurochem Int 22: 1–10. Yan, S. D., and D. M. Stern. 2005. Mitochondrial dysfunction and Alzheimer ’s disease: Role of amyloid-beta peptide alcohol dehydrogenase (Abad). Int J Exp Pathol 86 (3): 161–171. Yang, J. W., M. K. Raizada, and R. E. Fellows. 1981. Effects of insulin on cultured rat brain cells: Stimulation of ornithine decarboxylase activity. J Neurochem 36 (3): 1050–1057. Zhao, W. Q., H. Chen, M. J. Quon, and D. L. Alkon. 2004. Insulin and the insulin receptor in experimental models of learning and memory. Eur J Pharmacol 490 (1–3): 71–81. Zhao, W., H. Chen, H. Xu, E. Moore, N. Meiri, M. J. Quon, and D. L. Alkon. 1999. Brain insulin receptors and spatial memory. Correlated changes in gene expression, tyrosine phosphorylation, and signaling molecules in the hippocampus of water maze trained rats. J Biol Chem 274 (49): 34893–34902. Zhu, X., R. J. Castellani, A. Takeda, A. Nunomura, C. S. Atwood, G. Perry, and M. A. Smith. 2001. Differential activation of neuronal ERK, JNK/SAPK and P38 in Alzheimer disease: The “two hit” hypothesis. Mech Ageing Dev 123 (1): 39–46. Zhu, X., H. G. Lee, G. Perry, and M. A. Smith. 2007. Alzheimer disease, the two-hit hypothesis: An update. Biochim Biophys Acta 1772 (4): 494–502. Zhu, X., A. K. Raina, G. Perry, and M. A. Smith. 2004. Alzheimer ’s disease: The two-hit hypothesis. Lancet Neurol 3 (4): 219–226.
Chapter 4
Alzheimer’s Disease: Increased Neurogenesis and Possible Disease Mechanisms Related to Neurogenesis Philippe Taupin
Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease for which there is no cure. Aging is the major contributing factor for the increased risk of developing AD. The risk of developing AD doubles every 5 years after the age of 65 and the disease affects more 30% of individuals over the age of 80 (Ferri et al. 2006). There are two forms of the disease, the late-onset AD (LOAD) and the early-onset AD (EOAD). LOAD is diagnosed after the age of 65 and most cases of LOAD are sporadic forms of the disease. LOAD is the most common form of the disease, accounting for over 93% of all cases of AD (Burns, Byrne, and Maurer 2002). EOAD is diagnosed at younger than 65 and most cases of EOAD are inherited forms of AD or familial Alzheimer ’s disease (FAD). It is a rare form of the disease. Genetic, acquired, and environmental risks factors are believed to be causative factors for LOAD, whereas genetic inherited mutations are causal factors for EOAD (Zilka, Ferencik, and Hulin 2006). Among the genetic factors that are established risk factors for LOAD is the presence of certain alleles of the apolipoprotein E gene (ApoE) in the genetic makeup of the individual. These risk factors increase the probability of developing AD. Mutations causative for EOAD concern a number of genes, some of which have been characterized. These genes are referred to as familial Alzheimer genes, among which is the gene of beta-amyloid precursor protein (APP). About 200 families in the world
116
Dementia
carry genetic mutations that lead to the development of the disease. Rare cases of sporadic form of EOAD occur, with no family history and no identified causal genetic mutations. The diagnosis of AD is primarily performed by symptoms, like cognitive impairments and behavioral changes, and by the assessments of risk factors (Dubois et al. 2007; Patterson et al. 2008). The average life expectancy of patients diagnosed with AD is 8.5 years. Current treatments consist in drug and occupational therapies (Scarpini, Scheltens, and Feldman 2003). Recent advances in adult neurogenesis and neural stem cell (NSC) research open new opportunities for our understanding of and for developing new treatments and cures for AD. Neurogenesis, the generation of nerve cells, occurs in the adult brain and NSCs reside in the adult central nervous system (CNS) of mammals, including in humans (Gage 2000). NSCs are the self-renewing multipotent cells that have the ability to give rise to the main phenotypes of the nervous system, nerve cells, astrocytes, and oligodendrocytes. In the adult brain, neurogenesis occurs primarily in two regions, the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus and the subventricular zone (SVZ) along the ventricles (Eriksson et al. 1998; Taupin 2006; Curtis et al. 2007). In the DG, newly generated neuronal cells in the subgranular zone (SGZ) migrate to the granule cell layer, where they differentiate into granule-like cells and extend axonal projections to the CA3 region of the Ammon’s horn (Gould et al. 1998; Taupin 2009, “Characterization”). Newly generated neuronal cells in the anterior part of the SVZ migrate through the rostro-migratory stream to the olfactory bulb, where they differentiate into interneurons (Lois and Alvarez-Buylla 1994; Doetsch and Alvarez-Buylla 1996). It is postulated that newly generated neuronal cells in the adult brain originate from NSCs. Because of their potential to generate the main phenotypes of the nervous system, NSCs represent a promising model for cellular therapy for treating a vast array of neurological diseases and injuries, and particularly neurodegenerative disease like AD (Taupin 2008, “Adult neural stem cells”). The stimulation of locally endogenous neural progenitor or stem cells in the adult brain or the transplantation of neural progenitor and stem cells, isolated from the adult brain and propagated in vitro, are proposed to repair and restore the degenerated or injured nerve pathways. The confirmation that adult neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain and NSCs reside in the adult CNS, reveals that the adult brain may be amenable to repair. The contribution of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells to the physiopathology and functioning of the nervous system remains the source and center of intense interest and research. Reports show that neurogenesis is enhanced in the brain of patients with AD (Jin, Peel, et al. 2004). Aneuploidy would underlie the process of
Alzheimer’s Disease
117
neurodegeneration and amyloid formation. The process of adult neurogenesis holds the potential to generate populations of cells that are aneuploids, particularly in the hippocampus. Do adult neurogenesis and NSCs contribute to the pathology of neurological diseases like AD? Do adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain contribute to pathogenesis of AD? In the following sections, we will review and discuss the potential involvement of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain in the pathology and pathogenesis of AD. ETIOLOGY AND PATHOLOGY OF ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE Alzheimer ’s disease is a neurodegenerative disease. It is associated initially with the loss of nerve cells in areas of the brain that are vital to memory and other mental abilities, like the enthorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and neocortex. As the disease advances, other regions of the brain are affected, including the medial temporal area, lateral hemisphere, basal forebrain, and locus coeruleus, leading to severe incapacities (Burns et al. 2002). As the disease and neurodegeneration further progress, so do the disabilities and impairments, leading ultimately to death. AD was described by Alois Alzheimer in 1906, who reported first the histopathological features of AD: the presence of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the brain of patients with severe dementia (Alzheimer 1906). Amyloid Plaques and Neurofibrillary Tangles Amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles are the hallmarks of AD. Amyloid plaques are extracellular deposits of proteins surrounded by degenerating nerve cells, in the brain of patients with AD (Anderson et al. 2004). They are composed of amyloid fibrils. Amyloid fibrils are aggregates of protein beta-amyloid. Protein beta-amyloid is a 40 amino acid beta-peptide. It is synthesized and secreted by nerve cells, by posttranscriptional maturation of APP (Kang et al. 1987). APP is processed by alpha-, beta- and gamma-secretase enzymes. Protein beta-amyloid is an amyloidogenic protein. These proteins are soluble in their physiological state. Under pathological conditions, they form insoluble extracellular aggregates or deposits of amyloid fibrils (Serpell, Sunde, and Blake 1997). In physiological conditions, APP is cleaved by the alpha- and gamma-secretase enzymes into a 40 amino acid betapeptide. Certain pathological conditions, like the presence or expression of amyloid-promoting factors or certain gene mutations, including in
118
Dementia
APP, cause excessive cleavage of APP by the beta- and gamma-secretase enzymes, resulting in an increase production of a 42 amino acid betaamyloid peptide. This latter form of protein beta-amyloid aggregates into insoluble amyloid deposits, particularly in the brain, forming aggregates and deposits of amyloid fibrils. Amyloid plaques are thought to be the first histological change that occurs in the brain of patients with AD (St. George-Hyslop 2000). The density of amyloid plaques increases as the disease advances. They are distributed throughout the brain of those patients, particularly in the region of degeneration, like the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, temporal, frontal, and inferior parietal lobes. The role and contribution of amyloid plaques in the pathology of AD remain unclear and the source of controversies. On the one hand, it is proposed that deposits of protein beta-amyloid may be a causative factor of AD. According to this hypothesis, referred as amyloid hypothesis, as the amyloid deposits in the brain, brain cells start dying, and the signs and symptoms of the disease begin (Hardy and Selkoe 2002; Meyer-Luehmann et al. 2006). On the other hand, the correlation between the density of amyloid plaques and the severity of the dementia is not clearly established (Terry 1996). The deposit of protein beta-amyloid would be a consequence rather than a cause of AD. Neurofibrillary tangles are deposits of proteins present inside neuronal cells in the brain of patients with AD. They are composed of hyperphosphorylated tau proteins (Fukutani et al. 1995). Tau protein is a microtubuleassociated phosphoprotein. It is involved in the formation of microtubules (Kim, Jensen, and Rebhun 1986). The hyperphosphorylation of tau proteins result in their aggregation and in the breakdown of microtubules (Iqbal et al. 1998). This leads to the formation of neurofibrillary tangles and cell death (Alonso et al. 2001). As the disease advances, the regions of the brain affected expand, leading to severe incapacity and death (Brun and Gustafson 1976). Genetic Factors and Mutations There are two forms of the diseases, sporadic and inherited. Most cases of LOAD are sporadic forms of the disease and are diagnosed after the age of 65. EOAD is diagnosed at younger age than 65 and most cases of EOAD are inherited forms of AD. LOAD is believed to be caused by genetic, acquired, and environmental factors, among them the presence of certain alleles in the genetic makeup of the individuals, hypertension and diabetes, and neuroinflammation and oxidative stress (Cankurtaran et al. 2008). The presence of the apolipoprotein E varepsilon 4 allele (ApoE4) is the
Alzheimer’s Disease
119
best established genetic risk factor for LOAD. ApoE is a plasma protein; it participates in the transport of cholesterol and other lipids in the blood (Mahley 1988). There are four major isoforms of the gene coding for ApoE encoded by different alleles in humans, ApoE, ApoE2, ApoE3 and ApoE4. ApoE accounts for the vast majority of causes and risks to develop LOAD: up to 50% of people who have AD have at least one ApoE4 allele. Neuronal sortilin-related receptor (SORL1) belongs to a family of proteins termed retromer (Raber, Huang, and Ashford 2004). Retromers are involved in intracellular trafficking. Reduced expression of the gene coding for SORL1 (SORL1) is associated with an increase in density of amyloid plaques in the brain and increased risk for LOAD. The variants of SORL1 may promote AD by suppressing the activity of the gene. This may affect the processing of APP and increase its production (Rogaeva et al. 2007). Other genes have been linked with the occurence of LOAD, among them variants for the genes coding for alpha2-macroglobulin, monoamine oxidase A, myeloperoxidase and cystatin C (CST3) (Finckh et al. 2000). These risk factors increase the probability of developing the disease. So far, three genes have been identified as carrying genetic mutations underlying the development of EOAD. These genes are also known as FAD genes. These genes are the APP gene, the presenilin-1 gene (PSEN1) and the presenilin-2 gene (PSEN2) (Schellenberg 1995). APP is a 695-770 amino acid protein coding for the protein beta-amyloid. The PSEN proteins are components of the gamma-secretase complex. These enzymes play a role in the maturation of APP into the 42 protein beta-amyloid (Nishimura, Yu, and St. George-Hyslop 1999). Mutations in PSEN1 and PSEN2 lead to excessive cleavage by gamma-secretase enzyme, resulting in increased production and aggregation of protein beta-amyloid (Newman, Musgrave, and Lardelli 2007). Mutations in these genes almost always result in the individual developing the disease (Hardy 2001). Aneuploidy Aneuploidy is an abnormal number of chromosomes in the cells of the body. It is a common cause of genetic disorders. Several studies report that cells of patients with AD elicit aneuploidy, particularly for chromosome 21, 13, and 18. Lymphocytes of patients with LOAD present an elevation in aneuploidy for chromosomes 13 and 21 (Migliore et al. 1999). Preparations of lymphocytes of patients with sporadic and inherited forms of AD elicit a two-fold increase in the incidence of aneuploidy for chromosomes 18 and 21 (Geller and Potter 1999). In regions of degeneration 4–10% of neurons, like the hippocampus, are aneuploids and express proteins of
120
Dementia
the cell cycle in the brain of patients with AD (Busser, Geldmacher, and Herrup 1998; Yang, Geldmacher, and Herrup 2001). The adult brain contains a substantial number of cells that are aneuploids; estimated at 5–7% of the cells in the brain of adult mice (Rehen et al. 2005). The genetic imbalance in aneuploid cells signifies that they are fated to die (Herrup et al. 2004). The relatively high percentage of aneuploid cells in regions of degeneration in AD brains suggests that they undergo a slow death process. These cells may live in this state for months, possibly up to 1 year (Herrup and Arendt 2002; Yang, Mufson, and Herrup 2003). This supports their involvement in the slow and progressive neurodegenerative process of AD. Cyclin B, the marker of the phase G2 of the cell cycle, is also expressed in neurons in regions of degeneration, particularly the hippocampus, in patients with AD (Vincent, Rosado, and Davies 1996). In the adult brain, most nerve cells are post-mitotic cells. The characterization of aneuploidy and cyclin B in nerve cells in the region of degeneration reveal that cell cycle re-entry and DNA duplication, without cell division, precedes neuronal death in the brain of patients with AD. The deregulation and/or re-expression of proteins of the cell cycle in nerve cells triggering cycle re-entry, with blockage in phase G2, and aneuploidy would underlie the neurodegenerative process and pathogenesis of AD. Enhanced Neurogenesis The expression of markers of immature neuronal cells, like doublecortin and polysialylated nerve cell adhesion molecule, is enhanced in the hippocampus, particularly the DG, in the brain of AD patients, most likely with LOAD (Jin, Peel, et al. 2004). In animal models, neurogenesis is decreased in the DG of adult mice deficient for PSEN1 and/or APP, in the DG of adult transgenic mice over expressing variants of APP or PSEN1, and in the DG of adult PDAPP transgenic mice, a mouse model of AD with age-dependent accumulation of protein beta-amyloid (Wen et al. 2002; Donovan et al. 2006; Verret et al. 2007; Zhang et al. 2007; Rodríguez et al. 2008). It is increased in the DG of adult transgenic mice that express the Swedish and Indiana APP mutations (Jin, Galvan, et al. 2004). Mice deficient for or over expressing variants of APP or PSEN1, and transgenic mice that express the Swedish and Indiana APP mutations, a mutant form of human APP, are transgenic mice that express variants of FAD genes. Transgenic mice deficient for APP and PSEN1 provide information on the activities and functions of the proteins involved in EOAD. They are not representative of complex diseases, like LOAD. They do not represent the disease. The aggregation of protein beta-amyloid affects adult
Alzheimer’s Disease
121
neurogenesis (Heo et al. 2007). It may have adverse effects on neurogenesis during development in transgenic mice for APP, affecting the adult phenotype. In all, the discrepancies of the data observed on adult neurogenesis in autopsies and animal models of AD may originate from the validity of the animal models used in those studies, as representative of AD and to study adult phenotypes (German and Eisch 2004). The discrepancies of the data observed on adult neurogenesis may also originate from the validity of the protocols used as a paradigm to study adult neurogenesis, like the immunohistochemistry for markers of the cell cycle and for the thymidine analog bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU). Most of the studies conducted in autopsies and animal models of neurological diseases and disorders use either immunoshistochemistry for markers of the cell cycle or the BrdU labeling paradigm, to study and quantify adult neurogenesis in situ. Proteins of the cell cycle, like cyclin B—the marker of the phase G2—are expressed in neurons in regions where neurodegeneration occurs. Some at-risk neurons in regions of degeneration are aneuploids in the brain of patients with AD (Busser, Geldmacher, and Herrup 1998; Yang, Geldmacher, and Herrup 2001). Cell cycle re-entry and DNA duplication, without cell division, precedes neuronal death in degenerating regions of the CNS. This suggests that when using immunohistochemistry for proteins of the cell cycle, to study adult neurogenesis, this paradigm does not allow discrimination between cells undergoing DNA duplication, without cell division, as part of their pathological fate and newly generated neuronal cells (Taupin 2007). BrdU is used for birth dating and monitoring cell proliferation (Miller and Nowakowski 1988). There are pitfalls and limitations over the use of thymidine analogs, and particularly BrdU, for studying neurogenesis (Nowakowski and Hayes 2000; Gould and Gross 2002). BrdU is a thymidine analog. It is not a marker of cell proliferation; it is a marker for DNA synthesis. Studying and quantifying neurogenesis with BrdU require distinguishing cell proliferation and neurogenesis from other events involving DNA synthesis, like DNA repair, abortive cell cycle re-entry and cell cycle re-entry and gene duplication, without cell division, leading to aneuploidy (Taupin 2007). In addition, BrdU has a number of side effects. It is a toxic and mutagenic substance. It alters DNA stability and lengthens the cell cycle. BrdU has mitogenic, transcriptional, and translational effects on cells that incorporate it. It triggers cell death and the formation of teratomes. Hence, data involving the use of immunohistochemistry for proteins of the cell cycle and BrdU labeling, as paradigms for studying adult neurogenesis in neurological diseases and disorders, and particularly in AD, must be carefully assessed, analyzed, and discussed.
122
Dementia
In all, AD is a neurodegenerative disease that affect mostly individuals over 65 years of age. There are two forms of the disease, sporadic and inherited. It is characterized by widespread neurodegeneration, amyloid deposits and neurofibrillary tangles, aneuploidy and enhanced neurogenesis, though this latter observation remains to be fully established. It is proposed that enhanced neurogenesis may be a result, rather than a cause, of the illness (Taupin 2008, “Adult neurogenesis pharmacology”; Taupin 2008, “Adult neurogenesis and drug therapy”). Enhanced neurogenesis in the DG of the brain with neurological diseases and disorders, particularly neurodegenerative diseases, may contribute to a regenerative attempt, to compensate for the neuronal loss. POSSIBLE MECHANISMS RELATED TO ADULT NEUROGENESIS The confirmation that adult neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain and NSCs reside in the adult CNS not only brings new opportunities for the treatment of AD, but also raises the question of the involvement of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain in the etiology and pathogenesis of the disease. Amyloid plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, aneuploidy and enhanced neurogenesis are landmarks of the pathology of AD, but their role and contribution to AD remain to be fully elucidated and established, this particularly in light of and relation to recent developments in adult neurogenesis and NSC research. Aneuploidy in AD Patients Aneuploidy may originate from the nondisjunction of chromosomes during mitosis or meiosis. It may originate from cell cycle re-entry with cells undergoing DNA duplication without cell division and from cell fusion (Alvarez-Dolado et al. 2003; Torres, Williams, and Amon 2008). Cells that are the most likely to develop aneuploidy are dividing cells. Lymphocytes of patients with EOAD and LOAD elicit an elevation in aneuploidy for chromosome 13, 18, and 21 (Geller and Potter 1999; Migliore et al. 1999). Hence, the nondisjunction of chromosomes, particularly of chromosomes 13, 18 and 21, in stem cells and/or populations of somatic cells that retain their ability to divide is at the origin of aneuploidy in patients with AD (Potter 1991). In the adult brain, most nerve cells are post-mitotic. The characterization of cyclin B and aneuploidy in neurons suggests that cells re-entered the cell cycle and underwent DNA replication, but did not complete the cell cycle, in regions of degeneration in the brain of patients with AD
Alzheimer’s Disease
123
(Vincent, Rosado, and Davies 1996; Busser, Geldmacher, and Herrup 1998; Yang, Geldmacher, and Herrup 2001). AD is associated with the loss of nerve cells initially in areas of the brain, like the enthorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and neocortex. As the disease advances, other regions of the brain are affected by neurodegeneration, including the medial temporal area, lateral hemisphere, basal forebrain, and locus coeruleus. The genetic imbalance in aneuploid cells signifies that they are fated to die and that they undergo a slow death process (Yang, Mufson, and Herrup 2003; Herrup et al. 2004). Cell cycle re-entry and DNA replication, without mitosis, is at the origin of aneuploidy in nerve cells of the adult brain. It is an underlying factor in the neurodegenerative process and pathogenesis of AD. Aneuploidy for Chromosome 21 and Amyloid Deposits Amyloid plaques are deposits of protein amyloid (Anderson et al. 2004). Deposit of protein amyloid is one of the histopathological features of AD and one the probable cause for the pathogenesis of AD. The gene for APP is located on chromosome 21 (21q21) (Goldgaber et al. 1987; Schellenberg et al. 1992). Cells of patients with AD elicit aneuploidy, particularly for chromosome 21 (Geller and Potter 1999; Migliore et al. 1999). Aneuploidy for chromosome 21 would result in the overexpression of APP and promote the formation of amyloid plaques. In patients with FAD, with mutation of the APP gene, it would result in the overexpression of mutant form of amyloid protein in aneuploid cells and amyloid formation. In patients with the sporadic form of AD, it would result in the overexpression of wild type amyloid protein in aneuploid cells and amyloid formation, under certain conditions or risk factors. According to the amyloid hypothesis, aneuploidy for chromosome 21 would underlie cell death and the pathogenesis of AD. In support of this contention, Down’s syndrome has for pathogenic cause trisomy for the chromosome 21. Patients with Down’s syndrome develop, during their thirties and forties, dementia and neuropathology that share some characteristics with AD, particularly with regard to amyloid formation and deposits (Glenner and Wong 1984). Aneuploidy for chromosome 21 would underlie the pathogenesis and pathology of the dementia that occurs in patients with Down’s syndrome and AD. Aneuploidy for chromosome 21 has been proposed as one of the mechanisms underlying the formation of amyloid deposits and the pathogenesis of AD and Down’s syndrome (Potter 1991). Protein beta-amyloid induces cell cycle re-entry and neuronal death (Chen et al. 2000). Hence, aneuploidy
124
Dementia
for chromosome 21 in neurons in regions of degeneration would underlie the pathogenesis of AD, not only by promoting the formation of amyloid plaques, but also by promoting cell cycle re-entry and DNA duplication, without cell division, leading to aneuploidy and neuronal cell death. Aneuploidy for Chromosome 17 and Neurofibrillary Tangles Formation Neurofibrillary tangles are one of the histopathological features of AD and one the probable cause for cell death in AD. Neurofibrillary tangles are deposits of proteins present inside neuronal cells (Alonso et al. 2001). They are composed of hyperphosphorylated tau proteins (Fukutani et al. 1995). The tau gene is located on chromosome 17 (17q21.1) (Iqbal et al. 1989). Aneuploidy for chromosome 17 would result in the overexpression of tau protein. It would underlie the pathogenesis of AD, by promoting the formation of neurofibrillary tangles and cell death. Aneuploidy for Chromosomes 1, 14 and 19 and Pathogenesis of AD The PSEN1 and PSEN2 genes carry genetic mutations underlying the development of EOAD (Schellenberg 1995). The PSEN proteins, components of the gamma-secretase complex, play a role in the maturation of APP into protein beta-amyloid (Nishimura, Yu, and St. George-Hyslop 1999). Mutations in PSEN1 and PSEN2 lead to excessive cleavage by the gamma-secretase enzyme. This results in increased production and aggregation of protein beta-amyloid, leading to the development of the EOAD (Newman, Musgrave, and Lardelli 2007). The PSEN1 and PSEN2 genes are located on chromosome 14 (14q24.3) and 1 (1q31–q42), respectively (Nishimura et al. 1999). The presence the ApoE4 allele is a genetic risk factor for LOAD. The ApoE gene is located on chromosome 19q13.2. Aneuploidy for chromosomes 1, 14 and 19, and more generally for chromosomes carrying genes involved in the development of AD, including SORL1 and CST3 genes, would contribute to the pathogenesis of the disease, EOAD or LOAD, depending on the gene involved in the disease. In support of this contention, people who have two ApoE4 alleles have a higher risk of being diagnosed with AD, after age of 65 (Strittmatter et al. 1993). Aneuploidy and Adult Neurogenesis Neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain and NSCs reside in the adult CNS. In the adult mammalian brain, neurogenesis occurs primarily in the
Alzheimer’s Disease
125
DG and SVZ. The process of adult neurogenesis holds the potential to generate populations of cells that are aneuploids particularly in the neurogenic areas. The nondisjunction of chromosomes during the process of cell division of newly generated progenitor cells of the adult brain could lead to newly generated neuronal cells that are aneuploids or to aneuploid cells that would not proceed with their developmental program (Taupin 2009, “Adult neurogenesis, neural stem cells”; Taupin 2009, “Adult neurogenesis in the pathogenesis”) (see Figure 4.1). Hence, neurogenesis could also be a contributing factor of aneuploidy in AD. In the adult brain aneuploidy may therefore originate both from cycle re-entry and DNA duplication, without cell division, in regions of degeneration including the hippocampus, and from the nondisjunction of chromosomes in neural progenitor and stem cells of the adult brain, and their progenies, that retain their ability to divide in neurogenic areas particularly in the hippocampus. This reveals that adult neurogenesis could be an underlying factor in the neurodegenerative process and therefore pathogenesis of AD. It is estimated that 0.004% of the granule cell population is generated per day in the DG of adult macaque monkeys (Kornack and Rakic 1999). Despite neurogenesis being an event with relatively low frequency in the adult mammalian brain, the fact that cells that are the most likely to develop aneuploidy are dividing cells and that neurogenesis occurs in the adult hippocampus, a region of the brain particularly and among the first affected in AD, suggests that aneuploidy originating from adult neurogenesis may play a critical role in the process of degeneration and pathogenesis in AD. Such aneuploidy, particularly for chromosomes 17 and 21 and other genes involved in AD, would further contribute to the pathogenesis of AD, by promoting the formation of amyloid deposits and neurofibrillary tangles in the neurogenic areas, particularly the hippocampus. In all, adult neurogenesis may play a critical role in the pathogenesis of AD, in the process of neurodegeneration, amyloid deposits and neurofibrillary tangles formation.
Factors Promoting Aneuploidy in Alzheimer ’s Disease Hyperphosphorylation of Tau Protein Hyperphosphorylated tau protein is a component and promotes the formation of neurofibrillary tangles (Alonso et al. 2001). Tau is a microtubule-associated protein, involved in the formation of microtubules (Kim, Jensen, and Rebhun 1986). The hyperphosphorylation of tau by kinases leads to the dissociation of tau and tubulin, and to the breakdown of
126
Dementia
Figure 4.1 Fate of Newly Generated Neural Progenitor and Neuronal Cells in the Adult Brain
Neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain, primarily in the DG of the hippocampus and SVZ. Adult NSCs represent a promising model for cellular therapy for treating a vast array of neurological diseases and injuries, and particularly neurodegenerative diseases like AD. The role and contribution of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain to the physiopathology and functioning of the nervous system remain to be elucidated. NSCs are the self-renewing multipotent cells that have the ability to give rise to the main phenotypes of the nervous system; they generate a large number of progenies through an intermediate population of the cells, the neural progenitor cells (in light gray). (1. Apoptosis) Cell death is a normally occurring process in the neurogenic zones, as a significant proportion of newly generated cells are believed to undergo apoptosis rather than achieving maturity. (2. Normal development) Newly generated neuronal cells that survive, survive for an extended period of time, at least 2 years in humans, and extend functional projections. They may be involved in plasticity and contribute to regenerative attempts in the diseased and injured nervous system, particularly in AD. (3. Aneuploidy) Aneuploidy is a landmark of the pathology of AD and contributes to the pathogenesis of the disease. The process of adult neurogenesis holds the potential to generate populations of neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids (in dark gray). The nondisjunction of chromosomes during the process of cell division of newly generated neural progenitor cells of the adult brain could lead to newly generated neuronal cells that are aneuploids (A) or to newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program (B). The genetic imbalance in aneuploid cells signifies that they are fated to die. Newly generated neuronal cells that are aneuploids and newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program in the adult brain may contribute to the pathogenesis of AD. They may contribute to the process of neurodegeneration, amyloid deposits and neurofibrillary tangles formation, particularly in the hippocampus. This reveals that adult neurogenesis would not only be beneficial for the adult brain, but it may also be involved in the pathogenesis of neurological diseases, particularly in AD.
Alzheimer’s Disease
127
microtubles (Iqbal et al. 1998). This causes the disruption in the mitotic spindle, promoting aneuploidy during mitosis. Hyperphosphorylated tau protein may contribute to the pathogenesis of AD, not only by the polymerization and aggregation of tau proteins, resulting in the formation of neurofibrillary tangles and cell death, but also by promoting the nondisjunction of chromosomes and aneuploidy in dividing cells. Hyperphosphorylated tau protein could be a contributing factor in the generation of newly generated neuronal cells that are aneuploids or to newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program in the adult brain, particularly in the hippocampus (Taupin 2009, “Adult neurogenesis, neural stem cells”). Mutation in PSEN1 The PSEN1 proteins are components of the gamma-secretase complex and play a role in the maturation of APP into protein beta-amyloid (Nishimura, Yu, and St. George-Hyslop 1999). Mutated forms of PSEN1 are detected in the centrosomes and interphase kinetochores of dividing cells. Mutated PSEN1 proteins may be involved in the segragation and migration of chromosomes during cells division (Li et al. 1997). Mutated PSEN1 proteins may contribute to the pathogenesis of EOAD, not only by promoting the formation of deposits of amyloid fibrils, but also by promoting the nondisjunction of chromosomes and aneuploidy in dividing cells (Boeras et al. 2008). Mutated PSEN1 proteins could be a contributing factor in the generation of aneuploid newly generated neuronal cells or to aneuploid newly generated neural progenitor cells that would not proceed with their developmental program in the adult brain, particularly in the hippocampus (Taupin 2009, “Adult neurogenesis, neural stem cells”). Modulation of Adult Neurogenesis Neurogenesis is modulated in the adult brain, particularly in the hippocampus, by a broad range of environmental, physio- and pathological stimuli and processes, trophic factors/cytokines and drugs (Taupin 2007). The stimulation of neurogenesis in the adult brain may contribute to the generation of newly generated neuronal cells that are aneuploids or to newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program in the neurogenic regions of the adult brain, particularly the hippocampus. It is reported that neurogenesis is enhanced in the hippocampus, particularly the DG, in the brain of patients with AD (Jin, Peel, et al. 2004). Enhanced neurogenesis in the DG of the brain with neurological diseases and disorders, particularly
128
Dementia
neurodegenerative diseases, may contribute to a regenerative attempt to compensate for the neuronal loss (Taupin 2008, “Adult neurogenesis pharmacology”). Hence, enhanced neurogenesis in the DG of the brain of patients with AD could be a contributing factor in the generation of newly generated neuronal cells that are aneuploids or of newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program in the adult brain and therefore could be a contributing factor to the pathogenesis of AD (Taupin 2009, “Adult neurogenesis, neural stem cells”). Oxidative Stress Oxidative stress is an environmental risk factor for LOAD. Oxidative stress induces cell cycle re-entry and neuronal death (Langley and Ratan 2004). It promotes aneuploidy, particularly for chromosome 17 that carries the tau gene (Ramírez et al. 2000). Oxidative stress may promote the pathogenesis of LOAD, not only by promoting cell cycle re-entry and DNA duplication, without cell division, in the brain, leading to neuronal death and the process neurodegeneration, but also by promoting the generation of aneuploid cells fated to die and the formation of neurofibrillary tangles, leading to cell death (Taupin 2009, “Adult neurogenesis, neural stem cells”). CONCLUSION AND PERSPECTIVES The confirmation that adult neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain and NSCs reside in the adult CNS of mammals reveals that the adult brain has the potential for self repair. It opens new opportunities for the treatment of a broad range of neurological diseases and injuries, including neurodegenerative diseases like AD, cerebral strokes, and spinal cord injuries. The role and contribution of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells to the physio- and pathology of the adult brain remain to be elucidated, particularly in neurodegenerative diseases like AD. Neurogenesis is enhanced in the hippocampus of patients with AD. Though these data remain to be confirmed and validated, it suggests that adult neurogenesis would contribute to a regenerative attempt to compensate for the neuronal loss in AD. Aneuploidy is a landmark of the pathology of AD and contributes to the pathogenesis of the disease. Aneuploidy contributes directly and indirectly to the processes of amyloid deposits, neurofibrillary tangles formation and neurodegeneration in the brain. Cells that are the most likely to develop aneuploidy are dividing cells. Hence, the process of adult
Alzheimer’s Disease
129
neurogenesis holds the potential to generate populations of aneuploid cells particularly in the hippocampus, a region where neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain and that is primarily affected in AD. Newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain that are aneuploids or newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program may contribute to the pathogenesis of AD, in the process of neurodegeneration, amyloid deposits, and neurofibrillary tangle formation. This reveals that adult neurogenesis would not only elicit a beneficial effect for the adult brain, but it may also be involved in the pathogenesis of neurological diseases and disorders, particularly in AD. Adult neurogenesis may be the target of new drugs aimed at treating AD, by promoting neuroregeneration and decreasing the risk of the generation of newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain that are aneuploids or of newly generated neural progenitor cells that are aneuploids and would not proceed with their developmental program in the adult hippocampus. Future studies will aim at characterizing the role and contribution of adult neurogenesis and NSCs in the pathogenesis and pathology of AD and other neurological diseases. Results from such studies will lead to a better understanding of neurological diseases and disorders, and to novel and more effective treatments and cures for these diseases, particularly AD.
REFERENCES Alonso, A., T. Zaidi, M. Novak, I. Grundke-Iqbal, and K. Iqbal. 2001. Hyperphosphorylation induces self-assembly of tau into tangles of paired helical filaments/straight filaments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 6923–6928. Alvarez-Dolado, M., R. Pardal, J. M. Garcia-Verdugo, J. R. Fike, H. O. Lee, K. Pfeffer, C. Lois, S. J. Morrison, and A. Alvarez-Buylla. 2003. Fusion of bone-marrow-derived cells with Purkinje neurons, cardiomyocytes and hepatocytes. Nature 425: 968–973. Alzheimer, A. 1906. Uber einen eigenartigen schweren erkrankungsprozeb der hirnrinde. Neurologisches cenrealblatt 23: 1129–1136. Anderson, D. H., K. C. Talaga, A. J. Rivest, E. Barron, G. S. Hageman, and L. V. Johnson. 2004. Characterization of beta amyloid assemblies in drusen: The deposits associated with aging and age-related macular degeneration. Exp Eye Res 78: 243–256. Boeras, D. I., A. Granic, J. Padmanabhan, N. C. Crespo, A. M. Rojiani, and H. Potter. 2008. Alzheimer ’s presenilin 1 causes chromosome missegregation and aneuploidy. Neurobiol Aging 29: 319–328. Brun, A., and L. Gustafson. 1976. Distribution of cerebral degeneration in Alzheimer ’s disease: A clinico-pathological study. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 223: 15–33.
130
Dementia
Burns, A., E. J. Byrne, and K. Maurer. 2002. Alzheimer ’s disease. Lancet 360: 163–165. Busser, J., D. S. Geldmacher, and K. Herrup. 1998. Ectopic cell cycle proteins predict the sites of neuronal cell death in Alzheimer ’s disease brain. J Neurosci 18: 2801–2807. Cankurtaran, M., B. B. Yavuz, E. S. Cankurtaran, M. Halil, Z. Ulger, and S. Ariogul. 2008. Risk factors and type of dementia: Vascular or Alzheimer? Arch Gerontol Geriatr 47: 25–34. Chen, Y., D. L. McPhie, J. Hirschberg, and R. L. Neve. 2000. The amyloid precursor protein-binding protein APP-BP1 drives the cell cycle through the S-M checkpoint and causes apoptosis in neurons. J Biol Chem 275: 8929–8935. Curtis, M. A., M. Kam, U. Nannmark, M. F. Anderson, M. Z. Axell, C. Wikkelso, S. Holtas, et al. 2007. Human neuroblasts migrate to the olfactory bulb via a lateral ventricular extension. Science 315: 1243–1249. Doetsch, F., and A. Alvarez-Buylla. 1996. Network of tangential pathways for neuronal migration in adult mammalian brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: 14895–14900. Donovan, M. H., U. Yazdani, R. D. Norris, D. Games, D. C. German, and A. J. Eisch. 2006. Decreased adult hippocampal neurogenesis in the PDAPP mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. J Comp Neurol 495: 70–83. Dubois, B., H. H. Feldman, C. Jacova, S. T. DeKosky, P. Barberger-Gateau, J. Cummings, A. Delacourte, et al. 2007. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: Revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol 6: 734–746. Eriksson, P. S., E. Perfilieva, T. Bjork-Eriksson, A. M. Alborn, C. Nordborg, D. A. Peterson, and F. H. Gage. 1998. Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med 4: 1313–1317. Ferri, C. P., M. Prince, C. Brayne, H. Brodaty, L. Fratiglioni, M. Ganguli, K. Hall, et al. 2006. The role of oxidative stress in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer ’s disease. Bratisl Lek Listy 107: 384–394. Finckh, U., H. von der Kammer, J. Velden, T. Michel, B. Andresen, A. Deng, J. Zhang, et al. 2000. Genetic association of a cystatin C gene polymorphism with late-onset Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 57: 1579–1583. Fukutani, Y., K. Kobayashi, I. Nakamura, K. Watanabe, K. Isaki, and N. J. Cairns. 1995. Neurons, intracellular and extra cellular neurofibrillary tangles in subdivisions of the hippocampal cortex in normal ageing and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurosci Lett 200: 57–60. Gage, F. H. 2000. Mammalian neural stem cells. Science 287: 1433–1438. Geller, L. N., and H. Potter. 1999. Chromosome missegregation and trisomy 21 mosaicism in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Dis 6: 167–179. German, D. C., and A. J. Eisch. 2004. Mouse models of Alzheimer ’s disease: Insight into treatment. Rev Neurosci 15: 353–369. Glenner, G. G., and C. W. Wong. 1984. Alzheimer ’s disease and Down’s syndrome: Sharing of a unique cerebrovascular amyloid fibril protein. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 122: 1131–1135.
Alzheimer’s Disease
131
Goldgaber, D., M. I. Lerman, O. W. McBride, U. Saffiotti, and D. C. Gajdusek. 1987. Characterization and chromosomal localization of a cDNA encoding brain amyloid of Alzheimer ’s disease. Science 235: 877–880. Gould, E., and C. G. Gross. 2002. Neurogenesis in adult mammals: Some progress and problems. J Neurosci 22: 619–623. Gould, E., P. Tanapat, B. S. McEwen, G. Flugge, and E. Fuchs. 1998. Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 3168–3171. Hardy, J. 2001. The genetic causes of neurodegenerative diseases. J Alzheimers Dis 3: 109–116. Hardy, J., and D. J. Selkoe. 2002. The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer ’s disease: Progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science 297: 353–356. Erratum in: Science 297: 2209. Heo, C., K. A. Chang, H. S. Choi, H. S. Kim, S. Kim, H. Liew, J. A. Kim, E. Yu, J. Ma, and Y. H. Suh. 2007. Effects of the monomeric, oligomeric, and fibrillar Abeta42 peptides on the proliferation and differentiation of adult neural stem cells from subventricular zone. J Neurochem 102: 493–500. Herrup, K., and T. Arendt. 2002. Re-expression of cell cycle proteins induces neuronal cell death during Alzheimer ’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 4: 243–247. Herrup, K., R. Neve, S. L. Ackerman, and A. Copani. 2004. Divide and die: Cell cycle events as triggers of nerve cell death. J Neurosci 24: 9232–9239. Iqbal, K., A. C. Alonso, C. X. Gong, S. Khatoon, J. J. Pei, J. Z. Wang, and I. GrundkeIqbal. 1998. Mechanisms of neurofibrillary degeneration and the formation of neurofibrillary tangles. J Neural Transm Suppl 53: 169–180. Iqbal, K., I. Grundke-Iqbal, A. J. Smith, L. George, Y. C. Tung, and T. Zaidi. 1989. Identification and localization of a tau peptide to paired helical filaments of Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 86: 5646–5650. Jin, K., V. Galvan, L. Xie, X. O. Mao, O. F. Gorostiza, D. E. Bredesen, and D. A. Greenberg. 2004. Enhanced neurogenesis in Alzheimer ’s disease transgenic (PDGF-APPSw, Ind) mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 13363–13367. Jin, K., A. L. Peel, X. O. Mao, L. Xie, B. A. Cottrell, D. C. Henshall, and D. A. Greenberg. 2004. Increased hippocampal neurogenesis in Alzheimer ’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 343–347. Kang, J., H. G. Lemaire, A. Unterbeck, J. M. Salbaum, C. L. Masters, K. H. Grzeschik, G. Multhaup, K. Beyreuther, and B. Müller-Hill. 1987. The precursor of Alzheimer ’s disease amyloid A4 protein resembles a cell-surface receptor. Nature 325: 733–736. Kim, H., C. G. Jensen, and L. I. Rebhun. 1986. The binding of MAP-2 and tau on brain microtubules in vitro: Implications for microtubule structure. Ann NY Acad Sci 466: 218–239. Kornack, D. R., and P. Rakic. 1999. Continuation of neurogenesis in the hippocampus of the adult macaque monkey. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 5768–5773. Langley, B., and R. R. Ratan. 2004. Oxidative stress-induced death in the nervous system: Cell cycle dependent or independent? J Neurosci Res 77: 621–629.
132
Dementia
Li, J., M. Xu, H. Zhou, J. Ma, and H. Potter. 1997. Alzheimer presenilins in the nuclear membrane, interphase kinetochores, and centrosomes suggest a role in chromosome segregation. Cell 90: 917–927. Lois, C., and A. Alvarez-Buylla. 1994. Long-distance neuronal migration in the adult mammalian brain. Science 264: 1145–1148. Mahley, R. W. 1988. Apolipoprotein E: Cholesterol transport protein with expanding role in cell biology. Science 240: 622–630. Meyer-Luehmann, M., J. Coomaraswamy, T. Bolmont, S. Kaeser, C. Schaefer, E. Kilger, A. Neuenschwander, et al. 2006. Exogenous induction of cerebral beta-amyloidogenesis is governed by agent and host. Science 313: 1781–1784. Migliore, L., N. Botto, R. Scarpato, L. Petrozzi, G. Cipriani, and U. Bonuccelli. 1999. Preferential occurrence of chromosome 21 malsegregation in peripheral blood lymphocytes of Alzheimer disease patients. Cytogenet Cell Genet 87: 41–46. Miller, M. W., and R. S. Nowakowski. 1988. Use of bromodeoxyuridine-immunohistochemistry to examine the proliferation, migration and time of origin of cells in the central nervous system. Brain Res 457: 44–52. Newman, M., F. I. Musgrave, and M. Lardelli. 2007. Alzheimer disease: Amyloidogenesis, the presenilins and animal models. Biochim Biophys Acta 1772: 285–297. Nishimura, M., G. Yu, P. H. St. George-Hyslop. 1999. Biology of presenilins as causative molecules for Alzheimer disease. Clin Genet 55: 219–225. Nowakowski, R. S., and N. L. Hayes. 2000. New neurons: Extraordinary evidence or extraordinary conclusion? Science 288: 771. Patterson, C., J. W. Feightner, A. Garcia, G. Y. Hsiung, C. MacKnight, and A. D. Sadovnick. 2008. Diagnosis and treatment of dementia: 1. Risk assessment and primary prevention of Alzheimer disease. CMAJ 178: 548–556. Potter, H. 1991. Review and hypothesis: Alzheimer disease and Down syndrome— chromosome 21 nondisjunction may underlie both disorders. Am J Hum Genet 48: 1192–1200. Raber, J., Y. Huang, and J. W. Ashford. 2004. ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk and AD pathology. Neurobiol Aging 25: 641–650. Ramírez, M. J., S. Puerto, P. Galofré, E. M. Parry, J. M. Parry, A. Creus, R. Marcos, and J. Surrallés. 2000. Multicolour FISH detection of radioactive iodineinduced 17cen-p53 chromosomal breakage in buccal cells from therapeutically exposed patients. Carcinogenesis 21: 1581–1586. Rehen, S. K., Y. C. Yung, M. P. McCreight, D. Kaushal, A. H. Yang, B. S. Almeida, M. A. Kingsbury, et al. 2005. Constitutional aneuploidy in the normal human brain. J Neurosci 25: 2176–2180. Rodríguez, J. J., V. C. Jones, M. Tabuchi, S. M. Allan, E. M. Knight, F. M. LaFerla, S. Oddo, and A. Verkhratsky. 2008. Impaired adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of a triple transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. PLoS ONE 3: e2935.
Alzheimer’s Disease
133
Rogaeva, E., Y. Meng, J. H. Lee, Y. Gu, T. Kawarai, F. Zou, T. Katayama, et al. 2007. The neuronal sortilin-related receptor SORL1 is genetically associated with Alzheimer disease. Nat Genet 39: 168–177. Scarpini, E., P. Scheltens, and H. Feldman. 2003. Treatment of Alzheimer ’s disease: Current status and new perspectives. Lancet Neurol 2: 539–547. Schellenberg, G. D. 1995. Genetic dissection of Alzheimer disease, a heterogeneous disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92: 8552–8559. Schellenberg, G. D., T. D. Bird, E. M. Wijsman, H. T. Orr, L. Anderson, E. Nemens, J. A. White, et al. 1992. Genetic linkage evidence for a familial Alzheimer ’s disease locus on chromosome 14. Science 258: 668–671. Serpell, L. C., M. Sunde, and C. C. Blake. 1997. The molecular basis of amyloidosis. Cell Mol Life Sci 53: 871–887. St. George-Hyslop, P. H. 2000. Piecing together Alzheimer ’s. Sci Am 283: 76–83. Strittmatter, W. J., A. M. Saunders, D. Schmechel, M. Pericak-Vance, J. Enghild, G. S. Salvesen, and A. D. Roses. 1993. Apolipoprotein E: High-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90: 1977–1981. Taupin, P. 2006. Neural progenitor and stem cells in the adult central nervous system. Ann Acad Med Singapore 35: 814–817. Taupin, P. 2007. BrdU immunohistochemistry for studying adult neurogenesis: Paradigms, pitfalls, limitations, and validation. Brain Res Rev 53: 198–214. Taupin, P. 2008. Adult neural stem cells: A promising candidate for regenerative therapy in the CNS. Intl J Integ Biol 2: 85–94. Taupin, P. 2008. Adult neurogenesis pharmacology in neurological diseases and disorders. Expert Rev Neurother 8: 311–320. Taupin, P. 2008. Adult neurogenesis and drug therapy. Cent Nerv Syst Agents Med Chem 8: 198–202. Taupin, P. 2009. Characterization and isolation of synapses of newly generated neuronal cells of the adult hippocampus at early stages of neurogenesis. J Neurodegener Regen 2: 9–17. Taupin, P. 2009. Adult neurogenesis, neural stem cells and Alzheimer ’s disease: Developments, limitations, problems and promises. Curr Alzheimer Res 6: 461–470. Taupin, P. 2009. Adult neurogenesis in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurodegener Regen 2: 6–8. Taupin, P. 2010. A dual activity of ROS and oxidative stress on adult neurogenesis and Alzheimer ’s disease. Cent Nerv Syst Agents Med Chem 10: 16–21. Terry, R. D. 1996. The pathogenesis of Alzheimer disease: An alternative to the amyloidhypothesis. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 55: 1023–1025. Torres, E. M., B. R. Williams, and A. Amon. 2008. Aneuploidy: Cells losing their balance. Genetics 179: 737–746. Verret, L., J. L. Jankowsky, G. M. Xu, D. R. Borchelt, and C. Rampon. 2007. Alzheimer ’s-type amyloidosis in transgenic mice impairs survival of newborn
134
Dementia
neurons derived from adult hippocampal neurogenesis. J Neurosci 27: 6771–6780. Vincent, I., M. Rosado, and P. Davies. 1996. Mitotic mechanisms in Alzheimer ’s disease? J Cell Biol 132: 413–425. Wen, P. H., X. Shao, Z. Shao, P. R. Hof, T. Wisniewski, K. Kelley, V. L. Friedrich Jr., et al. 2002. Overexpression of wild type but not an FAD mutant presenilin-1 promotes neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult mice. Neurobiol Dis 10: 8–19. Yang, Y., D. S. Geldmacher, and K. Herrup. 2001. DNA replication precedes neuronal cell death in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 21: 2661–2668. Yang, Y., E. J. Mufson, and K. Herrup. 2003. Neuronal cell death is preceded by cell cycle events at all stages of Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurosci 23: 2557–2563. Zhang, C., E. McNeil, L. Dressler, and R. Siman. 2007. Long-lasting impairment in hippocampal neurogenesis associated with amyloid deposition in a knock-in mouse model of familial Alzheimer ’s disease. Exp Neurol 204: 77–87. Zilka, N., M. Ferencik, and I. Hulin. 2006. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer ’s disease: Protector or promoter? Bratisl Lek Listy 107: 374–383.
Chapter 5
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances in Dementia Anna Burke
Although dementia is frequently thought of as an impairment in cognition, a common and often overwhelming dilemma facing many families and clinicians is the presence of behavioral and psychological disturbances during the course of the illness. These can include depressive signs and symptoms, agitated behaviors, and psychotic symptoms. It is estimated that as many as 40% to 90% of individuals diagnosed with dementia will experience behavioral or psychological disturbances during the course of their illness (Teri et al 1992; Zuidema et al 2006). The presence of such symptoms is detrimental to patients and their caregivers and results in increased rates of caregiver burn-out, depression, and earlier institutionalization of the affected patient (Kaufer et al. 1998; Donaldson, Tarrier, and Burns 1998; Fuh et al. 2001; Mourik et al 2004; Buhr, Kuchibhatla, and Clipp 2006; Aarsland et al. 2007). Such disturbances also pose a quandary for clinicians as their etiology remains poorly understood and effective treatment options remain few. Behavioral and psychological impairments in dementia may also hinder appropriate treatment of other medical conditions, further frustrating clinicians and families while putting patients at greater medical risk. The focus of this chapter will be to provide a framework for the greater understanding of the changes occurring in the neurotransmitter systems of the brain and how they relate to clinical symptom manifestations in dementia, as well as possible treatment approaches. Since Alzheimer ’s
136
Dementia
dementia (AD) constitutes an estimated 60–80% of all diagnosed dementias, it will serve as the model for the discussion. NEUROTRANSMITTER CHANGES IN DEMENTIA Alzheimer ’s disease is the most common form of dementia, characterized by an insidious onset and a progressive decline in cognitive and functional abilities as a result of the loss of neurons in the cerebral cortex and certain subcortical regions. The disease commonly results in alterations of personality, mood, behaviors, and reality testing, which may occur at any stage of the illness. Despite the prevalence of behavioral and psychotic disturbances in dementia (BPSD), their underlying neurobiology has received far less attention than the neurobiology of cognitive features such as memory impairment in AD. However, disturbances in several neurotransmitter systems measured via serum levels, cerebrospinal fluid levels, and tissue levels have been demonstrated to be of clinical significance in symptom manifestation. Clues as to the underlying mechanisms have also been provided through a greater understanding of other clinical syndromes displaying similar symptoms. Changes in neurotransmitter biosynthesis, biodegradation, presynaptic and postsynaptic receptor binding sites, and second messenger systems, as well as decreased central nervous system uptake of neurotransmitter precursors have all been implicated in BPSD. The most extensively studied neurotransmitter system has been the cholinergic system. However, GABAergic dysfunction and disturbances of the serotonergic and noradrenergic systems have become a prominent focus and treatment target. Acetylcholine Cholinergic deficits are the best-established neurotransmitter changes in AD. In the central nervous system (CNS), acetylcholine is a major neurotransmitter vital for the formation and retention of new memories. It also acts as a neuromodulator, affecting and regulating CNS excitability, arousal, and reward systems. Current scientific theories hypothesize that dementia-related memory issues stem from the increased breakdown of acetylcholine by acetylcholinesterase, an enzyme that degrades acetylcholine, as well as decreased production of the neurotransmitter due to neuronal cell death in dementia. Acetylcholine deficiency in the CNS has been associated with the archetypal symptoms of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease: short-term memory loss, disorientation, sleep disturbances, as
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
137
well as development of psychotic and agitated behaviors. Symptom formation may vary depending on the region of the brain affected, hence the diversity of clinical pathology seen in individuals diagnosed with dementia. Decreased concentrations of choline acetyltransferase, the enzyme that synthesizes acetylcholine, in the neocortex and hippocampus and neuronal atrophy in the nucleus basalis of Meynert, have been well established. Postmortem, histopathological studies of brain tissue of patients with AD and visualization via positron emission tomography (PET) in living subjects reveal reductions in acetylcholinesterase. Degeneration in specific subtypes of cholinergic receptors has also been identified. Although postsynaptic muscarinic receptors appear to be well preserved, a prominent deterioration in presynaptic muscarinic receptors as well as in certain presynaptic nicotinic cholinergic receptors is noted. Aside from its vital function in memory, cholinergic deficiency has been linked to the development of behavioral disruptions frequently seen in dementia. Sunderland et al. (1987) observed that when scopolamine, an anticholinergic agent, was given intravenously to AD patients, agitation and hostility ensued. Gorman and colleagues (1993) in a double-blind, crossover study comparing the cholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine to haloperidol, found that both agents significantly reduced agitation with approximately equivalent effects. Cholinergic receptor agonists, such as Xanomeline, have also been reported to reduce agitated behaviors (Bodick et. 1997). Research has also suggested that there is a “cholinergic component” of conscious awareness. The cholinergic system controls activities that depend on selective attention, which are an essential component of conscious awareness. This awareness can be altered by various pharmacological agents and by the disease process and neuronal loss itself. For example, drugs that antagonize muscarinic receptors induce hallucinations and reduce the level of consciousness, while the nicotinic receptors are implicated as being involved in the mechanism of action of general anesthetics. It is theorized that the excessive loss of cortical acetylcholine may allow “irrelevant” information to enter the conscious awareness and hence produce hallucinations. The role of the cholinergic system in impairment of reality testing is further supported by pharmacological studies indicating an increase in psychotic features with anticholinergic agents and reduction of psychosis with cholinergic agents. Psychosis is common in conditions such as anticholinergic delirium. Psychotic symptoms in this disorder are readily reversed by treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors or physostigmine.
138
Dementia
Sunderland et al. (1987) reported that scopolamine increased the incidence of thought disorders in AD patients. Bodick et al. (1997) observed that xanomeline decreased psychosis in AD patients. Cummings, Gorman, and Shapira (1993) also reported benefit in reduction of psychotic symptoms in AD patients after administration of physostigmine. Given evidence of the detrimental consequences of cholinergic deficiencies, use of medications that modify the cholinergic system may be of benefit. Several studies of cholinesterase inhibitors, typically used to treat cognitive impairment in dementia, reveal evidence of modest clinically relevant psychotropic effects in some patients with dementia. Fundamentally, these studies were not designed to address behavioral outcomes as the primary goal. However, they did evidence reduced behavioral symptoms, particularly mood disturbances and delusions, in patients with AD with relatively severe psychopathology. A review of 24 trials involving nearly 5,800 participants receiving donepezil found benefits of treatment noted on measures of activities of daily living and behavior. Galantamine, donepezil, and rivastigmine also demonstrated effects in people with mild, moderate or severe Alzheimer ’s dementia in 10 randomized doubleblind, placebo-controlled trials (Birks and Harvey 2006). No significant differences in efficacy were noted between study medications. Similarly, a modest benefit was revealed in a 2003 meta-analysis of 29 parallel-group or cross-over randomized, placebo-controlled trials of outpatients diagnosed with mild to moderate AD meant to quantify the efficacy of these cholinesterase inhibitors for neuropsychiatric and functional outcomes. No difference in efficacy among various cholinesterase inhibitors was observed (Trihn et al. 2003). A 2006 exploratory analysis of data pertaining to the efficacy of donepezil in severely behaviorally disturbed patients with AD suggested that the medication reduces behavioral symptoms, particularly mood disturbances and delusions, in patients with AD with relatively severe psychopathology (Cummings, McRae, and Zhang 2006). Unlike prior studies, which had involved low-level psychopathology, the authors focused on more severe BPSD manifestations, further substantiating the potential benefits of cholinesterase inhibitors at various stages of the disease. As previously mentioned, in neurodegenerative diseases such as AD, alterations in consciousness and symptom development are associated with regional deficits in the cholinergic system. Symptom formation may vary depending on the region of the brain affected, hence the diversity of clinical pathology seen in individuals diagnosed with dementia. In AD, hypoactivity of cholinergic projections to the hippocampus and cortex
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
139
results in loss of explicit memory, the memory responsible for conscious, intentional recollection of previous experiences and information. Symptoms of apathy and withdrawal appear to be related to functional impairments in the prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices (Craig et al. 1996). Agitation, aggression, and psychosis, which tend to be more prevalent during the more advanced stages of the disease, have been associated with hypometabolism present in the frontal and temporal lobes (Jeste et al. 1992; Sultzer et al. 1995). Cholinergic neuron degeneration in the nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM) results in circadian rhythm disturbance and “sundowning” in Alzheimer ’s disease. NBM neurons modulate the activity of the mainly cholinergic suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), the core circadian pacemaker in the body, and the induction of non-REM sleep (Klaffke and Staedt 2006). SCN dysregulation alters the core body temperature, heart rate, and hormone secretion. These changes may manifest as disturbed sleep and agitation associated with “sundowning.” Suprachiasmatic nucleus volume and cell number have been found to be decreased in those between the ages of 80 and 100 (Swaab, Fliers, and Partiman 1985). The NBM also carries many afferents from the limbic system and is therefore believed to play a role in communicating the emotional state of the organism to the cerebral cortex. Though many hypotheses regarding cholinergic effects on mood have been put forth, evidence remains inconsistent. Janowsky, Risch, and Gillin (1983) hypothesized that cholinergic disturbances were closely tied to dysregulation of other neurotransmitter systems related to affective states. Cholinergic predominance was viewed as resulting in depression, while cholinergic depletion and resulting adrenergic predominance were responsible for mania. This was further supported by several studies of cholinergic agents used to treat AD which were found to increase depressive symptoms in patients (Davis et al 1987; Sunderland, Tariot, and Newhouse 1988). However, recent research data from clinical trials reveal no depressant effects related to the medications, and in some cases mild euphoria has even been noted (Sunderland et al 1987; Bodick et al. 1997; Morris et al. 1998; Davis et al. 1992; Elgamal and MacQueen 2008; Holtzheimer et al. 2008). Overall, cholinesterase inhibitors appear to have no significant destabilizing effects on mood and may in fact be of modest benefit in improving symptoms such as apathy and withdrawal. In summary, CNS cholinergic deficits appear to play a role in the development of behavioral disturbances in some patients with AD as evidenced by pharmacological studies indicating an increase in psychotic features with anticholinergic agents and reduction of psychosis with cholinergic
140
Dementia
agents. Data suggests that pharmacological agents such as cholinesterase inhibitors may be of benefit in ameliorating these symptoms. However, further investigation is warranted. Norepinephrine Disruptions of the central noradrenergic system have been implicated as an important factor in the development of BPSD in AD. Norepinephrine (NA) is a primary neurotransmitter of the sympathetic nervous system, which is involved at a basal level in maintaining the body’s homeostasis, as well as mobilizing the body’s resources under stress to induce the “fightor-flight” response. Most of the CNS noradrenergic neurons are located in the locus coeruleus (LC) and are associated with cognitive function. The LC is also involved in regulation of levels of arousal, agitation, anxiety, sleep-wake cycle, levels of vigilance, emotional control, and aggression (Thoa et al. 1972; Geyer and Segal 1974; Torda 1976; Chan-Palay and Asan 1989; Peskind et al. 1995; Arnsten, Steere, and Hunt 1996; Anand and Charney 2000). Neurons originating in the ventral tegmental area, which have projections to the forebrain, are associated with sexual and feeding behaviors. Traumatic injuries to this area of the brain have been linked to violent behaviors, rage, and behavioral dyscontrol. Noradrenergic neurons project diffusely to various areas of the brain including the thalamus, hypothalamus, midbrain, and cerebellum and thus exert influence on numerous functions. Postmortem studies have consistently revealed decreased NA levels in the LC of patients with AD. Subcortical and cortical decreases in the thalamus, hypothalamus, hippocampus, amygdala, cingulate gyrus, frontal medial gyrus, temporal superior gyrus, putamen, and caudate have also been noted (Bondareff, Mountjoy, and Roth 1982.; Mann, Yates, and Marcyniuk 1984; Moll et al. 1990; Hoogendijk, Sommer et al. 1999; Matthews et al. 2002). These decreases in tissues may not necessarily correlate with functional noradrenergic transmission. The intraneuronal metabolite, MHPG, may be a better measure of actual noradrenergic function. In postmortem studies, despite decreased levels of NA, MHPG was frequently unchanged or high in patients with AD when compared to controls (Gottfries et al. 1983; Arai, Kosaka, and Iizuka 1984; Raskind et al. 1984; Francis et al. 1985; Herregodts et al. 1989; Nazarali and Reynolds 1992; Hoogendijk, Feenstra et al. 1999). This suggests a possible compensatory mechanism for LC NA loss. Remaining LC neurons may become overactivated to counterbalance noradrenergic losses (Hoogendijk et al. 1999).
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
141
Alzheimer ’s disease severity and noradrenergic neuronal losses appear to be linked. A study comparing 19 subjects with AD and 10 nondemented controls found a correlation between NA neuron counts in the LC and disease severity. The group of more severely impaired subjects was noted to have an 81% decrease in LC neurons when compared to nondemented subjects. The less severely impaired group was found to have a 20% decrease in LC neurons (Bondareff, Mountjoy, and Roth 1982). Because of its diffuse NA axonal dissemination, LC neuronal losses may influence a variety of bodily functions. The link between NA disruption and behavioral disturbances has been studied in a variety of psychiatric conditions including depression, mania, anxiety, aggression, and agitation. Decreased NA receptor sensitivity and increased NA turnover have been noted in conditions such as generalized anxiety disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder (Abelson et al. 1991; Geracioti et al. 2001; Nutt 2001). High levels of NA have been associated with aggressive behaviors in depressed and manic patients, patients with personality disorders, and healthy subjects. Indications that noradrenergic hyperactivity may correlate with increased agitation are further evidenced by elevated cerebrospinal fluid MHPG levels in AD patients with motor restlessness (Brane et al. 1989). A possible link between agitated and aggressive behavior and beta-adrenergic receptor binding was suggested by Russo-Neustadt and Cotman (1997). The authors found a modest, but significant increase in total beta-adrenergic receptor concentrations in the cerebella of aggressive patients with AD when compared to nonaggressive patients with AD and healthy controls, though tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactive innervation of the cerebella was preserved. Alpha2-receptors may also play a role in modulating aggression. A postmortem study of the hypothalamus, frontal cortex, and cerebellum of agitated patients with AD, nonagitated patients with AD, and elderly healthy control subjects demonstrated that levels of Alpha2receptors in the cerebellum of aggressive patients were 70% higher than their nonagitated counterparts (Russo-Neustadt and Cotman 1997). These levels were also elevated in the nonagitated subjects, but not significantly higher than levels in healthy controls. Frontal cortex and hypothalamic levels were not found to vary significantly. In vivo studies measuring cerebrospinal fluid NA levels were performed by Anden and colleagues to address a potential link between BPSD and NA. Yohimbine, a substance known to increase release of NA, was administered to subjects with AD, healthy elderly subjects, and younger subjects (Anden, Pauksens, and Svensson 1982; Peskind et al. 1998). AD patients displayed increased sensitivity to the agent when compared to the other
142
Dementia
groups. A substantial portion of participants with AD developed agitation, hyperarousal, and psychotic signs and symptoms. This may be a result of increased postsynaptic noradrenergic sensitivity in AD (Raskind et al. 1984; Peskind et al. 1995). Though psychosis may also be linked to noradrenergic dysregulation, evidence of such a correlate remains inconsistent. Higher NA concentrations have been found in the sustantia nigra of psychotic patients with AD by Zubenko et al. (1991). Förstl et al. (1994) also demonstrated significantly higher neuron counts in the parahippocampal gyrus and lower dorsal raphe nucleus neuron counts in AD patients experiencing auditory hallucinations when compared to nonpsychotic AD patients. At this point, evidence of a link between psychosis and the noradrenergic system remains unclear and further investigation is warranted. Depressive symptoms and apathy, which tend to predominate in the early stages of AD have also been associated with noradrenergic dysfunction. A reduction in NA cortical levels has been observed in depressed AD patients (Zubenko, Moossy, and Kopp 1990). Several studies revealed a significantly higher level of LC degeneration in depressed patients with dementia as opposed to their nondepressed counterparts (Zubenko et al. 1988; Zweig et al. 1988; Förstl et al. 1994). However, such evidence has been contradicted by Hoogendijk et al. (1999), who discovered no significant differences in LC degeneration between depressed and nondepressed AD patients. Given the possibility that reducing noradrenergic neurotransmission in the CNS may benefit agitated and aggressive behaviors, potential therapeutic interventions have been explored. However, β-blocking agents have not been subject to rigorous study. Most of the evidence comes from open trials conducted more than 10 years ago (Rosenquist, Tariot, and Loy 2000). Propranolol may be helpful specifically for aggression and uncooperativeness. Modest benefits were illustrated in a recent study evaluating the efficacy of the β-adrenergic antagonist propranolol for treatment-resistant disruptive behaviors and overall behavioral status in nursing home residents AD (Peskind et al. 2005). Short-term propranolol augmentation treatment appeared modestly effective and well tolerated for overall behavioral status in patients with disruptive behaviors. However, the usefulness of propranolol in this very old and frail population was limited by the high frequency of relative contraindications to β-blocker treatment. Prazosin, a nonsedating medication used for hypertension and benign prostatic hypertrophy, antagonizes NA effects at brain postsynaptic α-1 adrenoreceptors. A recent small, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel group study examined the efficacy and tolerability of prazosin for
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
143
behavioral symptoms in patients with agitation/aggression in AD (Wang et al. 2009). Those receiving prazosin displayed a significant improvement in behavioral symptoms as compared to the placebo group with no significant increase in adverse effects. These encouraging preliminary results require confirmation in larger controlled studies. Substantial evidence from neuropathological studies and pharmacotherapeutic studies suggests that the noradrenergic system plays a role in modulating behavior, particularly aggression and agitation, though a direct link requires further investigation. This provides an argument for additional comprehensive research into the clinical utility of noradrenergic agents in the pharmacological treatment of BPSD in AD. Serotonin Disruptions in serotonergic neurotransmission in dementia are also believed to result in the clinical manifestations of BPSD. The serotonergic system has widespread connections throughout the brain enabling it to regulate mood, feeding, aggression, sleep, temperature, sexual activity, and motor activity. Serotonergic deficits are well documented in AD and appear to be more pronounced in patients displaying behavioral disturbances (Zubenko, Moossy, and Kopp 1990). Loss of neurons in the brainstem serotonergic nuclei have consistently been reported (Mann and Yates 1983; Yamamoto and Hirano 1985). Serotonin (5-HT) is also well known to be responsible for symptom formation in various other clinical entities that display similar behavioral and psychological symptoms as those seen in dementia, such as depression, anxiety disorders, eating disorders, substance abuse, and psychotic disorders. This neurotransmitter has also been implicated in the pathophysiology of aggression and impulsivity in nondemented individuals (Greenberg and Coleman 1976). The relationship between specific behaviors and decreased serotonin levels in AD patients has been studied. Postmortem studies found that 5-HT levels in AD patients with psychosis were lower than those in AD patients without psychosis (Zubenko, Moossy, and Kopp 1990). However, another study looking at serotonin levels in the temporal cortex of psychotic versus nonpsychotic patients was unable to confirm these results (Lawlor et al. 1995). Serotonin has also been linked to nonpsychotic behavioral symptoms, such as agitation, anxiety, panic and depression in both clinical studies and postmortem analyses. Decreased cortical levels of 5-HT were found post mortem in patients with a history of aggression compared with nonagitated patients, whereas normal numbers of 5-HT2 receptors were found
144
Dementia
in nonaggressive patients. Recently, two neuroendocrine studies found an increased response to the 5-HT releasing agent fenfluramine in agitated aggressive patients when compared with nonagitated aggressive patients. Both neuroendocrine BPSD studies excluded AD patients with significant depression. One study also found a significant gender effect (Mintzer et al. 1998; Lanctôt et al. 1998, 1999). Gender had not been controlled for in other studies looking at the serotonergic system and may be an important contributor to variations in the serotonergic system throughout the life span. A positive correlation between CSF levels of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) was seen in patients exhibiting symptoms such as anxiety and panic. The loss of serotonergic neurons in the raphe nucleus and most major areas of the brain has also been associated with depressive features in AD (Zubenko 1992). Postmortem studies have similarly found reduced serotonin levels in the frontal and temporal cortex of depressed individuals with AD (Chen et al. 1996). Additional indirect evidence of the influence of the serotonergic system on behavioral symptoms has been gained from clinical manipulation of this neurotransmitter system through pharmacotherapy. A number of clinical trials have addressed the efficacy of serotonergic agents in AD patients exhibiting depressive or disruptive behaviors. For example, studies conducted by Nyth and Gottfries (1990) on citalopram, which among the SSRIs has the greatest in vitro selectivity ratio for the serotonergic versus the noradrenergic system, revealed positive evidence of the impact of serotonin on behavior. Citalopram was administered for four weeks to patients with mild to moderate AD or multi-infarct dementia in a multicenter, placebo-controlled, parallel group study. Significant improvements were noted in the AD patients on a geriatric rating scale in emotional bluntness and in all six BPSD (irritability, restlessness, anxiety, fear/panic, confusion, and depressed mood) for baseline versus citalopram scores. Irritability and depression improved significantly for citalopram versus placebo groups. There was no significant treatment effect in the multiinfarct group. The patients in this study had very mild BPSD at baseline, with mean scores of less than 2 out of 6 on each symptom. A similar positive correlation between pharmacological manipulation of the serotonergic system and BPSD in AD has been reported as a result of atypical antipsychotic use. Most of the new antipsychotics have strong antagonistic affinity for the 5-HT2 receptor. These agents have frequently been used off-label with good effect in AD patients with BPSD. Numerous studies have revealed significant efficacy of these agents on symptoms such as agitation, aggression, and psychosis (Burke and Tariot 2009). However, it is unclear whether this result is secondary to serotonergic or
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
145
dopaminergic antagonism, since the typical antipsychotics have also demonstrated a similar benefit. Though an effect of serotonin on behavior is apparent, the complexity of the serotonergic system in the brain, as well as its interdependence on other neurotransmitter systems, results in difficulty studying it in isolation. For instance, serotonin and acetylcholine interact extensively in the brain. Serotonin inhibits release of ACh from cortical and hippocampal cholinergic nerve terminals, possibly via 5-HT1B receptors in the hippocampus. The 5-HT3 receptors may also inhibit the release of ACh, whereas 5-HT1A receptors may mediate an increase in ACh release. Thus, disruptions in serotonin have the potential to influence an already impaired cholinergic system in individuals with AD. Similarly, an interplay between the noradrenergic and serotonergic systems is present in the human brain. Serotonin is a co-transmitter with NA, and uptake of 5-HT and NA can be accomplished by either 5-HT or NA neurons. The serotonergic system also inhibits the release of NA via 5-HT1 receptors, hence influencing behaviors such as the sleep-wake cycle, level of vigilance, and emotion. The loss of serotoninergic neurons affects the dopaminergic system since 5-HT neurons from the raphe nuclei are known to synapse with dopaminergic neurons and to control dopamine release in the midbrain, striatum, and nucleus accumbens. Serotonergic neurons may either inhibit (via 5-HT1A) or increase release of DA, influencing depression, agitation, and psychotic behaviors. GABA, the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in the brain, has been shown to regulate behaviors such as aggression. Increased GABA levels have been associated with decreased aggression. Serotonin acts as a co-transmitter with GABA, impacting its effect on such behaviors (Eichelman 1987). Gamma- aminobutyric Acid (GABA) GABA is the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter in the CNS. It is a local inhibitory interneuron for other neurotransmitters that are key in controlling behavior, including serotonin and dopamine. In addition to these local circuit actions, GABAergic neurons project from the striatum to the lateral globus pallidus and from the cerebellar inferior olivary nucleus to the vestibular nucleus. Consequently, GABA exerts additional influence on behavior through interactions with serotonin. GABA modulates behaviors such as fear, phobias, anxiety, and depression. It is thought to play a vital role in the development of psychiatric disorders such as anxiety disorders and depression, which share some
146
Dementia
common behavioral symptoms with AD. Evidence from postmortem studies, antemortem studies, neuroimaging studies, and markers of CNS GABA indicate a relationship between AD and GABAergic dysfunction. There are 22 studies examining GABA concentrations and GABA benzodiazepine binding in patients with AD (Lanctôt et al. 2004). The postmortem studies reveal a reduction of GABA levels throughout key cortical areas: frontal lobe (24–29%), temporal lobes (19–47%), and parietal lobes (21–47%). Binding studies using GABA or benzodiazepines show decreased binding in the temporal region. Limbic areas affected include amygdala (17–28%), thalamus (28–36%), and the cingulate (26–36%). The hippocampus, caudate, nucleus accumbens, and putamen appear to be unaffected. Neuroimaging studies, including positron emission tomography (PET) and single-photon emission tomography (SPECT), illustrate cortical and subcortical GABA deficits (Foster et al. 1987; Meyer et al. 1995; Wyper, Kelly, and Patterson 1998; Ohyama et al. 1999). In particular, the parietal cortex appears to be most affected. Investigation of markers of CNS GABA in the CSF and in plasma of patients with AD has yielded varied results. Of the 12 studies examining CSF GABA levels, only four found significant reductions (40–77%) (Enna et al. 1977; Manyam et al. 1980; Zimmer et al. 1984; Mohr et al. 1986). A single study of plasma GABA levels found a 51% decrease in AD patients (Jimenez-Jimenez et al. 1998). However, two other studies failed to detect any significant variation. Unfortunately, the link between GABAergic dysfunction and BPSD in AD remains unclear. Of the above mentioned studies, only two described BPSD. Procter et al. (1992) reported no significant differences in cortical GABA concentrations between AD patients and control subjects with or without behavioral symptoms. An imaging study conducted by Wyper, Kelly, and Patterson (1998) failed to show a correlation between behaviors and cortical GABA deficits. However, clinical experience supports GABA as a therapeutic target for treatment of BPSD in AD. Benzodiazepines, which specifically affect the GABA receptors, have been widely used to ameliorate behavioral disturbances in AD. Anticonvulsants, which are believed to directly and indirectly modulate GABAergic neurotransmission, have also shown promise in treatment of BPSD. However, many of the above-mentioned treatments are limited by potential toxicity and side-effect profiles. In summary, the role of GABA in AD requires further study. Currently, clinical research trials examining the influence of GABA specifically on behaviors have been limited in number. However, it is highly probable
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
147
that this neurotransmitter is directly or indirectly involved in the development of behavioral symptoms associated with dementia and that it may provide a target for future therapeutic intervention. Glutamate Approximately 70% of cortical neurons use glutamate (Glu) as a neurotransmitter. Hence it is the primary excitatory neurotransmitter in the cortex and hippocampus that is involved in many higher mental functions. A link between presynaptic and postsynaptic glutamatergic losses and cognitive dysfunction in AD has been well established. This neurotransmitter plays a pivotal role in learning and memory. Excessive glutamate levels in the brain can, however, lead to neurotoxicity which in turn may result in both cognitive and behavioral symptoms of dementia. Pertubations in glutamate homeostasis have been implicated in the development of both cognitive and behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer ’s dementia. Glutamate release and uptake are chronically decreased in AD leading to impaired neural activity in many cortical regions (Francis et al. 1993; Francis 2003). Neuropathologic studies have documented reduced levels of glutamate reuptake in the frontal and temporal cortices of patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Vesicular glutamate transporters (VGluT) have also been noted to be lost in various cortical regions. These alterations lead to inefficient removal of free glutamate from the synapse resulting in the presence of abnormally high synaptic glutamate levels under resting condition. Because the reduction in vesicular glutamate uptake causes less glutamate to be stored in the vesicles, neurons are left with fewer neurotransmitter molecules to release into the synaptic cleft in times of neuronal activity. This results in two major pathophysiological consequences. First, the presence of elevated neurotransmitter levels in the synapse under resting conditions creates a constant “background signal,” leading to chronic lowlevel activation of glutamatergic receptors on postsynaptic neurons and potential neurotoxicity. Second, because of this background signal, as well as the fact that neurons are left with smaller amounts of neurotransmitter to release into the synapse during neuronal firing, the difference between synaptic glutamate concentration during neuronal activity and synaptic glutamate concentration under resting conditions, referred to as the “peak signal,” is attenuated, leading to suboptimal neurotransmission as exemplified by a lack of long-term potentiation (LTP). Though glutamate exerts its effects through four types of receptors, this neural process relies heavily upon neurotransmission mediated by N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors and alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid
148
Dementia
(AMPA) receptors, as these receptors allow the influx of large amounts of stimulatory Ca2+ into the postsynaptic cell. If synaptic glutamate levels and intracellular calcium levels become significantly elevated, neurotoxicity and neurodegeneration ensue. The majority of evidence related to the effects of altered glutamate transmission and resulting neurotoxicity in psychosis stem from the glutamate hypothesis in illnesses such as schizophrenia. This hypothesis originated from the observed psychomimetic action of phencyclidine (PCP), a noncompetitive NMDA receptor antagonist. An interdependence between dopamine and glutamate in the pathophysiology of psychosis in schizophrenia and AD has been postulated. NMDA hypofunction in the prefrontal cortex may result in dysregulation of the dopamine system and this in turn affects the glutamate-mediated systems (Tamminga 1998; Laruelle et al. 2005). There is also evidence supporting the influence of glutamate neurotransmission in the cause and treatment response of depression (Paul and Skolnick 2003). As noted previously, many studies link serotonergic dysfunction to aggressive behaviors, although the postsynaptic location of most serotonergic receptors, including those positioned on glutamatergic pyramidal neurons, suggests a role for glutamate neurotransmission. Further indication as to the potential effect of altered glutamate neurotransmission on behavioral manifestations of AD is suggested by evidence from clinical trials of pharmaceuticals, such as memantine, which act on glutamate receptors. Memantine is a noncompetitive inhibitor of NMDA receptors that may permit normal memory formation but block their excitotoxic activation (Winblad, Mobius, and Stoffler 2002). A review of double-blind, parallel group, placebo-controlled, randomized trials of memantine in people with dementia conducted by McShane, Areosa Sastre, and Minakaran (2006) reported a slight decrease in development of agitation in patients taking memantine. This effect was slightly larger, but still small, in moderate to severe AD. There was no evidence about whether memantine had an effect on agitation which is already present. A significant advantage for memantine over placebo was also supported by a pooled analysis of 3 studies of patients with symptoms of aggression/agitation, delusions, and hallucinations (Wilcock et al. 2008) Although further study is indicated to establish the full impact of glutamatergic disruption in the human brain, initial evidence points to a probable link between behavioral and psychotic manifestations of AD and glutamate induced excitotoxicity. Additional research is currently underway to establish the significance of modification of non-NMDA receptors,
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
149
such as the AMPA receptors, as possible targets for new anti-agitation therapies. Neuropeptides Little is known about the possible clinical consequences of alterations in neuropeptide levels in the brains of patients with AD. Most neuropeptides, including vasoactive intestinal peptide and cholecystokinin appear to remain unchanged. However, others, such as beta-endorphin in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), galanin in the nucleus basalis, and substance P in the neocortex, are significantly altered. Somatostatin and its receptors, typically found in high concentrations in the hypothalamus, neocortex, and limbic system, have been shown to be reduced in the hippocampus, the frontal and temporal cotices, and the superior temporal gyrus of patients with AD in postmortem studies (Gabriel et al. 1996; Tamminga et al. 1987). Somatostatin is involved in the regulation of hormone release from the anterior pituitary and may act as a neurotransmitter-modulator. One of its roles also appears to be that of induction and maintenance of long-term potentiation in the hippocampus, which is vital for the formation of new memories. Though the consequences of somatostatin deficiency are not yet fully understood, decreased CSF somatostatin levels have been displayed in several psychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders. Neuropeptide Y, a potent anxiolytic, has also been implicated as a potential contributor to the development of behavioral symptoms in dementia. Neuropeptide Y and somatostatin coexist in the amygdala, basal ganglia, and cerebral cortex. Minthon et al. (1997) analyzed the CSF levels of somatostatin-like immunoreactivity and NPY-like immunoreactivity of patients with AD and with frontotemporal dementia (FTD). They correlated these levels to 54 different clinical items, such as restlessness, anxiety, irritability and depression. The CSF levels of somatostatin and neuropeptide Y were significantly correlated in FTD, a neurodegenerative disorder whose hallmark features are behavioral disturbances, but not in AD. Several significant correlations to the clinical signs were found; disorientation and dyspraxia in AD and agitation, irritability, and restlessness in FTD. Somatostatin showed a significant negative correlation with severity of dementia in AD (Minthon, Edvinsson, and Gustafson 1997). Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRF) immunoreactivity reductions, corresponding to interneuron losses in the neocortex, have also been consistently reported in AD. CRF reductions are not unique to AD and develop in numerous neurodegenerative processes and psychiatric illnesses. The
150
Dementia
role of CRF deficits in AD is not fully understood. Nevertheless, evidence from disorders such as depression and anxiety indicates a clear relevance of CRF homeostasis in the physiological and behavioral responses to stress (Todorovic et al. 2005). CRF release is stimulated by norepinephrine and acetylcholine, while its release is inhibited by GABA. Limited evidence from current studies and from analyses of other disorders manifesting with behavioral and psychotic symptoms suggests that neuropeptide alteration play a role in modulating behavior, particularly aggression and agitation, though a direct link requires further investigation. Dopamine Dopamine (DA) has long been implicated as a vital neurotransmitter affecting human behaviors, cognition, movement, motivation, mood, sleep, attention, reward/punishment, and memory. Excessive dopaminergic activity in the striatum has been viewed as a common final pathway for the development of psychotic symptoms in numerous psychiatric conditions, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and psychotic depression. Dopamine has also been implicated as playing a critical role in determining response rates in reward-related behaviors that are essential for effortful choices such as pleasure-seeking behaviors. Hence this neurotransmitter plays a role not only in psychosis, but also in affective states and addictions. However, the role of dopamine in the development of BPSD in AD remains unclear. Central dopaminergic dysfunction has been reported in a subgroup of AD patients. However, in the absence of Lewy body pathology, AD appears to be associated with only mild dopaminergic dysfunction. The delicate interplay between the cholinergic, serotonergic and dopaminergic systems within the corticostriatal neurocircuitry, have been hypothesized to lead to relative striatal hyperdopaminergia and hence to psychosis. Dopamine D1 receptors appear to be only modestly reduced in the neocortex and basal ganglia, while more consistent losses of D2 receptors are evident. In vivo studies in small numbers of patients with moderately severe AD have reported higher D2 receptor availability in association with wandering behavior and lower striatal D2 receptor availability in more behaviorally disturbed patients (Tanaka et al. 2003; Meguro et al. 1997). More recently, higher striatal DA transporter availability has been correlated with apathy (“loss of initiative”) in a larger sample that included eight patients with Lewy body dementia (David et al. 2008). Apathy is believed to represent a deficit in corticostriatal (ventral striatum, prefrontal cortex)
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
151
processing, characterized by a reduced ability to process emotionally significant stimuli. Despite some indication of dopaminergic involvement in disturbances of mood and behavior, a clear link cannot be established. Zubenko (1992) found that despite greater cytopathology in the substantia nigra of AD patients with major depression, dopamine levels were relatively intact when compared to AD patients without major depression. Decreased levels of homovanillic acid (HVA), a major dopamine metabolite, were associated with greater depression in some, but not all studies. Several postmortem and in vivo studies found a correlation between increased presynaptic dopamine levels and aggression in AD patients (Lopez et al. 1996).
SUMMARY Much remains to be learned about the neurochemistry of psychological and behavioral function in normal states and in dementia. Despite a rapidly developing understanding of the underlying neuropathology and neurochemistry, most evidence of explicit links between particular neurotransmitter abnormalities and a single behavior or behavior clusters remains inconsistent. This is likely related to the extensive interaction between many neurotransmitters and neuropeptides altered in dementia. Further, it is unclear whether changes in a particular neurotransmitter or neuropeptide are primary or secondary to changes in another. Adding to the confusion is the dynamic nature of BPSD. As opposed to static structural alterations, changes in behaviors are fluctuating and often intermittent in nature. They frequently involve biochemical and structural changes rather than static structural alterations. The study of the neurochemical etiology of BPSD presents many challenges. It is essential for future work to focus on identifying coexisting neuropeptide and neurotransmitter changes through the progression of the disease. Future research in this area must integrate neurobiological, neuroimaging, and clinical investigation. However, thorough study of BPSD clusters and their parallel neurochemical abnormalities may allow us to develop future patient-specific, evidence-based therapeutic interventions for dementia.
REFERENCES Aarsland D., K. Brønnick, U. Ehrt, P. P. De Deyn, S. Tekin, M. Emre, and J. L. Cummings. 2007. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with Parkinson’s
152
Dementia
disease and dementia: Frequency, profile and associated care giver stress. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 78 (1): 36–42. Abelson, J. L., D. Glitz, O. G. Cameron, et al. 1991. Blunted growth hormone response to clonidine in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48: 157–162. Anand, A., and D. S. Charney. 2000. Norepinephrine dysfunction in depression. J Clin Psychiatry 61 (Suppl. 10): 16–24. Anden, N. E., K. Pauksens, and K. Svensson. 1982. Selective blockade of brain alpha 2-autoreceptors by yohimbine: Effects on motor activity and on turnover of noradrenaline and dopamine. J Neural Transm 55: 111–120. Arai, H., K. Kosaka, and R. Iizuka. 1984. Changes of biogenic amines and their metabolites in postmortem brains from patients with Alzheimer-type dementia. J Neurochem 43: 388–393. Arnsten, A. F., J. C. Steere, and R. D. Hunt. 1996. The contribution of alpha 2noradrenergic mechanisms of prefrontal cortical cognitive function: Potential significance for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53: 448–455. Birks, J., and R. J. Harvey. 2006. Donepezil for dementia due to Alzheimer ’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 25 (1): CD001190. Review. Bodick, N. C., W. W. Offen, A. I. Levey, N. R. Cutler, S. G. Gauthier, A. Satlin, H. E. Shannon, et al. 1997. Effects of xanomeline, a selective muscarinic receptor agonist, on cognitive function and behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 54 (4): 465–473. Bondareff, W., C. Q. Mountjoy, and M. Roth. 1982. Loss of neurons of origin of the adrenergic projection to cerebral cortex (nucleus locus ceruleus) in senile dementia. Neurology 32: 164–168. Brane, G., C. G. Gottfries, K. Blennow, et al. 1989. Monoamine metabolites in cerebrospinal fluid and behavioral ratings in patients with early and late onset of Alzheimer dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 3: 148–156. Buhr, G. T., M. Kuchibhatla, and E. C. Clipp. 2006. Caregivers’ reasons for nursing home placement: Clues for improving discussions with families prior to the transition. Gerontologist 46 (1): 52–61. Burke, A. D., and P. N. Tariot. 2009. Atypical antipsychotics in the elderly: A review of therapeutic trends and clinical outcomes. Expert Opin Pharmacother 10 (15): 2407–2414. Review. Chan-Palay, V., and E. Asan. 1989. Alterations in catecholamine neurons of the locus coeruleus in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type and in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia and depression. J Comp Neurol 287: 373–392. Chen, CPL-H., J. T. Alder, D. M. Bowen, et al. 1996. Presynaptic serotonergic markers in community-acquired cases of Alzheimer ’s disease: Correlations with depression and neuroleptic medication. J Neurochem 66: 1592–1598. Craig, A. H., J. L. Cummings, L. Fairbanks, L. Itti, B. L. Miller, J. Li, and I. Mena. 1996. Cerebral blood flow correlates of apathy in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 53 (11): 1116–1120.
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
153
Cummings, J. L., D. G. Gorman, and J. Shapira. 1993. Physostigmine ameliorates the delusions of Alzheimer ’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 33 (7): 536–541. Cummings, J. L., T. McRae, and R. Zhang. 2006. Donepezil-sertraline study group: Effects of donepezil on neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with dementia and severe behavioral disorders. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 14 (7): 605–612. David, R., M. Koulibaly, M. Benoit, et al. 2008. Striatal dopamine transporter levels correlate with apathy in neurodegenerative diseases: A SPECT study with partial volume effect correction. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 110: 19–24. Davis, K. L., E. Hollander, M. Davidson, B. M. Davis, R. C. Mohs, and T. B. Horvath. 1987. Induction of depression with oxotremorine in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 144 (4): 468–471. Davis, K. L., L. J. Thal, E. R. Gamzu, C. S. Davis, R. F. Woolson, S. I. Gracon, D. A. Drachman, et al. 1992. A double-blind, placebo-controlled multicenter study of tacrine for Alzheimer ’s disease. The Tacrine Collaborative Study Group. N Engl J Med 327 (18): 1253–1259. Donaldson, C., N. Tarrier, and A. Burns. 1998. Determinants of carer stress in Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 13 (4): 248–256. Eichelman, B. 1987. Neurochemical basis of aggressive behavior. Psychiatric Annals 17: 371–374. Elgamal, S., and G. MacQueen. 2008. Galantamine as an adjunctive treatment in major depression. J Clin Psychopharmacol 28 (3): 357–359. Enna, S. J., L. Z. Stern, G. J. Wastek, and H. I. Yamamura. 1977. Cerebrospinal fluid gamma-aminobutyric acid variations in neurological disorders. Arch Neurol 34: 683–685. Förstl, H., A. Burns, R. Levy, et al. 1994. Neuropathological correlates of psychotic phenomena in confirmed Alzheimer ’s disease. Br J Psychiatry 165: 53–59. Foster, N. L., A. F. VanDerSpek, M. S. Aldrich, S. Berent, R. H. Hichwa, J. C. Sackellares, et al. 1987. The effect of diazepam sedation on cerebral glucose metabolism in Alzheimer ’s disease as measured using positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 7: 415–420. Francis, P. T. 2003. Glutamatergic systems in Alzheimer ’s disease. Int. J Geriatr Psychiatry 18 (Suppl. 1): S15–S21. Francis, P. T., A. M. Palmer, N. R. Sims, et al. 1985. Neurochemical studies of earlyonset Alzheimer ’s disease: Possible influence on treatment. N Engl J Med 313 (1): 7–11. Francis, P. T., N. R. Sims, A. W. Procter, and D. M. Bowen. 1993. Cortical pyramidal neurone loss may cause glutamatergic hypoactivity and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer ’s disease: Investigative and therapeutic perspectives. J. Neurochem 60 (5): 1589–1604. Fuh, J. L., C. K. Liu, M. S. Mega, S. J. Wang, and J. L. Cummings. 2001. Behavioral disorders and caregivers’ reaction in Taiwanese patients with Alzheimer ’s disease Int Psychogeriatr 13 (1): 121–128. Gabriel, S. M., M. Davidson, V. Haroutunian, P. Powchik, L. M. Bierer, D. P. Purohit, D. P. Perl, and K. L. Davis. 1996. Neuropeptide deficits in schizophrenia vs. Alzheimer ’s disease cerebral cortex. Biol Psychiatry 39 (2): 82–91.
154
Dementia
Geracioti, T. D., Jr., D. G. Baker, N. N. Ekhator, et al. 2001. CSF norepinephrine concentrations in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 158: 1227–1230. Geyer, M. A., and D. S. Segal. 1974. Shock-induced aggression: Opposite effects of intraventricularly infused dopamine and norepinephrine. Behav Biol 10: 99–104. Gorman, D. G., S. Read, and J. L. Cummings. 1993. Cholinergic therapy of behavioral disturbances in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neuropsychiatr Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 6: 229–234. Gottfries, C. G., R. Adolfsson, S. M. Aquilonius, et al. 1983. Biochemical changes in dementia disorders of Alzheimer type (AD/SDAT). Neurobiol Aging 4: 261–271. Greenberg, A. S., and M. Coleman. 1976. Depressed 5-hydroxyindole levels associated with hyperactive and aggressive behavior: Relationship to drug response. Arch Gen Psychiatry 33 (3): 331–336. Herregodts, P., M. Bruyland, De J. Keyser, et al. 1989. Monoaminergic neurotransmitters in Alzheimer ’s disease: An HPLC study comparing presenile familial and sporadic senile cases. J Neurol Sci 92: 101–116. Holtzheimer, P. E., III, T. W. Meeks, M. E. Kelley, M. Mufti, R. Young, K. McWhorter, N. Vito, et al. 2008. A double blind, placebo-controlled pilot study of galantamine augmentation of antidepressant treatment in older adults with major depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 23 (6): 625–631. Hoogendijk, W. J., M. G. Feenstra, M. H. Botterblom, et al. 1999. Increased activity of surviving locus ceruleus neurons in Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann Neurol 45: 82–91. Hoogendijk, W. J., I. E. Sommer, C. W. Pool, et al. 1999. Lack of association between depression and loss of neurons in the locus coeruleus in Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56: 45–51. Janowsky, D. S., S. C. Risch, and J. C. Gillin. 1983. Adrenergic-cholinergic balance and the treatment of affective disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 7 (2–3): 297–307. Jeste, D. V., R. E. Wragg, D. P. Salmon, M. J. Harris, and L. J. Thal. 1992. Cognitive deficits of patients with Alzheimer ’s disease with and without delusions. Am J Psychiatry 149 (2): 184–189. Jimenez-Jimenez, F. J., J. A. Molina, P. Gomez, C. Vargas, F. de Bustos, J. BenitoLeon, et al. 1998. Neurotransmitter amino acids in cerebrospinal fluid of patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neural Transm 105: 269–277. Kaufer, D. I., J. L. Cummings, D. Christine, T. Bray, S. Castellon, D. Masterman, A. MacMillan, P. Ketchel, and S. T. DeKosky. 1998. Assessing the impact of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory Caregiver Distress Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 46 (2): 210–215. Klaffke, S., and J. Staedt. 2006. Sundowning and circadian rhythm disorders in dementia. Acta Neurol Belg 106 (4): 168–175.
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
155
Lanctôt, K. L., N. Herrmann, G. Eryavec, et al. 1998. Central serotonergic function is related to agitated aggression in Alzheimer ’s disease. Clin Pharmacol Ther 63: 222. Lanctôt, K. L., N. Herrmann, G. Eryavec, et al. 1999. Gender variations in effect of sertraline on behavioral disorders (BD) in Alzheimer ’s disease (AD). Clin Pharmacol Ther 65: 190. Lanctôt, K. L., N. Herrmann, P. Mazzotta, L. R. Khan, and N. Ingber. 2004. GABAergic function in Alzheimer ’s disease: Evidence for dysfunction and potential as a therapeutic target for treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Can J Psychiatry 49: 439–453. Laruelle, M., W. G. Frankle, R. Narendran, L. S. Kegeles, and A. Abi-Dargham. 2005. Mechanism of action of antipsychotic drugs: From dopamine D(2) receptor antagonism to glutamate NMDA facilitation. Clin. Ther 27 (Suppl A): S16–S24. Lawlor, B. A., T. M. Ryan, L. M. Bierer, et al. 1995. Lack of association between clinical symptoms and postmortem indices of brain serotonin function in Alzheimer ’s disease (letter). Biol Psychiatry 37: 895–896. Lopez, O. L., D. Kaufer, C. T. Reiter, et al. 1996. Relationship between CSF neurotransmitter metabolites and aggressive behavior in Alzheimer ’s disease. Eur J Neurol 3: 153–155. Mann, D. M., and P. O. Yates. 1983. Serotonin nerve cells in Alzheimer ’s disease J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 46 (1): 96. Mann, D. M., P. O. Yates, and B. Marcyniuk. 1984. A comparison of changes in the nucleus basalis and locus caeruleus in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 47: 201–203. Manyam, N. V., L. Katz, T. A. Hare, J. C. Gerber, and M. H. Grossman. 1980. Levels of gamma-aminobutyric acid in cerebrospinal fluid in various neurologic disorders. Arch Neurol 37: 352–355. Matthews, K. L., C. P. Chen, M. M. Esiri, et al. 2002. Noradrenergic changes, aggressive behavior, and cognition in patients with dementia. Biol Psychiatry 51: 407–474. McShane, R., A. Areosa Sastre, and N. Minakaran. 2006. Memantine for dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 19 (2): CD003154. Review. Meguro, K., S. Yamaguchi, M. Itoh, T. Fujiwara, and A. Yamadori. 1997. Striatal dopamine metabolism correlated with frontotemporal glucose utilization in Alzheimer ’s disease: A double-tracer PET study. Neurology 49: 941–945. Meyer, M., R. A. Koeppe, K. A. Frey, N. L. Foster, and D. E. Kuhl. 1995. Positron emission tomography measures of benzodiazepine binding in Alzheimer ’s disease. Arch Neurol 52: 314–317. Minthon, L., L. Edvinsson, and L. Gustafson. 1997. Somatostatin and neuropeptide Y in cerebrospinal fluid: correlations with severity of disease and clinical signs in Alzheimer ’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 8 (4): 232–239.
156
Dementia
Mintzer, J., O. Mintzer-Brawman, D. F. Mirski, et al. 1998. Fenfluramine challenge test as a marker of serotonin activity in patients with Alzheimer ’s dementia and agitation. Biol Psychiatry 44: 918–921. Mohr, E., G. Bruno, N. Foster, M. Gillespie, C. Cox, T. A. Hare, et al. 1986. GABAagonist therapy for Alzheimer ’s disease. Clin Neuropharmacol 9: 257–263. Moll, G., W. Gsell, I. Wichart, et al. 1990. Cholinergic and monoaminergic neuromediator systems in DAT: Neuropathological and neurochemical findings. In Alzheimer ’s disease: Epidemiology, neuropathy, neurochemistry, and clinics, ed. K. Maurer, P. Riederer, and H. Beckmann, 235–243. New York: Springer Verlag. Morris, J. C., P. A. Cyrus, J. Orazem, J. Mas, F. Bieber, B. B. Ruzicka, and B. Gulanski. 1998. Metrifonate benefits cognitive, behavioral, and global function in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 50 (5): 1222–1230. Mourik, J. C., S. M. Rosso, M. F. Niermeijer, H. J. Duivenvoorden, J. C. Van Swieten, and A. Tibben. 2004. Frontotemporal dementia: Behavioral symptoms and caregiver distress. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 18 (3–4): 299–306. Nazarali, A. J., and G. P. Reynolds. 1992. Monoamine neurotransmitters and their metabolites in brain regions in Alzheimer ’s disease: A postmortem study. Cell Mol Neurobiol 12: 581–587. Nutt, D. J. 2001. Neurobiological mechanisms in generalized anxiety disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 62 (Suppl. 11): 22–27; discussion 28. Nyth, A. L., and C. G. Gottfries. 1990. The clinical efficacy of citalopram in treatment of emotional disturbances in dementia disorders. A Nordic multicentre study. Br J Psychiatry 157: 894–901. Ohyama, M., M. Senda, K. Ishiwata, S. Kitamura, M. Mishina, K. Ishii, et al. 1999. Preserved benzodiazepine receptors in Alzheimer ’s disease measured with C-11 flumazenil PET and I-123 iomazenil SPECT in comparison with CBF. Ann Nucl Med 13: 309–315. Owen, F., M. Poulter, J. L. Waddington, R. D. Mashal, and T. J. Crow. 1983. [3H] R05-4864 and [3H]flunitrazepam binding in kainate-lesioned rat striatum and in temporal cortex of brains from patients with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain Res 278: 373–375. Paul, I. A., and P. Skolnick. 2003. Glutamate and depression: Clinical and preclinical studies. Ann NY Acad Sci 1003: 250–272. Peskind, E. R., R. Elrod, D. J. Dobie, et al. 1998. Cerebrospinal fluid epinephrine in Alzheimer ’s disease and normal aging. Neuropsychopharmacology 19: 465–471. Peskind, E. R., D. W. Tsuang, L. T. Bonner, M. Pascualy, R. G. Riekse, M. B. Snowden, R. Thomas, and M. A. Raskind. 2005. Propranolol for disruptive behaviors in nursing home residents with probable or possible Alzheimer disease: A placebo-controlled study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 19 (1): 23–28. Peskind, E. R., D. Wingerson, S. Murray, et al. 1995. Effects of Alzheimer ’s disease and normal aging on cerebrospinal fluid norepinephrine responses to yohimbine and clonidine. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52: 774–782.
Pathophysiology of Behavioral and Psychological Disturbances
157
Procter, A. W., P. T. Francis, G. C. Stratmann, and D. M. Bowen. 1992. Serotonergic pathology is not widespread in Alzheimer patients without prominent aggressive symptoms. Neurochem Res 17: 917–922. Raskind, M. A., E. R. Peskind, J. B. Halter, et al. 1984. Norepinephrine and MHPG levels in CSF and plasma in Alzheimer ’s disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41: 343–346. Rosenquist, K., P. Tariot, and R. Loy. 2000. Treatments for behavioural and psychological symptoms in AD and other dementias. In Dementia, ed. J. O’Brien, 571–602. London: Edward Arnold. Russo-Neustadt, A., and C. W. Cotman. 1997. Adrenergic receptors in Alzheimer ’s disease brain: Selective increases in the cerebella of aggressive patients. J Neurosci 17: 5573–5580. Sultzer, D. L., M. E. Mahler, M. A. Mandelkern, J. L. Cummings, W. G. Van Gorp, C. H. Hinkin, and M. A. Berisford. 1995. The relationship between psychiatric symptoms and regional cortical metabolism in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 7 (4): 476–484. Sunderland, T., P. N. Tariot, R. M. Cohen, H. Weingartner, E. A. Mueller III, and D. L. Murphy. 1987. Anticholinergic sensitivity in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type and age-matched controls: A dose-response study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44 (5): 418–426. Sunderland, T., P. N. Tariot, and P. A. Newhouse. 1988. Differential responsivity of mood, behavior, and cognition to cholinergic agents in elderly neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Res 472 (4): 371–389. Review. Swaab, D. F., E. Fliers, and T. S. Partiman. 1985. The suprachiasmatic nucleus of the human brain in relation to sex, age and senile dementia. Brain Res 342: 37–44. Tamminga, C. A. 1998. Schizophrenia and glutamatergic transmission. Crit Rev Neurobiol 12 (1–2): 21–36. Tamminga, C. A., N. L. Foster, P. Fedio, E. D. Bird, and T. N. Chase. 1987. Alzheimer ’s disease: Low cerebral somatostatin levels correlate with impaired cognitive function and cortical metabolism. Neurology 37 (1): 161–165. Tanaka, Y., K. Meguro, S. Yamaguchi, et al. 2003. Decreased striatal D2 receptor density associated with severe behavioral abnormality in Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann Nucl Med 17: 567–573. Teri, L., P. Rabins, P. Whitehouse, et al. 1992. Management of behavior disturbances in Alzheimer ’s disease: Current knowledge and future directions Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 6: 77–88. Thoa, N. B., B. Eichelman, J. S. Richardson, et al. 1972. 6-Hydroxydopa depletion of brain norepinephrine and the function of aggressive behavior. Science 178: 75–77. Torda, C. 1976. Effects of catecholamines on behavior. J Neurosci Res 2: 193–202. Todorovic, C., O. Jahn, H. Tezval, C. Hippel, and J. Spiess. 2005. The role of CRF receptors in anxiety and depression: Implications of the novel CRF1 agonist cortagine. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 9 (8): 1323–1333.
158
Dementia
Trinh, N. H., J. Hoblyn, S. Mohanty, and K. Yaffe. 2003. Efficacy of cholinesterase inhibitors in the treatment of neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment in Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. JAMA 289 (2): 210–216. Wang, L. Y., J. B. Shofer, K. Rohde, K. L. Hart, D. J. Hoff, Y. H. McFall, M. A. Raskind, and E. R. Peskind. 2009. Prazosin for the treatment of behavioral symptoms in patients with Alzheimer disease with agitation and aggression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17 (9): 744–751. Wilcock, G. K., C. G. Ballard, J. A. Cooper, and H. Loft. 2008. Memantine for agitation/aggression and psychosis in moderately severe to severe Alzheimer ’s disease: A pooled analysis of 3 studies. J Clin Psychiatry 69 (3): 341–348. Winblad, B., H. J. Mobius, and A. Stoffler. 2002. Glutamate receptors as a target for Alzheimer ’s disease—are clinical results supporting the hope? J Neural Transm Suppl 62: 217–225. Wyper, D., C. Kelly, and J. Patterson. 1998. Single photon emission computed tomography in Alzheimer ’s disease: A review of cerebral perfusion and acetylcholine muscarinic receptor imaging and a novel study of the GABA/ benzodiazepine system. Inter J Geriatr Psychopharmacol 1: 126–133. Yamamoto, T., and A. Hirano. 1985. Nucleus raphe dorsalis in Alzheimer ’s disease: Neurofibrillary tangles and loss of large neurons. Ann Neurol 17 (6): 573–577. Zimmer, R., A. W. Teelken, W. B. Trieling, W. Weber, T. Weihmayr, and H. Lauter. 1984. Gamma-aminobutyric acid and homovanillic acid concentration in the CSF of patients with senile dementia of Alzheimer ’s type. Arch Neurol 41: 602–604. Zubenko, G. S. 1992. Biological correlates of clinical heterogeneity in primary dementia. Neuropsychopharmacology 6 (2): 77–93. Review. Zubenko, G. S., and J. Moossy. 1988. Major depression in primary dementia: Clinical and neuropathologic correlates. Arch Neurol 45: 1182–1186. Zubenko, G. S., J. Moossy, and U. Kopp. 1990. Neurochemical correlates of major depression in primary dementia. Arch Neurol 47: 209–214. Zubenko, G. S., J. Moossy, A. J. Martinez, et al. 1991. Neuropathologic and neurochemical correlates of psychosis in primary dementia. Arch Neurol 48: 619–624. Zuidema, S. U., M. M. van der Meer, G. A. Pennings, and R. T. Koopmans. 2006. Prevalence of behavioural problems in a group of demented nursing home patients. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr 37 (1): 19–24. Zweig, R. M., C. A. Ross, J. C. Hedreen, et al. 1988. The neuropathology of aminergic nuclei in Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann Neurol 24: 233–242.
Chapter 6
Dementia in Parkinson Disease: Current Concepts in Neuropathology, Neuroanatomy, and Neurochemistry Raymon Durso
The characterization of dementia in Parkinson disease has undergone over the past four decades a complex evolution parallel to advances in neurobehavioral cognitive sciences, neurochemistry, neuropathology, imaging, and genetics. It continues to be further defined with an ultimate goal of integrating information from these multiple disciplines and discovering unifying concepts that would significantly impact clinical treatment. This chapter will review current ideas concerning dementia in Parkinson disease along with relevant historical concepts that have shaped the field. It will focus on dopamine and acetylcholine as the major neurotransmitters mediating cognitive deficits in Parkinson disease. HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES REGARDING THE CLINICAL EXPRESSIONS OF DEMENTIA IN PARKINSON DISEASE While initial descriptions of Parkinson disease were focused solely on motor deficits (Parkinson 1817), it became evident with further observation especially in patients with long-term disease that cognitive dysfunction was an undeniable part of this disorder. The occurrence of dementia in the early literature had been highly variable ranging from
160
Dementia
approximately 10–40% of Parkinson patients (Marttila and Rinne 1976; Lieberman et al. 1979; Taylor, Saint-Cyr, and Lang 1985; Tison et al. 1995; Aarsland et al. 1996; Hobson and Meara 1999). Contributions to this variance were primarily a lack of a cohesive approach to define exactly what constitutes dementia in the disorder. An international task force to develop unifying definitions and criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson disease was undertaken in the recent past (Emre et al. 2007). It is a complicated undertaking as it requires the inclusion of multiple disciplines including neuropathology, neurochemistry, neuroanatomy, epidemiology, and neuroimaging all of which continue to advance and provide new sources of unique data. From a clinical point of view, a better clarity regarding the nature of dementia in Parkinson disease and other parkinsonian disorders came with theories that two types of dementia could occur in neurodegenerative disorders. These were coined “subcortical” dementia and “cortical” dementia (Albert, Feldman, and Willis 1974; Cummings 1986). The entity “subcortical” dementia was associated with Parkinson disease and was characterized by problems with attention and deficits in spatial working memory and cognitive planning similar to those seen in animals with prefrontal damage (Mishkin 1957; Shallice 1982). Descriptions of “subcortical dementia” were often ascribed to a slowness of thought or “bradyphrenia” that results in prolonged cognitive processing time most evident when tasks become more complex (Zimmerman et al. 1992; Cooper et al. 1994). “Cortical” dementia had Alzheimer disease as its prototype and centered on language abnormalities and the presence of apraxia and agnosia (Cummings 1986). While the terms “subcortical” and “cortical” dementia are now rarely used, they nonetheless have set a valuable framework to understand how cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson disease might be rationally organized. The best present-day correlate to “subcortical” dementia is executive cognitive dysfunction. Executive cognitive function represents a battery of intellectual functions centered on attention span, concentration, and memory. Inclusive in these functions is an assortment of complicated processes that are involved in decision-making such as anticipation, judgment, motivation, social/ethical appropriateness, and goal-directed behavior. A fundamental requirement of executive cognitive function involves the ability to store and hold information on-line so that it can be continuously accessed in order to plan and change strategies. These functions, as will be discussed later in this chapter, are believed to be mediated through dopaminergic pathways involving midbrain neurons in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area that are either directly or indirectly distributed to the prefrontal cortex. The concept of executive cognitive
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
161
function implies the requirement of networks to process information from multiple areas of brain as a necessary part of decision making. At the core of this processing lies “working memory,” which involves prefrontal dopaminergic neurons that have the capacity to maintain firing as a probable method of storing information when tasks are being planned and executed (Goldman-Rakic 1996). One can envision initiating an activity that requires a wide range of information to accomplish. For example, throwing an initial punch in a fight would not only require the gathering of visual, proprioceptive, and motor information but would also process more complicated data such as the ethics and social consequence of such an action. In addition, long-term memory information would be likely included (e.g., What happened when I previously performed this action?). The process of storing all information as it is gathered prior to actual output appears to be an important function of prefrontal neurons mediating working memory. The prefrontal area is ideally situated to receive information from the neocortex as well as the limbic system. A more complete gathering and maintaining of information in prefrontal areas prior to initiating a motor or cognitive task could be viewed as a neurochemical/ neurophysiological correlate for the concept of logic. That is, all input has been considered for a task and consequently the output has been a decision based on the processing of the most complete database possible. Conversely, an inability to employ a full database because either the prefrontal working memory cells cannot receive information from all parts of the brain or they cannot maintain a firing rate as information is gathered would result in an output that has not considered a full complement of potentially available information for planning. The result may be more impulsive appearing behavior that appears illogical. One of the best tests capable of demonstrating executive cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson disease and the role of “working memory” is the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. In this test “stimulus cards” with shapes of different colors, designs, and quantities are initially presented to the subject. Without informing the subject, the examiner then decides whether additional cards to be given to the subject will be matched to the stimulus cards by color, design, or quantity. The examiner tells the subject only whether the match of the new additional cards to the stimulus cards is right or wrong. Consequently, by trial and error, the subject learns how he or she should be matching the cards. The matching rules are subsequently changed (without informing the subject) and the subject must change strategy to learn and apply new rules. The test measures how long it takes the subject to learn the correct strategy a well as how long it takes to acquire the new strategy (“change sets”). Consequently, the Wisconsin
162
Dementia
Card Sorting Test gives information on how well the subject can plan and change strategies based on goal-directed behavior, all of which are fundamental skills related to executive function. It provides information on both impulsive and perseverative tendencies. It is easy to envision how tasks associated with the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test would depend heavily on maintaining activity in working memory cells since decisions are closely based on information gathered in previous successful matching attempts. In early Parkinson disease patients, deficits are seen in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test with a prominent feature being the tendency to perseverate; that is, patients find it difficult to learn new strategies when the task has changed (Canavan et al. 1989; Paolo et al. 1996). Clinically, behavioral perseveration is frequently manifested in some patients when they seem unable to change their actions even when such previous behavior has resulted in an unwanted outcome (e.g., persistent falling because of not using an available cane or walker, continuing to drive a car despite previous accidents). Others have also found problems in early Parkinson disease involving visual working memory with a sparing of verbal working memory (Bradley, Welch, and Dick 1989). Of interest is the observation that there appears to be preservation of visual working memory for shapes (Postle, Jonides, and Smith 1997; Owen et al. 1997). It is suggested that the visual working memory deficits become more evident in early Parkinson disease only when more complex executive function tasks are added to the testing paradigms such as set shifting (Owen 2004). Abnormalities in the Ravens Progressive Matrices and Tower of London Test also suggest that the greatest deficits are evident when tasks are changed and new strategies need to be used (Farina et al. 2000; Owen, James, and Leigh 1992). Perseveration of previous strategies and inconsistent performance on newly acquired strategies tend to predominate. The Ravens Progressive Matrices tests a subject’s ability to find patterns in an apparent chaos of visual scenes by having the subject complete the last image in a series. The Tower of London Test and its variations involve a test subject moving colored beads among different pegs on a pegboard in order to replicate the design of the examiner. Subjects are scored by the number of moves undertaken to replicate the examiner ’s design with the fewest moves resulting in the best score. In both tests subjects learn the strategy being employed, which is then changed, and the new strategy needs to be discovered and applied. While working memory deficits in visual spatial tasks have been especially noted in Parkinson disease, verbal memory tests using “working memory” have also proven problematic in Parkinson disease. There is no evidence for language deficits (i.e., aphasia) but rather apparent deficits in the executive function of planning speech. As expected and similar to
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
163
previously described visual spatial testing, deficits are most notable with greater complexity in the task. Testing that requires multiple simultaneous levels of processing results in the greatest deficits. Patients appear to be less able than control subjects to develop strategies that integrate and prioritize information to perform more complex memory tasks. For example, when interference is interjected into a memory testing paradigm deficits become more clearly evident (Taylor, Saint-Cyr, and Lang 1990; Cooper, Sagar, and Sullivan 1993). There would appear to be an inability to maintain information in “working memory” when extraneous information is being introduced. It is evident that executive cognitive dysfunction occurs early in Parkinson disease with the greatest involvement mediated through “working memory” in prefrontal areas. These executive deficits are more evident when tasks become more complicated and especially when new rule changes must be incorporated. The next section discusses the dopaminergic pathways believed to mediate these processes. THE NEUROANATOMICAL AND NEUROCHEMICAL BASIS OF EXECUTIVE COGNITIVE DYSFUNCTION IN EARLY PARKINSON DISEASE: ROLE OF DOPAMINE The nature of the earliest type of cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease as discussed earlier involves executive cognitive dysfunction. While the understanding of the neuroanatomy and neurochemistry of executive cognitive function continues to be investigated and better understood, current research has focused on two neuroanatomical pathways as most important to mediating executive cognitive function where dopaminergic transmission seems to play the major role. These involve (1) nigrostriatalthalamic-frontal cortex circuitry and (2) mesocortical pathways in the brain. Nigrostriatal-thalamic-cortex pathways have been recognized as a potential circuit for linking basal ganglia with cortical function (Alexander, Delong, and Strick 1986). The pathway linking nigra with prefrontal cortex initiates with the nigrostriatal tract which has classically been argued to arise predominantly from the zona compacta (A9) area of the substantia nigra. The long-excepted role of the nigrostriatal tract in mediating normal movement by supplying dopamine to the striatum has been expanded to include cognitive function in recent decades. The ventrolateral portion of the nigra innervates putamen and is most damaged in Parkinson disease. It is this part of the nigra that is believed to mediate the prominent motor dysfunction. Medioventral and dorsal
164
Dementia
areas of the nigra, however, connect to the caudate (Rinne et al. 1989). The caudate nucleus of the striatum has been of particular interest as a structure that plays a role in normal executive function. Some [18F]fluorodopa positron emission tomography (PET) studies in Parkinson disease have reported reduced dopaminergic activity in caudate nucleus that correlates with deficits in neuropsychological performance (Marie et al. 1999; Bruck et al. 2001). A most recent study using single photon emission tomography with [I-123] Iofluplane-CIT, a marker that binds to presynaptic dopamine transporter proteins and hence an indicator of integrity of the nigrostriatal tract, has found a positive correlation between tests of executive cognitive function and caudate uptake in both hemispheres (Nobili et al. 2010). The efferent pathways from striatum to frontal cortex mediated through thalamus represent an indirect effect of caudate dopamine levels on “working memory” neurons in prefrontal cortex. In addition, reciprocal connections between these frontal areas and parietal cortex (Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989) provide a neuroanatomical basis for understanding how information required for planning and managing complex tasks in space and time is manipulated. At the level of prefrontal cortex, there is consensus that two areas are most important in mediating executive cognition, the ventrolateral frontal cortex (areas 45 and 47) and the dorsolateral frontal cortex (areas 46 and 9) (Owen 2004). These areas likely differ in the way they process similar memory-related information with ventrolateral areas handling first order processing and dorsolateral frontal cortex further manipulating data in a manner that allows for retention and constant monitoring in order to develop and change strategies when undertaking a complicated dynamic task. The ventrolateral frontal cortex has well-described connections to posterior association areas in parietal and temporal lobes and appears to mediate more simple memory functions such as the intentional retrieval of data. It can also serve to activate working memory that allows for decision making and judgments but only when tasks are not complicated and contain few steps. The second prefrontal processing area for executive cognitive function, the dorsolateral frontal lobes (area 46 and 9), appear to mediate more complex memory processing. Connections between ventrolateral and dorsolateral frontal lobes provide a network for first order memory information handled in the ventrolateral frontal cortex to be transferred to dorsolateral frontal areas for further processing. This more complicated processing is the basis for the development of complex planning and strategy. The dorsolateral frontal cortex is likely fundamental in mediating change in
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
165
strategy based on incoming new information. This ability to “change sets” requires that earlier memories be temporarily stored and monitored in order to be compared to newer experiences representing change. Recognition of this change can then allow for a new strategy to be developed. Imaging data from PET marking cerebrovascular blood flow lends support for these suppositions. Specifically, in healthy normal volunteers who are asked to perform tasks involving short-term retention of five serially presented spatial tasks with no further ongoing manipulation, imaging changes representing increased cerebrovascular blood flow are noted in the right ventrolateral frontal cortex. However, when the task presented becomes more complicated and involves ongoing rule changes requiring continuous updating of information and resultant strategy changes, then mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex activity is noted along with bilateral ventrolateral frontal activation (Owen, Evans, and Petrides 1996). In Parkinson disease patients undergoing PET to examine cerebral blood flow, clear deficits in these frontolateral cortex subdivisions have been difficult to find (Owen et al. 1998). However, functional MRI testing comparing PD patients with mild executive cognitive deficits to cognitively intact PD subjects has supported deficits in caudate and frontolateral cortex in impaired subjects (Lewis et al. 2003). Furthermore, memory testing that required manipulation of presented data (i.e., that which is theorized to both require ventrolateral and dorsolateral frontal processing) showed a greater MRI abnormality in frontal regions than tasks involving more simple memory recall (Lewis et al. 2005). This is consistent with previously discussed neuropsychology research that emphasized cognitive deficits in early PD were in the realm of working memory function that involved more complex processing and manipulation of memory data. More recent functional MRI studies have emphasized that only some executive cognitive tasks, especially those associated with set-shifting, require caudate activity (Monchi et al. 2004, 2007). When such set-shifting tasks are analyzed with functional MRI, reductions in dorsolateral and ventrolateral frontal cortex along with expected diminished caudate activity are evident in Parkinson disease. However, when executive tasks not requiring set-shifting are employed (i.e., tasks not requiring caudate activation), increases in dorsolateral and ventrolateral frontal cortex activity are noted. In addition, posterior cortical areas are activated as well (Samuel et al. 1997; Cools et al. 2002, Monchi et al. 2007). It is believed that such a pattern of increased activation may represent an attempt to recruit more cortical activity in order to compensate for and circumvent existing deficits in Parkinson patients (Samuel et al. 1997; Dagher et al. 2001). These kinds of results have also been argued as evidence that additional systems other
166
Dementia
than nigrostriatal-thalamic-prefrontal circuitry (e.g., the mesocortical dopamine pathway) must exist to mediate executive cognitive function not requiring set-shifting. The second dopaminergic pathway theorized as important for executive cognitive function involves direct dopamine stimulation of the prefrontal cortex from mesocortical tracts arising predominantly from cell bodies in the ventral tegmental area of midbrain. This mesocortical tract supplies prefrontal and basal frontal cortex. Loss of dopaminergic neurons in the mesocortical tracts of Parkinson disease patients has been well described. Using a tyrosine hydroxylase enzyme marker that identifies catecholamine neurons, loss of dopamine cells in the ventral tegmental area of the midbrain was reported in Parkinson disease postmortem tissue (Javoy-Agid and Agid 1980). Similarly, autopsy examination of basal frontal cortex levels of dopamine and its metabolites in Parkinson patients who had not taken levodopa for at least four days prior to death also showed declines as compared to controls (Scatton et al. 1983). Dopamine loss in frontal areas (averaging 40%) tends to be more variable and less severe as compared to that in the striatum (> 80%) (Agid, Javoy-Agid, and Ruberg 1987). [18F]fluorodopa PET has demonstrated in Parkinson disease subjects reductions in frontal dopaminergic activity (presumed to represent mesocortical tracts) of 45% of controls (Rinne et al. 2000). In this same study the importance of both caudate and mesodopaminergic activity in mediating executive cognitive dysfunction was reported. Specifically, significant decreases in caudate [18F]fluorodopa uptake (and not frontal cortex) were correlated with slower performance on tasks requiring subjects to make multiple associations that were not intuitive. Other types of tasks examining executive function involving more simple immediate memory (digit span backwards, phonologic fluency, and paired word associations) were significantly correlated with frontal cortex uptake and not caudate activity. The greater the radioactive fluorodopa uptake in the frontal lobes (indicative of more intact direct mesocortical dopaminergic innervation), the better was the performance on these simpler tasks. Further results emphasizing the importance of direct mesocortical dopaminergic activity in activating dorsolateral frontal cortex after performance of a planning task was reported with positron emission tomography measuring cerebral blood flow (Cools et al. 2002). These authors indicated that in Parkinson patients evaluated both on levodopa medication and off levodopa medication for approximately 18 hours that levodopa-induced increases in cerebral blood flow were significantly diminished in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as a function of poor performance on the planning task. Such a correlation was not seen with basal ganglia blood flow, indicating that the finding was not mediated
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
167
through indirect nigrostriatal-thalamic-frontal pathways. There are also functional MRI studies that support an independent role of the mesocortical dopaminergic pathway in mediating executive cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson disease (Mattay et al. 2002). In examining early Parkinson disease subjects with functional MRI during a working memory task on levodopa and off levodopa for at least 12 hours, investigators found that increased activity in frontal lobe during the working memory task was most evident only when subjects were off levodopa. Under this “no levodopa” condition greater activity in frontal lobes was associated with a greater number of testing errors, suggesting that there was an attempt to recruit more frontal cortex in order to compensate for working memory deficits. They found no difference in frontal activity during working memory testing when patients were taking levodopa. They argued that if nigrostriatal-thalamic-frontal cortex pathways were important during this working memory task, frontal activity should have been increased after administration of levodopa similar to the increase they found in cortical motor regions. In summary, Parkinson disease, even early in the disease process, is associated with impaired cognition. The latter takes the form of executive cognitive dysfunction and is most evident in tasks that require the storing and monitoring of incoming information required to develop strategies of action. Parkinson disease patients appear most disabled in their ability to use such on-line processing in order to “change sets” or change strategies based on new conditions and rules. Their tendency is to perseverate on older strategies. It is believed that such defects come from abnormalities and deficiencies in dopamine processing both through the nigrostriatalthalamic-frontal cortex circuit and the mesocortical dopamine pathway. There is evidence that deficiencies in both of these systems are responsible for the executive cognitive dysfunction seen in Parkinson disease. What remains uncertain is the exact nature of how these different dopaminergic pathways serve specific aspects of working memory and how they integrate with each other to provide for “normal” function. Answers to these questions will be better elucidated as improved imaging techniques become available and better testing strategies evolve to examine potential underlying principles that comprise normal working memory. THE NEUROANATOMICAL AND NEUROCHEMICAL BASIS OF DEMENTIA IN PARKINSON DISEASE: ROLE OF ACETYLCHOLINE As previously discussed, even early in Parkinson disease there is evidence of cognitive impairment in the form of executive cognitive dysfunction.
168
Dementia
From a practical point of view this degree of impairment would likely go unnoticed by family and acquaintances of the affected patient. However, the development of severe dementia also is evident in Parkinson disease. Such dementia can develop into severe cognitive loss rivaling that of Alzheimer disease or dementia with Lewy bodies. Parkinson patients run a five times greater risk of developing dementia as compared to age-matched controls (Hobson and Meara 2004). The most recent reports citing incidence and prevalence of dementia in Parkinson disease remain, like their predecessors, highly variable. Hughes et al. (2000) found the prevalence of dementia in Parkinson disease to be to be 38% after 10 years in a cohort of 83 Parkinson patients without evidence of dementia at time of recruitment. Aarsland, Anderson, et al. (2003), using three different cognitive rating scales (MiniMental State Examination, the mental subscale of the UPDRS [United Parkinson Disease Rating Scale], and the Gottfries-Brane-Steen scale) found that the prevalence of dementia grew in their cohort of 224 Parkinson patients from an initial 26% to 78% after eight years. In a cohort of 86 Parkinson patients with no evidence of dementia, the prevalence of the problem after four years was found to be 35.3% as compared to 7% in control subjects (Hobson and Meara 2004). Finally, the incidence of dementia as measured with a Mini-Mental State Examination in 740 newly diagnosed patients participating in the DATATOP study was 5.8% after five years (Uc et al. 2009). The differences in prevalence among these studies mostly relate to the rigorousness of the diagnostic criteria employed and differences in the prevalence of dementia in the initial cohorts. The most important risk factors for developing dementia have been reported as severity of Parkinson motor signs (Marder et al. 1995; Hughes et al. 2000; Levy et al. 2002; Uc et al. 2009), duration of Parkinson symptoms (Hobson and Meara 2004), age (Hughes et al. 2000; Levy et al. 2002; Hobson and Meara 2004; Uc et al. 2009), and development of hallucinations (Aarsland, Anderson, et al. 2003; Hobson and Meara 2004: Uc et al. 2009). Cognitive function in Parkinson patients with dementia involves numerous deficits not seen in early Parkinson disease. In the latter, as previously discussed, executive cognitive dysfunction is subtle, involving primarily complex working memory tasks that require set changes and online monitoring of associations among incoming memory traces. In demented Parkinson patients, executive cognitive abnormalities become more profound. For example, a simple version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test examining visual working memory is abnormal in demented Parkinson disease patients and not in their nondemented counterparts (Paolo et al. 1996). In studies comparing demented Parkinson subjects with Alzheimer disease there is a tendency for demented Parkinson patients to have more severe
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
169
abnormalities in executive cognitive function such as attention and working memory than their Alzheimer counterparts (Litvan et al. 1991; Pillon et al. 1993; Aarsland, Litvan, et al. 2003; Noe et al. 2004). However, it is evident from these same reports that differences tend to blur as dementia severity worsens especially in tests of simple learning and immediate memory. Another tendency seen is that on tests of executive function and memory, Parkinson disease dementia more closely simulates dementia with Lewy bodies than Alzheimer disease (Litvan et al. 1991; Paolo et al. 1995; Aarsland, Litvan, et al. 2003). In addition to executive dysfunction, patients with Parkinson dementia appear to manifest more global deficits that would suggest involvement of additional cortical structures outside prefrontal areas. For example, abnormalities in visual spatial skills are also a feature of dementia in Parkinson disease. These same abnormalities are often described as a hallmark of Alzheimer disease and are presumed to represent early parietal cortical involvement. In Parkinson disease dementia, visual spatial and constructional skills are impaired as compared to normal subjects (Postle, Jonides, and Smith 1997; Farina et al. 2000). When compared to Alzheimer disease these defects are either equally or more severely involved (Paolo et al. 1995; Litvan et al. 1991; Cahn-Weiner et al. 2003; Cormack et al. 2004). In addition to dysexecutive problems, differences in the cognitive profile separating Parkinson disease dementia from Alzheimer disease has been an emphasis of memory dysfunction and language deficits in the latter (Cummings et al. 1988; Emre 2003). Imaging studies to be discussed later in this chapter give credence to the concept that larger areas of cortex including parietal cortex and hippocampus become involved when Parkinson patients become demented. This could explain these findings of significant visual-spatial dysfunction rivaling or even surpassing that seen in Alzheimer disease. It must be remembered, however, that visual spatial and drawing tasks often use both motor skills and complex processing that requires planning and strategy development. As previously discussed these abilities require prefrontal cortex and working memory. Therefore, the known severe executive dysfunction present in demented Parkinson patients almost assuredly plays a significant role in poor visual spatial function and constructional abnormalities even if a superimposed progressive cortical impairment exists. The findings of increased risk for dementia in Parkinson disease have raised the issue as to whether dementia is (1) part of the natural pathological course of Parkinson disease and (2) the natural progression of Parkinson disease involves the co-occurrence of other dementing illnesses notably dementia with Lewy bodies and/or Alzheimer disease.
170
Dementia
DEMENTIA AS A NATURAL PROGRESSION OF PARKINSON DISEASE: SUPPORT FROM IMAGING STUDIES The best support that dementia may be part of the natural course of Parkinson disease is derived from imaging studies. PET was used to image both dopaminergic integrity with [18F]fluorodopa and cholinergic integrity using N-[11C] –methl-4-piperidayl acetate (MP4A) in Parkinson disease patients with and without dementia as compared to normal controls (Hilker et al. 2005). Investigators found decreased striatal fluorodopa uptake in Parkinson patients with and without dementia as compared to controls. This was consistent with the presence of their motor disabilities. In Parkinson patients with dementia, however, cholinergic MP4A global cortical binding showed significant reductions of 29% versus controls while Parkinson patients without dementia demonstrated moderate reductions of 10.7% compared to controls. Most interestingly, patients with dementia had a significant correlation between reductions in striatal fluorodopa update and MP4A diminished cortical binding. These results led the authors to propose that Parkinson disease might represent a complex pathological process involving multiple linked neurotransmitters deficits. They speculated that with advancing disease cholinergic activity is increasingly lost resulting in more severe cognitive dysfunction. The observation that cholinergic loss is present in Parkinson disease patients with dementia is supported by other studies as well. The nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM) represents the major projecting cholinergic system from basal forebrain to amygdala and neocortex (Whitehouse et al. 1983; Perry et al. 1985). In demented patients with Parkinson disease, a selective loss of neurons within this nucleus has been reported (Whitehouse et al. 1983). Cortical concentrations of choline acetyltransferase, the synthetic enzyme for acetylcholine, represent an excellent marker of cholinergic neuronal integrity. This enzyme is located in cholinergic fibers projecting from cell bodies in the NBM. The finding of severe choline acetyltransferase reductions in temporal neocortex of demented Parkinson patients postmortem with the degree of loss correlating with severity of cognitive impairment further emphasizes the importance of this neurotransmitter loss in contributing to Parkinson disease dementia (Perry et al. 1985). Concentrations of acetylcholinesterase, the enzyme metabolizing acetylcholine, have also frequently been employed to evaluate the cholinergic system. In PET studies using the ligand 1-[11C]methylpiperidin-4-yl propionate that binds to this enzyme, an average reduction of 20% in cortical activity was seen in Parkinson patients with dementia as compared to 12.9% in nondemented Parkinson disease patients (Bohnen et al. 2003). Similar results were
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
171
reported using PET and N-[11C]-methyl-4-piperidyl acetate as a ligand for acetylcholinesterase (Shimada et al. 2009). In the latter study, this enzyme in cerebral cortex was found to be reduced 12% in Parkinson disease without dementia and 27% in Parkinson disease with dementia as compared to controls. All of these imaging studies indicate that cholinergic loss is seen early in Parkinson disease and becomes more severe when dementia develops. In Bohnen and colleagues’s study (2003) the reduction in cholinergic activity in Parkinson patients with dementia was even more severe than patients with mild Alzheimer disease. Cholinergic deficits contributing to cognitive loss in Parkinson disease with dementia is also supported by clinical pharmacology research demonstrating cognitive improvement in these patients (including executive function) after administration of rivastigmine, a drug that inhibits breakdown of acetylcholine (Emre et al. 2004). There is also an indication that interaction between acetylcholine and dopamine in the frontal and hippocampal cortex is required to accomplish some types of working memory tasks (Wisman et al. 2008). Specifically, a working memory task involving preservation of visual spatial memory acquired over several days remains unaffected in rats receiving ventral tegmental lesions to disrupt dopaminergic mesocortical tracts. In a similar fashion, the task also remains intact in animals given lesions of the NBM and/or septohippocampal projection to interrupt cholinergic input to the neocortex and hippocampus. While neither the dopaminergic nor cholinergic lesion alone will affect working memory performance, when both lesions are simultaneously undertaken (specifically those involving ventral tegmental and septohippocampal regions) diminished working memory performance is first noted. These imaging studies lend support for the concept that Parkinson disease may represent a progressive illness that initially involves executive cognitive dysfunction mediated through prefrontal dopaminergic pathology and then expands to involve more diffuse cortical structures (e.g., parietal and hippocampal anatomy) with increasing involvement of the cholinergic system. The basis for this expanding pathology remains unknown. The possibility that either dementia with Lewy bodies or Alzheimer disease are etiologically linked with Parkinson disease dementia is next discussed as possible pathological explanations for this expanding pathology. ARE PARKINSON DISEASE DEMENTIA AND DEMENTIA WITH LEWY BODIES ETIOLOGICALLY LINKED? The premise that Parkinson disease may progress to dementia with Lewy bodies derives from pathological studies in recent years that have
172
Dementia
emphasized that a common link may exist among different neurodegenerative illnesses centered on abnormalities in the protein synuclein. The Lewy body, a cytoplasmic inclusion in neurons and neurites, represents the pathological hallmark of Parkinson disease (Forno 1996). Synuclein is a major component of the Lewy body (Spillatini et al.1997). It is normally a soluble small protein occurring in cytoplasm with evidence of membrane association (Maroteaux and Scheller 1991; Jakes, Spillantini, and Goedert 1994). The pathological form of this protein appears to occur when it aggregates and becomes insoluble resulting in accumulation in neurons and glial cells in a filamentous configuration (Goedert and Spillantini 1998; Baba et al. 1998). Lewy bodies in Parkinson disease are typically concentrated in brainstem neurons, most notably in substantia nigra, locus coeruleus, dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus, and the NBM (Forno 1996). Dementia with Lewy bodies is characterized by an additional presence of Lewy bodies diffusely distributed throughout the cortex. Clinically, while some overlap may occur, the disorder is differentiated from Parkinson disease with dementia by an earlier emergence of dementia and hallucinations, more rapid clinical deterioration, and poorer response to levodopa (McKeith et al. 1996). The Braak hypothesis of staging idiopathic Parkinson disease has argued that Parkinson disease and dementia with Lewy bodies are etiologically linked through the Lewy body and synuclein (Braak et al. 2003, 2004) and represent a continuum of disease. They have proposed six stages of pathology in Parkinson disease that begin with Lewy body formation in brainstem nuclei including the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus and NBM (stage 1) progressing to involvement of substantia nigra (stages 3 and 4) and ending in a diffuse presence of Lewy bodies throughout the brain (stages 5 and 6) analogous to pathology seen in dementia with Lewy bodies. Multiple studies have found the presence of increased amounts of brain synuclein in various neurodegenerative disorders including Parkinson disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and multiple system atrophy (Wakabayashi et al. 1997, 1998; Spillantini et al. 1998, “Filamentos”; Spillantini et al. 1998, “Alpha-synuclein”). These reports involved qualitative staining techniques based on antibody immunoreactivity. For multiple system atrophy, in addition to neurons and neurites, staining was seen in glial cells. The term alpha-synucleinopathies was coined as a term to represent the concept that these orders were linked through synuclein (Goedert and Spillantini 1998; Galvin, Lee, and Trojanowski 2001). The association between synuclein and Parkinson disease was first established in two families with familial Parkinson disease (Polymeropoulos et al. 1997; Kruger et al. 1998). However, the link between synuclein and idiopathic
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
173
Parkinson disease with or without dementia continues to be a work in progress. In addition, still absent is the discovery of a distinct pathological mechanism by which synuclein would damage cells. Familial forms of Parkinson disease that are clearly associated with synuclein are extremely rare and other genetic markers such as the PARK genes some of which are associated with synuclein are not present in the vast majority of patients with sporadic occurring disease. The PARK genes tend to be found in young-onset Parkinson disease patients who have considerably less risk of dementia as compared to patients with typical age of onset and no evidence of genetic involvement (Khan et al. 2003; Lohmann et al. 2009). In addition when using more accurate quantitative techniques to measure synuclein, there appears to be a lack of consensus regarding abnormal increases in synuclein in the substantia nigra of “idiopathic” Parkinson disease. Changes reported range from large increases (Devi et al. 2008) to unchanged (Fuchs et al. 2008). A most recent study using a quantitative western blotting methodology did not find synuclein increases in the nigra, striatum, or frontal cortex of Parkinson patients with brainstem predominant Lewy body pathology nor were there any significant correlations between intensity of Lewy bodies and levels of synuclein (Tong et al. 2010). These authors did report marked increases of synuclein throughout the brain including the nigra in a patient with familial Parkinsonism–dementia. They also reported similar large increases of synuclein in the cortex and nigra of patients with multiple-system atrophy. The observed differences in Parkinson disease results as compared to previously cited studies were attributed to differences in technique (qualitative verses quantitative analysis) and the possibility that Parkinson specimens in other studies may have been contaminated with patients having dementia with Lewy bodies (i.e., studies were less rigorous in defining Parkinson disease as only having Lewy bodies isolated to brainstem). In addition, the Braak hypothesis, which has been used as a basis of support for linking Parkinson disease and dementia with Lewy bodies through a common synuclein pathology, has been challenged in recent years (Jellinger 2008). The presence of dementia in patients with early Braak stages, the absence of a caudo-rostral progression over time in young onset Parkinson patients, the lack of a correlation between Parkinson clinical severity and Braak staging, and sparing of caudal regions (medullary nuclei) in Parkinson post-mortem tissue demonstrating synuclein inclusions in the midbrain and cortex consistent with Braak stages 4 and 5 have all been cited deviations questioning validity. Consequently, the role of synuclein in pathologically linking Parkinson disease with other neurodegenerative disorders representing the synucleinopathies remains elusive.
174
Dementia
It would seem that Parkinson disease with dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies still remain as distinct clinical entities although it is clear that pathologically this remains an open question. In the future, better clarification of genetic risk in sporadic Parkinson disease may uncover more unifying genetic pathways that etiologically link Parkinson disease with other neurodegenerative disorders. To date, however, research attempting to determine if genes causing specific monogenic forms of familial Parkinson disease may also serve as susceptibility factors for sporadic Parkinson disease has not yielded clear results (Lesage and Brice 2009). Synuclein may serve as a marker for a common pathology still to be determined. It also still remains possible that synuclein changes may result as a secondary response to other suspected pathophysiologic mechanisms. In that regard, there is some evidence that dopamine and possibly related oxidative metabolism, a longstanding pathological mechanism proposed to underlie Parkinson cell death, can change soluble synuclein into the insoluble form proposed to be linked to neurodegenerative pathology (Kim and Kang 2005; Outeiro et al. 2009).
ARE PARKINSON DISEASE DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMER DISEASE PATHOLOGIES LINKED? Alzheimer disease neuropathology is characterized by the occurrence of neurofibrillary tangles and amyloid plaques in the brain whereby Parkinson disease is best differentiated pathologically from Alzheimer disease by the occurrence of Lewy bodies in brainstem nuclei. Clinically, at least early in the course of these two diseases, there are differences in cognitive deficits as previously discussed in this chapter that involve abnormalities in executive function in Parkinson disease and more predominant memory and language deficits, agnosia, and apraxias in Alzheimer disease. It should be emphasized, however, that any such differences tend to vanish when the dementias of both disorders progress. When comparing postmortem brains of Parkinson patients with and without dementia, a higher burden of Alzheimer cortical changes are seen in the demented Parkinson disease (Werner and Jellinger 1991: Selikhova et al. 2009). However, it is also noted that the presence of Lewy bodies are a better predictor of dementia than the occurrence of neurofibrillary tangles or plaques (Hurtig et al. 2000; Selikhova et al. 2009). In patients with dementia, when Lewy body and neurofibrillary tangle/amyloid plaque pathologies overlapped faster progression of dementia was noted as compared to patients with only Alzheimer or Lewy body pathology (Kraybill
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
175
et al. 2005). This would suggest an increased cognitive burden when both pathologies co-occur. With regard to genetics, there appear to be only weak links tying Alzheimer disease to Parkinson disease with dementia. Siblings of patients with Parkinson disease and dementia as compared to siblings of normal controls were found to have a three times greater risk of developing Alzheimer disease. No increased risk of developing Alzheimer disease was found among the parents of these demented patients (Marder et al. 1999). The apolipoprotein E 4 genotype that provides an increased risk for developing Alzheimer disease is not associated with risk for dementia in Parkinson disease (Huang, Chen, and Poole 2004; Kurz et al. 2009). At the present time there does not seem to be enough supportive data to link the pathology of Parkinson disease and Alzheimer disease. It seems reasonable to still treat these two disorders as distinct clinical entities. It is possible that they may co-occur in some individuals leading to even greater cognitive decline than either disorder alone. REFERENCES Aarsland, D., K. Anderson, J. P. Larsen, A. Lolk, and P. Kragh-Sorensen. 2003a. Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology 60: 387–392. Aarsland, D., I. Litvan, D. Galasko, T. Wentzel-Larsen, and J. P. Larsen. 2003b. Performance on the dementia rating scale in Parkinson’s disease with dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies: Comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 74: 1215–1220. Aarsland, D., E. Tandberg, P. J. Larsen, and J. L. Cummings. 1996. Frequency of dementia in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology 53: 538–542. Agid, Y., E. Javoy-Agid, and M. Ruberg. 1987. Biochemistry of neurotransmitters in Parkinson’s disease. In Movement disorders, ed. C. D. Marsden and S. Fahn, 166–230. London: Butterworth. Albert, M. L., R. G. Feldman, and A. L. Willis. 1974. The subcortical dementia of progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 37: 121–130. Alexander, G. E., M. R. Delong, and P. L. Strick. 1986. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 9: 357–381. Baba, M., S. Nakajo, P.-H. Tu, T. Tomita, K. Nakaya, V. M.-Y. Lee, J. Q. Trojanowski, and T. Iwatsubo. 1998. Aggregation of alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies of sporadic Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. American Journal of Pathology 152: 879–884.
176
Dementia
Bohnen, N. I., D. I. Kaufer, L. S. Ivanco, B. Lopresti, R. A. Koeppe, J. G. Davis, C. A. Mathis, R. Y. Moore, and S. T. DeKosky. 2003. Cortical cholinergic function is more severely affected in parkinsonian dementia than in Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology 60: 1745–1748. Braak, H., K. Del Tredici, U. Rub, R. A. de Vos, E. N. Jansen Steur, and E. Braak. 2003. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging 24: 197–211. Braak, H., E. Ghebremedhin, U. Rub, H. Bratzke, and K. Del Tredici. 2004. Stages in the development of Parkinson’s disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Research 318: 121–134. Bradley, V. A., J. L. Welch, and D. J. Dick. 1989. Visuospatial working memory in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 52: 1228–1235. Bruck, A., R. Portin, A. Lindell, A. Laihinen, J. Bergman, M. Haaparanta, O. Solin, and J. O. Rinne. 2001. Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in Parkinson disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus. Neuroscience Letters 311: 81–84. Cahn-Weiner, D. A., K. Williams, J. Grace, G. Tremont, H. Westervelt, and R. Stern. 2003. Discrimination of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease using the clock drawing test. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 16: 85–92. Canavan, A. G. M., R. E. Passingham, C. D. Marsden, N. Quinn, M. Wyke, and C. E. Polkey. 1989. The performance on learning tasks of patients in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 27: 141–156. Cavada, C., and P. S. Goldman-Rakic. 1989. Posterior parietal cortex on rhesus monkey. II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe. Journal of Comparative Neurology 287: 422–445. Cools, R., E. Stefanova, R. A. Barker, T. W. Robins, and A. M. Owen. 2002. Dopaminergic modulation of high-level cognition in Parkinson’s disease: The role of the prefrontal cortex revealed by PET. Brain 125: 584–594. Cooper, J. A., H. J. Sagar, and E. V. Sullivan. 1993. Short-term memory and temporal ordering in early Parkinson’s disease: Effects of disease chronicity and medication. Neuropsychologia 31: 933–949. Cooper, J. A., H. J. Sagar, P. Tidswell, and N. Jorda. 1994. Slowed central processing in simple and go/no-go reaction time tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 117: 517–529. Cormack, F., D. Aarsland, C. Ballard, and M. J. Tovee. 2004. Pentagon drawing and neuropsychological performance in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer ’s disease, Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 19: 371–377. Cummings, J. L. 1986. Subcortical dementia: Neuropsychology, neuropsychiatry and pathophysiology. British Journal of Psychiatry 149: 682–697.
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
177
Cummings, J. L., A. Darkins, M. Mendez, M. A. Hill, and D. F. Benson. 1988. Alzheimer ’s disease and Parkinson’s disease: Comparison of speech and language alterations. Neurology 38: 680–684. Dagher, A., A. M. Owen, H. Boecker, and D. J. Brooks. 2001. The role of the striatum and hippocampus in planning: A PET activation study in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 124: 1020–1032. Devi, L., V. Raghavendran, B. M. Prabhu, N. G. Avadhani, and H. K. Anandatheerthavarada. 2008. Mitochondrial import and accumulation of alpha-synuclein impair complex I in human dopaminergic neuronal cultures and Parkinson disease brain. Journal of Biological Chemistry 283: 9089–9100. Emre, M. 2003. Dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurology 2: 229–237. Emre, M., D. Aarsland, A. Albanese, E. J. Byrne, G. Deuschl, P. P. De Deyn, F. Duriff, et al. 2004. Rivastigmine for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. New England Journal of Medicine 351: 2509–2518. Emre, M., D. Aarsland, R. Brown, D. J. Burn, C. Duyckaerts, Y. Mizuno, G. A. Broe, et al. 2007. Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 22: 1689–1707. Farina, E., G. Gattellaro, S. Pornati, E. Magni, A. Perretti, A. P. Cannata, P. Nichelli, and C. Mariani. 2000. Researching a differential impairment of frontal functions and explicit memory in early Parkinson’s disease. European Journal of Neurology 7: 259–267. Forno, L. S. 1996. Neuropathology of Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 55: 259–272. Fuchs, J., A. Tichopad, Y. Golub, M. Munz, K. J. Schweitzer, B. Wolf, D. Berg, J. C. Mueller, and T. Gasser. 2008. Genetic variability in the SNCA gene influences alpha-synuclein levels in the blood and brain. FASEB Journal 22: 1327–1334. Galvin, J. E., V. M. Lee, and J. Q. Trojanowski. 2001. Synucleinopathies: Clinical and pathological implications. Archives of Neurology 58: 186–190. Goedert, M., and M. G. Spillantini. 1998. Lewy body diseases and multiple system atrophy as alpha-synucleinopathies. Molecular Psychiatry 3: 462–465. Goldman-Rakic, P. S. 1996. The prefrontal landscape: Implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 351: 1445–1453. Hilker, R., A. V. Thomas, J. C. Klein, S. Weisenbach, E. Kalbe, S. Burghaus, A. H. Jacobs, K. Herholz, and W. D. Heiss. 2005. Dementia in Parkinson disease: Functional imaging of cholinergic and dopaminergic pathways. Neurology 65: 1716–1722. Hobson, P., and J. Meara. 1999. The detection of dementia and cognitive impairment in a community population of elderly people with Parkinson’s disease by use of the CAMCOG neuropsychological test. Age and Ageing 28: 39–43.
178
Dementia
Hobson, P., and J. Meara. 2004. Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of older subjects with Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. Movement Disorders 19: 1043–1049. Huang, X., P. C. Chen, and C. Poole. 2004. APOE-E2 allele associated with higher prevalence of sporadic Parkinson disease. Neurology 62: 2198–2202. Hughes, T. A., H. F. Ross, S. Musa, S. Bhattacherjee, R. H. Nathan, and H. S. Mindham. 2000. A 10-year study of the incidence of and factors predicting dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 54: 1596–1603. Hurtig, H. I., J. Q. Trojanowski, J. Galvin, D. Ewbank, M. L. Schmidt, V. M.-Y. Lee, C. M. Clark, et al. 2000. Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate with dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 54: 1916–1921. Jakes, R., M. G. Splillantini, and M. Goedert. 1994. Identification of two distinct synucleins from human brain. FEBS Letters 345: 27–32. Javoy-Agid, F., and Y. Agid. 1980. Is the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in Parkinson disease? Neurology 30: 1326–1330. Jellinger, K. A. 2008. A critical reappraisal of current staging of Lewy-realted pathology in human brain. Acta Neuropathologica 116: 1–16. Khan, N.L., E. Graham, P. Critchley, A. E. Schrag, W. Wood, A. J. Lees, K. P. Bhatia, and N. Quail. 2003. Parkin disease: A phenotypic study of a large case series. Brain 126: 1279–1292. Kim, S. K., and J. H. Kang. 2005. Aggregation of alpha-synuclein induced by oxidized catecholamines as a potential mechanism of Lewy body. Bulletin of the Korean Chemical Society 26: 1255–1259. Kraybill, M. L., E. B. Larsen, D. W. Tsuang, L. Teri, W. C. McCormick, J. D. Bowen, W. A. Kukull, J. B. Leverenz, and M. M. Cherrier. 2005. Cognitive differences in dementia patients with autopsy-verifies AD, Lewy body pathology, or both. Neurology 64: 2069–2073. Kruger, R., W. Kuhn, T. Muller, D. Woitalla, M. Graeber, S. Kosel, H. Przuntek, J. T. Epplen, L. Schols, and O. Riess. 1998. Ala30Pro mutation in the gene encoding alpha-synuclein in Parkinson’s disease. Nature Genetics 18: 106–108. Kurz, M. W., G. Dekomien, O. B. Nilsen, J. P. Larsen, D. Aarsland, and G. Alves. 2009. APOE alleles in Parkinson disease and their relationship to cognitive decline: A population-based, longitudinal study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 22: 166–170. Lesage, S., and A. Brice. 2009. Parkinson’s disease: From monogenic forms to genetic susceptibility factors. Human Molecular Genetics 18: 48–59. Levy, G., N. Schupf, M. X. Tang, L. J. Cote, E. D. Louis, H. Meija, Y. Stern, and K. Marder. 2002. Combined effect of age and severity on the risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology 51: 722–729. Lewis, S. J. G., A. Dove, T. W. Robbins, R. A. Barker, and A. M. Owen. 2003. Cognitive impairments in early Parkinson’s disease are accompanied by reductions in activity in frontostriatal neural circuitry. Journal of Neuroscience 23: 6351–6356.
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
179
Lewis, S. J. G., A. Slabosz, T. W. Robbins, R. A. Barker, and A. M. Owen. 2005. Dopaminergic basis for deficits in working memory but not in attentional set-shifting in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 43: 823–832. Lieberman, A., M. Dziatolowski, M. Kupersmith, M. Serby, A. Goodgold, J. Korein, and M. Goldstein. 1979. Dementia in Parkinson disease. Annals of Neurology 6: 355–359. Litvan, I., E. Mohr, J. Williams, C. Gomez, and T. N. Chase. 1991. Differential memory and executive functions in demented patients with Parkinson’s and Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 54: 25–29. Lohmann, E., S. Thobois, S. Lesage, E. Broussolle, S. Tezenas du Montcel, M.-J. Ribeiro, P. Remy, et al. 2009. A multidisciplinary study of patients with early-onset PD with and without parkin mutations. Neurology 72: 110–116. Marder, K., M.-X. Tang, B. Alfaro, H. Mejia, L. Cote, E. Louis, Y. Stern, and R. Mayeux. 1999. Risk of Alzheimer ’s disease in relatives of Parkinson’s disease patients with and without dementia. Neurology 52: 719–725. Marder, K., M.-X. Tang, L. Cote, Y. Stern, and R. Mayeux. 1995. The frequency and associated risk factors for dementia in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Archives of Neurology 52: 695–701. Marie, R. M., L. Barre, B. Dupuy, F. Viader, G. Gefer, and J. C. Baron. 1999. Relationships between striatal dopamine denervation and frontal executive tests in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience Letters 260: 77–80. Maroteaux, L., and R. H. Scheller. 1991. The rat brain synucleins; A family of proteins transiently associated with neuronal membrane. Brain Research: Molecular Brain Research 11: 335–343. Marttila, R. J., and U. K. Rinne. 1976. Dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurological Scandinavica 54: 431–441. Mattay, V. S., A. Tessitore, J. H. Callicott, A. Bertolino, T. E. Goldberg, T. N. Chase, T. M. Hyde, and D. R. Weinberger. 2002. Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology 51: 156–164. McKeith, I. G., D. Galasko, K. Kosaka, E. K. Perry, D. W. Dickson, L. A. Hansen, D. P. Salmon, et al. 1996. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. Neurology 47: 1113–1124. Mishkin, M. 1957. Effects of small frontal lesions of delayed alternation in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 20: 615–622. Monchi, O., M. Petrides, J. Doyon, R. Postuma, K. Worsley, and A. Dagher. 2004. Neural bases of set-shifting deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience 24: 702–710. Monchi, O., M. Petrides, B. Mejaia-Constain, and A. P. Strafella. 2007. Cortical activity in Parkinson’s disease during executive processing depends on striatal involvement. Brain 130: 233–244.
180
Dementia
Nobili, F., C. Campus, D. Arnaldi, F. De Carli, G. Cabassi, A. Brugnolo, B. Dessi, et al. 2010. Cognitive-nigrostriatal relationships in de novo, drug-naïve Parkinson’s disease patients: A [I-123]FP-CIT SPECT study. Movement Disorders 25: 35–43. Noe, E., K. Marder, K. L. Bell, D. M. Jacobs, J. J. Manly, and Y. Stern. 2004. Comparison of dementia with Lewy bodies to Alzheimer ’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Movement Disorders 19: 60–67. Outeiro, T. F., K. Klucken, K. Bercury, J. Tetzlaff, P. Putcha, L. M. A. Oliveira, A. Quintas, P. J. McLean, and B. T. Hyman. 2009. Dopamine-induced conformational changes in alpha-synuclein. PLoS One 4: 1–11. Owen, A. M. 2004. Cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: The role of frontostriatal circuitry. Neuroscientist 10: 525–537. Owen, A. M., J. Doyon, A. Dagher, A. Sadikot, and A. C. Evans. 1998. Abnormal basal ganglia outflow in Parkinson’s disease identified with PET: Implications for higher cortical functions. Brain 121: 949–965. Owen, A. M., A. C. Evans, and M. Petrides. 1996. Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: A positron emission tomography study. Cerebral Cortex 6: 31–38. Owen, A. M., J. L. Iddon, J. R. Hodges, B. A. Summers, and T. W. Robbins. 1997. Spatial and non-spatial working memory at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 35: 519–532. Owen, A. M., M. James, and P. N. Leigh. 1992. Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain 15: 1727–1751. Paolo, A. M., A. I. Troster, K. T. Blackwell, W. C. Koller, and B. N. Axelrod. 1996. Utility of a Wisconsin card sorting test short form in persons with Alzheimer ’s and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 8: 892–897. Paolo, A. M., A. I. Troster, S. C. Glatt, J. P. Hubble, and W. C. Koller. 1995. Differentiation of the dementias of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease with the dementia rating scale. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 8 (3): 184–188. Parkinson, J. 1817. An essay on the shaking palsy. London: Sherwood, Neely and Jones. Perry, E. K., M. Curtis, D. J. Dick, J. M. Candy, J. R. Atack, C. A. Bloxham, G. Blessed, A. Fairbairn, B. E. Tomlinson, and R. Perry. 1985. Cholinergic correlates of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Comparisons with Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 48: 413–421. Pillon, B., B. Deweer, Y. Agid, and B. Dubois. 1993. Explicit memory in Alzheimer ’s, Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Archives of Neurology 50: 374–379. Polymeropoulos, M. H., C. Lavedan, E. Leroy, S. E. Ide, A. Dehejia, A. Dutra, B. Pike, et al. 1997. Mutation in the alpha-synuclein gene identified in families with Parkinson’s disease. Science 276: 2045–2047.
Dementia in Parkinson Disease
181
Postle, B. R., J. Jonides, and E. E. Smith. 1997. Spatial, but not object, delayed response is impaired in early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 11: 171–179. Rinne, J. O., R. Portin, H. Ruottinen, E. Nurmi, J. Bergman, M. Haaparanta, and O. Solin. 2000. Cognitive impairment and the brain dopaminergic system in Parkinson disease: [18F]fluorodopa positron emission tomographic study. Archives of Neurology 57: 470–475. Rinne, J. O., J. Rummukainen, L. Paljarvi, and U. K. Rinne. 1989. Dementia in Parkinson’s disease is related to neuronal loss in the medial substantia nigra. Annals of Neurology 26: 47–50. Samuel, M., A. O. Ceballos-Baumann, J. Blin, T. Uema, H. Boecker, R. E. Passingham, and D. J. Brooks. 1997. Evidence for lateral premotor and parietal overactivity in Parkinson’s disease during sequential and bimanual movements, a PET study. Brain 120: 963–976. Scatton, B., F. Javoy-Agid, L. Rouquier, B. Dubois, and Y. Agid. 1983. Reduction of cortical dopamine, noradrenaline, serotonin and their metabolites in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Research 275: 321–328. Selikhova, M., D. R. Williams, P. A. Kempster, J. L. Holton, T. Revesz, and A. J. Lees. 2009. A clinicopathological study of subtypes in Parkinson disease. Brain 132: 2947–2957. Shallice, T. 1982. Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 298: 199–209. Shimada, H., S. Hirano, H. Shinotoh, A. Aotsuka, K. Sato, N. Tanaka, T. Ota, et al. 2009. Mapping of brain acetylcholinesterase alterations in Lewy body disease by PET. Neurology 73: 273–278. Spillantini, M. G., R. A. Crowther, R. Jakes, N. J. Cairns, P. L. Lantos, and M. Goedert. 1998. Filamentous alpha-synuclein inclusions link multiple systems atrophy with Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neuroscience Letters 251: 205–208. Spillantini, M. G., R. A. Crowther, R. Jakes, M. Hasegawa, and M. Goedert. 1998. Alpha-synuclein in filamentous inclusions of Lewy bodies from Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95: 6469–6473. Spillantini, M. G., M. L. Schmidt, V. M.-Y. Lee, J. Q. Troganowski, R. Jakes, and M. Goedert. 1997. Alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies. Nature 388: 839–840. Taylor, A., J. A. Saint-Cyr, and A. E. Lang. 1985. Dementia prevalence in Parkinson’s disease. Lancet 1: 1037. Taylor, A. E., J. A. Saint-Cyr, and A. E. Lang. 1990. Memory and learning in early Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Cognition 2: 211–232. Tison, F., J. F. Dartigues, S. Auriacombe, L. Letenneur, F. Boller, and A. Alperovitch. 1995. Dementia in Parkinson’s disease: A population-based study in ambulatory and institutionalized individuals. Neurology 45: 705–708. Tong, J., H. Wong, M. Guttman, L. C. Ang, L. S. Forno, M. Shimadzu, A. H. Rajput, et al. 2010. Brain alpha-synuclein accumulation in multiple system atrophy,
182
Dementia
Parkinson’s disease and progressive supranuclear palsy: A comparative investigation. Brain 133: 172–188. Uc, E. Y., M. P. McDermott, K. S. Marder, S. W. Anderson, I. Litvan, P. G. Como, P. Auinger, et al. 2009. Incidence of and risk factors for cognitive impairment in an early Parkinson disease clinical trial cohort. Neurology 73: 1469–1477. Wakabayashi, K., K. Matsumoto, K. Takayama, M. Yoshimoto, and H. Takahashi. 1997. NACP, a presynaptic protein, immunoreactivity in Lewy bodies in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience Letters 239: 45–48. Wakabayashi, K., M. Yoshimoto, S. Tsuji, and H. Takahashi. 1998. Alpha-synuclein immunoreactivity in glial cytoplasmic inclusions in multiple systems atrophy. Neuroscience Letters 249: 180–182. Werner, P., and K. Jellinger. 1991. The neuropathologic basis of different clinical subgroups of Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 50: 743–755. Whitehouse, P. J., J. C. Hedreen, C. L. White III, and D. L. Price. 1983. Basal forebrain neurons in the dementia of Parkinson disease. Annals of Neurology 13: 243–248. Wisman, L. A. B., G. Sahin, M. Maingay, G. Leanza, and D. Kirik. 2008. Functional convergence of dopaminergic and cholinergic input is critical for hippocampus-dependent working memory. Journal of Neuroscience 28: 7797–7807. Zimmerman, P., R. Sprengelmeyer, B. Fimm, and C. W. Wallesch. 1992. Cognitive slowing in decision tasks in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Cognition 18: 60–69.
Chapter 7
Aspects in Neuropsychology: Depression and Dementia Ilan Halperin and Amos D. Korczyn
A complex association exists between late-life depression, cerebrovascular disease, and poor cognitive outcome, including progressive dementia and especially Alzheimer ’s disease. While neuroimaging evidence suggests that cerebrovascular disease plays a prominent role in causing dementia, it appears that depression may also confer substantial risk for developing dementia. The relationships between the cerebrovascular changes, other structural abnormalities in the brain, specific forms of cognitive dysfunction, and increased risk for developing dementia among those with late-life depression have been difficult to reconcile. Various findings suggest the existence of multiple pathways to poor cognitive outcomes. In this chapter, we present a framework outlining multiple etiologic and pathogenetic links between depression, cognitive impairment and progressive cognitive decline, including dementia. The suggested framework can assist research by synthesizing the knowledge obtained so far on the depression-dementia relationship, underlying the neurobiological mechanisms which contribute to the identification of at-risk individuals and monitor the impact of depression on the clinical status and course of both illnesses. OVERVIEW OF LATE ADULTHOOD: NEUROBIOLOGICAL, PSYCHOLOGICAL AND SOCIOECONOMICS OF OLD AGE Gerontologists divide older ages into two groups: young elders aged 65 to 74 years, and the oldest-old, aged 75 years and beyond. For many
184
Dementia
individuals the passage from midlife to late adulthood is marked also by a shift from the pursuit of wealth to maintenance of health. (The term “late adulthood” usually refers to the stage of the life cycle that begins at age 65 years, replacing the now politically incorrect term “old age.”) The aging body attracts growing concerns, reflecting diminution in function, altered physical appearance and increased incidence of physical illness. Despite these occurrences, the body in late adulthood can still be a source of considerable feeling of well-being in the sense of competence. This is particularly true if attention is paid to maintaining quality of life through involvement in leisure activities, trusting relationships with family and friends and maintenance of physical health. Neurobiological Changes in Old Age The aging process is characterized by a gradual decline in functions of all body systems, including cardiovascular, respiratory, endocrine, and immune. But the common popular belief that old age is associated with profound intellectual infirmity is a myth since many older persons remain fit in their cognitive abilities and physical capacities (Von Faber et al. 2001). The biological changes that accompany aging do not progress in a linear fashion in all systems, nor do they follow a similar pattern of decline in all persons. Each individual is genetically endowed with one or more vulnerable systems, or a system may become vulnerable because of environmental stressors or intentional misuse, such as excessive ultraviolet exposure, smoking or alcohol abuse. Human aging is a multidimensional process of physical, psychological, and social change. In the central nervous system (CNS) changes result in the decrement of both white and gray matter resulting in reduction of overall brain weight. Dementia is a common neurodegenerative syndrome in old age with many different potential causes, including primary degenerative processes. Other influences include particularly vascular factors. In practice, different types of lesions frequently co-occur. In older individuals, a pure vascular or degenerative picture is rarely seen. Thus, mixed dementia is considered by some the most common type of dementia. This observation leads to an important conclusion, mainly that no single measure may be found which will eliminate the disease globally (Korczyn 2002). All epidemiological studies of dementia show increased prevalence of dementia in advanced age. In the age group of 60 to 64 years the prevalence of dementia is below 1%; it then doubles approximately every five years reaching a prevalence of about 50% among those age 90 years or older (Brayne et al. 2006; Korczyn and Vakhapova 2007).
Aspects in Neuropsychology
185
A variety of neurodegenerative pathologies are associated with the development of late-life dementia. Alzheimer disease (AD) is considered by far the most prevalent pathology before age of 85 years; 60–75% of all cases of dementia are diagnosed as AD (Fratiglioni et al. 2000; Kawas et al. 2000). AD is associated with progressive deterioration of memory and other cognitive functions resulting partly from loss of cholinergic transmission in cortical brain regions innervated by neurons arising in the nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM). The presence of extracellular plaques containing deposits of amyloid and other proteins, as well as intracellular neurofibrillay tangles, are hallmarks of the AD pathology and are thought to contribute to the cognitive deficit. These may result from destructive processes involving the microtubules and synaptic loss. Such destructive processes could further contribute to neuronal damage and disease progression (Youdim 2006). After AD, vascular dementia (VaD) is considered by some to be the second-leading cause of dementia. The frequency of VaD varies by study population, screening methodology, diagnostic criteria, and time period. VaD is a cerebrovascular disease that leads to neuronal loss and decline in cognitive functioning. It occurs when the blood supply carrying oxygen and nutrients to the brain is interrupted or restricted by a diseased vascular system (Lobo et al. 2000). In the United States and Europe, VaD accounts for 10–20% of cases; However in Asia VaD may be as common or even more common than AD. This is likely related to the high prevalence of vascular risk factors in this region (Ueda et al. 1992). Skoog and colleagues (1993) have shown that the prevalence of VaD was slightly higher (46.9%) then the prevalence of AD (43.5%) in people 85 years of age or over. Unlike AD, VaD is not a unitary nosologic entity, but rather a complex of multisided pathologic mechanisms and clinical presentations (Lobo et al. 2000). The several types of VaD include multi-infarct dementia (MID) and poststroke VaD. MID is caused by a series of small strokes that often go unnoticed. Over time, however, the cumulative damage caused to brain tissue interferes with basic cognitive functions and disrupts everyday functioning (Jellinger 2007). MID causes loss of functioning to specific areas of the brain, impairing neurological and mental functions. Neuropsychological findings in VaD vary with the site and severity of the brain lesion (Tomlinson, Blessed, and Roth 1970). For patients with single or multiple large infarcts, deficits correlate with the site and extent of the infarct. In patients with extensive deep white matter disease, impairments may be observed in psychomotor speed, dexterity, executive functions, and motor aspects of speech (e.g., dysarthria or reduced verbal output).
186
Dementia
Patients with subcortical vascular dementia show reduced ability to set and reach goals with mental slowing and gradual executive dysfunction (de Haan, Nys, and Van Zandvoort 2006). Surprisingly, the risk factors associated with VaD are similar to those associated with AD and with the risk for cardiovascular disease (CVD) (Carlson et al. 2008; Fernández Martínez et al. 2008; Abellan van Kan et al. 2009). Based on Chobanian and colleges (2003), some CVD risk factors can be medically treated (see Table 7.1). Psychological Changes and Quality of Life in Old Age In healthy old age persons usually maintain a level of social activity that is only slightly changed from that of earlier years. For many, old age is a period of continued intellectual, emotional, and psychological stimulation. However, in many cases physical illness or death of friends and relatives may limit social interaction. This isolation from family and friends may lead to the development of depression (Blane, Netuveli, and Montgomery 2008). Mandatory retirement due to age, which was an important social milestone in the nineteenth century, has become increasingly outdated since many old persons still feel very potent. However, in some cases old persons may themselves resent and fear other old persons, discriminate against them, and hold negative stereotypes about old age. This phenomenon, first described by Robert Butler (1969) is known as ageism. Ageism is the result of the society’s popular (but wrong) belief that often old age is associated with senility, general weakness, and infirmity (Kang and Chasteen 2009). Table 7.1 Risk Factors for Cerebrovascular Disease Medical intervention Advanced age Genetic factors Male gender High blood pressure Dyslipidemia (elevated LDL, decreased HDL) Diabetes mellitus Atrial fibrillation Smoking Physical inactivity Obesity
Not possible Not possible Not possible Possible Possible Possible Possible Possible Possible Possible
Aspects in Neuropsychology
187
The World Health Organization defines Quality of Life (QoL) as “individuals’ perception of their position in life in the context of the culture and the value system in which they live and in relation to their goals, expectations, standards and concerns” (WHOQOL Group 1995). What influences QoL? The relationship between age and functioning, be it psychological, emotional, or social, has been looked at from two perspectives: chronological perspectives and individual difference perspectives (Baltes 1998). Regarding chronological perspectives, we know that stressful factors accumulate over the years and may result in impaired health or productivity. These factors may explain age-related variance in cognition, emotion, social connectedness, and so on. Research has given credence to this assumption. Chronological age was shown to be a proxy to stressful events such as declining health, lowered income, reduced socializing, cognitive impairment, and institutionalization (Andrews, Clark, and Luszcz 2002). However powerful the effect of the latter factors may be, they account for only about 8–15% of the difference in individual subjective feeling of happiness. Thus it appears that in old age people may became more dependent on their individual psychological resources for maintaining QoL (Lyubomirsky 2001; Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, and Schkade 2005). Individual differences refer to the psychological resources and strategies that serve one to adapt positive outcomes of quality of life in the context of ageing, and preservation of one’s self feeling of well-being and quality of life (Baltes and Carstensen 1996). The effect of such psychological resources on everyday competence was studied by Baltes and Lang (1997). These investigators looked at differential daily functioning between resource-poor and resource-rich elders. Four groups of elderly were identified on the basis of two resource factors: a sensorimotor-cognitive factor and a social-personality factor. Resource-rich elderly differed in several indicators of everyday functioning from resource-poor elderly: length of waking day, variety in activities, frequency of leisure and social activities, as well as resting times. These findings suggest that age-associated activities decline more commonly and to a greater extent in the resource-poor than in the resource-rich group. However, it is not that the resource-rich elders do not experience decline, but that they experience less of it or are able to better compensate. Another psychological resource is known as psychological acceptance (PA). As people get older, they may struggle with reduction in their ability to participate in activities they feel to be meaningful. PA is the ability to accept such age-related changes (Hayes et al. 2004). Higher PA also allows one to spend more time living actively, making choices to assist completion of goals or sorting out problems, rather than spending time and mental
188
Dementia
resources on controlling psychological events. Thus it is hypothesized that people with higher PA have better QoL in the areas of health, community participation, and mental health, and less adverse psychological reactions related to these changes (Bond and Bunce 2003). Surprisingly, the amount of scientific data that directly links individual differences in PA and quality of life in the elderly is meager. However, the existing research suggests that elderly people who are able to do things they enjoy, despite age-related limitations, are more satisfied with their lives (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, and Schkade 2005; Maher and Cummins 2001). Davis and Friedrich (2004) have suggested that elderly subjects who had more knowledge about aging had a better life satisfaction. Presumably increased knowledge enabled them to mentally adapt more readily and accept changes. Socioeconomic Position, Health-Related Quality of Life, and Overall Quality of Life in Old Age Aging is changing in Western communities due to a combination of the increase in life expectancy. Consequently an increasing proportion of the old age population in Western countries can expect to spend from 10 to 20 years after retirement from paid employment pension (Oeppen and Vaupel 2002; Blane et al. 2004). Several socioeconomic factors (SEFs) were investigated in an effort to evaluate their influence on QoL at old age: education, social position, wealth, and gender differences. Education has been widely perceived as one of the most important socioeconomic determinants of health. It is acquired early in life and for most people remains relatively unchanged thereafter. Education may be a surrogate of lifestyle, past-time interests, health behavior, problemsolving abilities, social relations, self-esteem and stress management, in ways which, with regard to health, are to the advantage of the more educated, as well as through income or occupation (Sulander et al. 2006; Winkleby et al. 1992; Minicuci and Noale 2005). Research from a number of countries has shown that this effect is present even at older ages (Martelin, Koskinen, and Valkonen 1998; Silventoinen and Lahelma 2002). Social position is also thought to affect health and mortality in many ways: by influencing attitudes, beliefs and values people use to make life-course choices, through psychosocial stresses, and by influencing lifecourse opportunities. Studies have demonstrated clearly that even marginal class differences can strongly affect health and mortality and that this effect remains even at older ages (Breeze et al. 2001; McMunn et al. 2006).
Aspects in Neuropsychology
189
However the social classification of older people is ambiguous, because the majority is no longer employed (Grundy and Holt 2001). It has been suggested (Arber and Ginn 1993) that instead of an elderly person’s previous class, an alternative indicator of structural position such as current material circumstances (i.e., wealth) could be used. Wealth is particularly important when studying SEF at older ages since it can reflect an individual’s accumulated lifetime experience (Filakti and Fox 1995). Wealth, especially at older ages, is likely to influence health by providing material resources and a feeling of security and control and by influencing health behaviors (Allin, Masseria, and Mossialos 2009). Wealth is therefore a better measure of economic status than income, especially after retirement (Smith and Kington 1997). Regarding gender differences women experience longer survival and therefore may accumulate more disabilities (Verbrugge 1989). The contribution of SEF to these differences is not clear. Gender may influence health through occupation and social position (which differ especially at older ages) and through employment and earning history, which influence familial wealth (Dahl 1994; Lahelma et al. 1999). The various factors of SEF mentioned above, and their potential effect on QoL at older ages are also explained by their association to other measures of health-related quality of life (HRQoL). HRQoL is a term originally coined as a measure of overall health status evaluating how good or bad it would be in a given health status measured by various domains of health (e.g., pain, impaired physical function and impaired mental health) (Robert et al. 2009). People with lower SEF (low income, education, and assets) have poorer QoL and also poorer HRQoL at all age groups including older ages. However, it was found that specific factors such as low income and low education at midlife ages were specifically strong predictors of worse HRQoL in ages 65 and older (Robert et al. 2009). Understanding the relationships between SEF and HRQoL and their combined effect on QoL in old age is further complicated by the fact that some social variables are theorized to positively affect QoL in old age (further discussed in the study by Netuveli and colleagues), thus conflicting with low SEF and HRQoL and contradicting with the results of Stock et al. (1983). Netuveli and colleagues (2006) concluded that doing volunteer work, being a resident in a neighborhood perceived to be good, having trusting relationships with children, family, and friends, and access to a car or owning a pet yielded positive effects on QoL and health in old age. In conclusion, aging is perceived to decrease QoL, but it is important to look for QoL predictors other than chronological age per se. Many factors
190
Dementia
are suspected; however, little is known about their relative importance and no single factor solitarily determines QoL at old age. Factors like perceived poor financial situation, depression, functional limitation attributable to longstanding illness, and limitations in everyday activities can affect QoL negatively, while those like having trusting relationships with children, family, and friends, and affluence can improve quality of life. These factors are frequently interconnected and interdependent. LATE-LIFE DEPRESSION Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a mood disorder characterized by low mood accompanied by low self-esteem, and loss of interest or pleasure in normally enjoyable activities (Belmaker and Agam 2008). In the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), the diagnosis of MDD requires the presence of single episodes persisting for at least two weeks or recurrent major depressive episodes. Further qualifiers are used to classify both the episode itself and the course of the disorder (American Psychiatric Association 2000). MDD is common in the elderly, with an estimated prevalence of ∼3% in the general population and 15–25% among nursing home residents (Rothschild 1996). Approximately 15% of community-dwelling elderly have clinically significant depressive symptoms, and such symptoms are present in ∼25% of those elderly who have a chronic medical illness (Unutzer et al. 1997). MDD has been chronologically divided into two types: early-onset depression (EOD) and late-life depression (LLD). EOD arises in adults between the ages of 45 to 64 years, while LLD refers to depression starting after age 65 (Lyness et al. 1994). Late- and Early-Onset Depression When compared with elderly individuals with EOD, individuals with LLD have less frequently family history of mood disorders, larger impairment in neuropsychological tests, more neurosensory hearing impairment, enlargement of lateral brain ventricles, more white-matter hyperintensities, and a higher rate of dementia development on follow-up (Alexopoulos, Young, and Shindledecker 1992; Jacoby and Levy 1980). LOD individuals are less likely to have psychiatric co-morbidities, such as personality disorders, substance abuse, or panic disorder, than EOD patients (Lyness et al. 1994) and are more likely to have associated medical co-morbidities (Emery and Oxman 1992) and fewer episodes of depression,
Aspects in Neuropsychology
191
and experience more often feelings of loss of interest and apathy (Krishnan et al. 1995; Heun, Kockler, and Papassotiropoulos 2000). The importance for identifying and treating LOD stems from the fact that it is considered a risk factor for death; the severity of depressive symptomatology has been found to be a strong predictor for suicide (Alexopoulos 2001). LLD also interferes with the patient’s cooperation and compliance to prescription medication and treatment of his or her depression; this is due to the negative perception and social stigma about depression (DiMatteo, Lepper, and Croghan 2000). QoL is negatively affected by LLD because of lower self-rated health that is beyond the levels predicted by objectively measured physical conditions (Schulberg et al. 1998), psychomotor slowing, emotional lability, crying spells, insomnia, weight loss, and pessimism (McGuire and Rabins 1994). The effect of LLD on HRQoL includes amplified perception of pain (Lynch 2001). Compared to nondepressed old persons, older depressed patients tend to have, or report, more somatic and cognitive symptoms than affective symptoms. Older patients who deny having depressed mood may report a lack of feeling or emotion, or acknowledge a loss of interest and pleasure in activities. The tendency of depressed older adults to report fewer affective symptoms is captured by the concept of “depression without sadness” (Gallo and Rabins 1999). This variant has been identified in elderly primary care populations and consists of apathy, loss of interest, fatigue, difficulty sleeping, and other somatic symptoms, but not sad mood. It is unclear whether “depression without sadness” is an idiopathic depression, a depression secondary to medical illness, or a nonaffective syndrome related to apathy. Screening for Depression While both dementia and LLD are commonly seen in old age, no data exist as to their co-occurrence. This is because epidemiological studies on dementia typically exclude patients diagnosed as being depressed and vice versa, studies on depression exclude people suffering from dementia. Lacking established biological markers for both dementia and LLD prevents a clear distinction between the two entities or studying their simultaneous existence. Yet several features, such as poor concentration and impaired attention, are common in both (Halperin and Korczyn 2007). Commonly used screening instruments measuring the severity of depression include the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck and Steer 1987), the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD) (Hamilton 1960), the Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al. 1983), the General Health Questionnaire
192
Dementia
(Goldberg 1978), the Zung self-rated depression scale (Zung 1965), and others. In a review of 18 studies of major depression, these assessment instruments have been found to have 84% sensitivity and 72% specificity for detecting major depression in depressed old persons (Mulrow et al. 1995). However, the characteristics of old patients who screen positive for depression but who do not have major depression remain to be identified. Some of these patients may have persistent minor depression, dysthymia, or a partially remitted depression. It is largely unknown whether elevated screening scores but no diagnosis (“subsyndromal depression”) are a risk for later affective or cognitive deterioration. One study (Lyness et al. 2006) compared outcomes among patients with minor and subsyndromal depression, major depression, and no depression, to examine putative outcome. Results showed that compared with patients who were not depressed, those who had minor or subsyndromal depression had a 5.5-fold risk for major depression at one year. As discussed below, those individuals are also at increased risk to develop dementia. Correctly identifying depression at early stages may allow more benefit from treatment. Yet most primary care providers do not consistently use screening instruments in their practice. Routine screening, when incorporated into practice, can improve diagnosis; however, screening alone may be insufficient for initiating treatment and does not lead to better outcomes (Coyne et al. 2000). In primary care centers other barriers can decrease the likelihood of successful diagnosis of LLD. Brown and colleagues (1995) found that men are less likely to report mood-related symptoms than women, and primary care physicians are less able to recognize depression in men than in women (Potts, Burnam, and Wells 1991). Furthermore, reports of somatic complaints may lead to misdiagnosis of depression in the primary care setting (Goldman 1997). In summary, making a diagnosis of LLD can be difficult for many reasons including more subtle presentations in older adults or coexisting medical illnesses. Numerous screening instruments exist and such instruments can help detect depressive symptoms. Patients who screen positive should be further interviewed to determine whether they can benefit from treatment. Screening for Dementia Much progress has been made in identifying the typical pattern of cognitive deficits associated with early AD. Yet the boundaries between normal age-related cognitive changes and early signs of AD remain especially difficult to delineate in very elderly individuals (i.e., over the age of 80).
Aspects in Neuropsychology
193
This is because many of the early structural and functional brain changes of AD overlap with changes observed in normal aging. Normal aging is associated with mild brain atrophy (Jack et al. 1997; Pfefferbaum et al. 1994) and reduced synaptic density (Masliah et al. 1993). These brain changes are thought to mediate age-related decline in information processing speed, executive function, learning efficiency, and retrieval (Hedden and Gabrieli 2004). Because normal aging can detrimentally affect many of the same cognitive abilities affected by AD, specific deficits related to AD may be much less evident in the AD oldest-old patients than in the younger AD patients. As a result, a less-distinct cognitive deficits profile is associated with AD in the oldest-old AD patients. Thus, Bondi and colleagues (2003) noted that despite achieving similar raw scores on all neuropsychological measures, the old and the oldest-old AD patients differed in the severity and pattern of the cognitive deficits in relation to agematched healthy controls. The old AD patients were generally more impaired than the oldest-old AD patients and exhibited worse deficits in memory and executive functions than in other cognitive domains. In contrast, the oldest-old AD patients exhibited a similar level of impairment across all cognitive domains so that their deficits profile lacked the disproportionate saliency of memory and executive function deficits typical of the disease. Thus, aging can significantly affect the severity and pattern of neuropsychological deficits associated with early AD and reduce the saliency of the deficit. Several studies suggest that memory performance may be poor a number of years prior to the development of the dementia and then decline rapidly. Small and colleagues (2000) and Backman and colleagues (2001) found that episodic memory was mildly impaired six years prior to dementia onset. Yet, neuropsychological screening tools are not free from limitation. Some have noted that screening failure rates have been estimated between 50% and 80% for moderate to severe dementia and up to 91% for mild dementia cases (Boustani et al. 2005). International survey revealed that the most frequently used cognitive assessment tests are the Folstein Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) and the Clock Drawing Test (CDT) (Shulman et al. 2006). However, the MMSE has been criticized for poor sensitivity and specificity. One study demonstrated that the MMSE has only 80% sensitivity and 86% specificity for correctly identifying dementia (Cherbuin, Anstey, and Lipnicki 2008). Others (Lorentz, Scanlan, and Borson 2002) concluded that the CDT has higher accuracy than the MMSE for identifying dementia. However, in the
194
Dementia
absence of specialist physician examination, the CDT by itself cannot be considered reliable enough for accurate clinical dementia screening purpose. In summary, screening tools for dementia have advanced in delineating patterns of impaired neuropsychological abilities. Future advances may offer clinicians the opportunity to begin early drug treatment soon as it becomes available. DEPRESSION WITH REVERSIBLE DEMENTIA One association between depression and cognitive impairment (CI) has previously been termed “pseudo-dementia.” This term refers to cognitive decline that can be reversed by treating the depression (or other psychiatric symptoms). However, dementia is a syndrome and anything that fulfills its criteria is dementia proper, regardless of cause or reversibility (Korczyn 1991). Moreover, Kiloh (1961) argued that the term “pseudodementia” has no valid diagnostic value since it does not provide any specific diagnostic information. Nevertheless, the term is still widely used implying cognitive impairment that is not due to structural changes of the brain and thus is potentially reversible. Alexopoulos and colleagues (1993) suggested that the CI associated with depression is often not reversible or only partially reversible, and that CI associated with LLD persists despite amelioration of depressive symptoms. Kral and Emery (1989) found that 39 of 44 elderly patients (89%) diagnosed with depressive pseudodementia developed AD within 4 to 18 years (average of 8 years). In a study of 20 depressed patients aged 60 years or over (half had their first episode of depression after 50 years of age), 37% of recovered depressed patients showed residual CI compared to normal control subjects (Abas, Sahakian, and Levy 1990). In order to estimate how often dementia can be reversed, Weytingh and colleagues (1995) carried out a quantitative review of 16 studies comprising 1551 patients. The percentages of reversed dementia varied widely: from 0 to 23% for partial and from 0 to 10% for full recovery. Depression and drug intoxication were the most frequent causes of reversible dementia, followed by metabolic and neurosurgical disorders. The percentage of both partial and full recovery of dementia has dropped in recent years, to less than 1% for both. This decrease could be associated with setting and the use of stricter diagnostic methods. The researchers concluded that reversible dementia is very rare in an outpatient setting when using strict diagnostic methods. These studies indicate that elderly patients suffering a depressive disorder with associated CI are more likely to develop dementia than depressed subjects who are cognitively intact.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
195
THE SEARCH FOR A POSSIBLE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN DEPRESSION AND DEMENTIA Depressive symptoms are observed in 35–50% of dementia patients, particularly in the earlier stages of the disease (Nilsson et al. 2002). There seems to be a complex relationship between depression and dementia pointing toward several possible hypotheses, not necessarily mutually exclusive: (1) depression is a mental reaction in patients experiencing cognitive decline; (2) depression may be a characteristic of early dementia; (3) depression is a risk factor for dementia; and (4) dementia and depression share common underlying brain changes or risk factors. These possibilities will be now discussed.
Depression Is a Mental Reaction in Patients Experiencing Cognitive Decline Social events have great impact on a person’s mood. Living alone, having limited social interaction and support, or living in neighborhoods with limited transportation all predispose to cognitive decline (La Gory and Fitpatrick 1992; Dean et al. 1992). Physical illnesses are also known to produce depressive mood, such as is common in victims of cancer and cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases (Gruneir et al. 2005). Facing a serious illness like cancer or dementia can result in denial, or in a search for treatment, but it is also stressful and can cause depressed mood. This is true even in cases when the diagnosis is not delivered directly to the patient. Patients with incipient cognitive decline can feel the loss of mental functions and react to it emotionally. Clinicians should be aware of such mental reactions that develop in patients experiencing cognitive decline and strive to look for them even in cases when there are no clear complaints. Poor appetite, insomnia, and loss of libido can be the manifestations of depression, which should be identified and treated (American Psychiatric Association 2000).
Depression May Be a Characteristic of Early Dementia Retrospective studies describe an association between a history of depression and an increased risk for the development of AD. That risk was particularly high when depression appeared in elderly individuals (Jorm et al. 1991). In recent years investigators have been trying to define an intermediate stage between normal aging and AD, using the term mild cognitive impairment (MCI). The common tendency is to view normal
196
Dementia
aging, MCI, and AD as a continuum (Burns and Zaudig 2002). However, it is not necessarily true that every adult will develop MCI or that all those that suffer from MCI will ultimately progress to dementia. Longitudinal studies found that 19–50% of elderly people suffering from MCI will develop dementia during a period of three to four years from their initial diagnosis (Hanninen et al. 2002). Modrego and Ferrandez (2004) found increased risk to develop dementia among subjects with MCI who are also depressed. At the beginning of their study depression or depressive symptoms were diagnosed in 36% of the patients. After an average period of three years, 85% of these patients developed dementia as compared to only 32% of the subjects who were not suffering from depression. These results show the intimate relationship between late onset of depression and incipient cognitive decline and may pave the road in helping to identify subjects that are likely to develop dementia. It is well established that abnormalities in serotonergic neurotransmission are central to the pathophysiology of depression in younger adults, but underlying pathological changes associated with these abnormalities are less clear (McAllister-Williams, Ferrier, and Young 1998), but seem to imply the serotonergic system (Meltzer et al. 1998). In a study by Baumann and colleagues (2002) autopsy examinations were performed on brains of 12 patients with mood disorders in comparison to the brains of 12 normal subjects. Results showed reduction of 31% in the number of neurons of the ventrolateral subnucleus of the dorsal raphe of patients with mood disorders compared with control subjects. This neuronal deficiency in the dorsal raphe may contribute to impaired serotonergic innervations of brain regions which are involved in mood regulation. In conclusion, similar neuropathology related processes might underline the association between AD and depression. This may result in earlier expression of MCI. The additive or synergistic effects of additional pathologic processes associated with depression may result in reduced neuronal reserve leading to earlier symptoms of dementia. Depression Is a Risk Factor for Dementia Depression is an important issue for those working with the elderly. This is because depression affects a large number of elderly subjects and has been associated with increased morbidity and mortality. Kessing and Nilsson (2003) studied the association between affective disorder and subsequent dementia in patients with unipolar or bipolar affective disorders compared to patients with other chronic illnesses (osteoarthritis and diabetes) during the period of 1977 to 2003. Patients with unipolar or bipolar
Aspects in Neuropsychology
197
affective disorder had an increased risk of developing dementia compared to patients with other illnesses. Nilsson and colleagues found in follow-up cohorts up to 21 years that patients (mean age 77 years) who developed dementia had a prior increased risk of being admitted to a hospital due to major depression or mania (Nilsson et al. 2002). Ownby and colleagues (2006) systematically reviewed and completed a meta-analysis of 20 relevant studies, from an even larger sample, which provided data of 102,172 persons from eight countries. The meta-analysis showed that persons with a history of depression were more likely to be diagnosed as having AD in later life. This finding was robust across analyses stratified sampling type, retrospective versus prospective data collection and strictness of diagnostic criteria used for AD and depression. However, these data did not provide information about why depression and AD may be linked. In conclusion, depression is a risk factor predisposing for future development of dementia. However, whether prior depression is a true etiologic risk factor or rather an early clinical manifestation of dementia is unclear. Dementia and Depression Share Common Underlying Brain Changes or Risk Factors AD is characterized by the formation of neurofibrillary tangles and plaques, and neuronal loss across the CNS (Small et al. 2000). The histopathological changes show a characteristic sequence, with the entorhinal cortex and the hippocampus being among the first affected regions of the brain, followed by selected regions of the neocortex (Braak and Braak 1991). Neuroimaging studies present supporting evidence to neuroanatomical changes in AD brains: Using voxel-based morphometry, a study by Ohnishi and colleagues (2001) found a significant reduction of gray matter volume in the hippocampal formation and entorhinal cortex bilaterally in AD patients’ brains. As imaging techniques developed during the last two decades, evidence began to accumulate that depression is accompanied by structural changes in the brain that have a resemblance to AD. The hippocampus particularly is severely damaged in the early stages of AD (Van de Pol et al. 2006), but Bremner and colleagues (2000) found reduction of hippocampal volume also in nondemented depressed patients. Neuropathological studies such as the postmortem study of Rapp and colleagues (2006) found distinct differences in both neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the hippocampus of patients with AD as a function of depression history. Specifically, patients with neuropathologically
198
Dementia
confirmed diagnosis of AD who had a history of major depressive disorder exhibited a larger number of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in the hippocampus than patients with AD who never had an episode of major depressive disorder in their life. Such was not the case in the study conducted by Wilson and colleagues (2003) who evaluated prospectively a group of 130 Catholic nuns, priests, and brothers. Brain autopsy at death revealed that the association of depressive symptoms with clinical AD and cognitive impairment appeared to be independent of cortical plaques and tangles. Studies that have pointed out a connection between cardiovascular risk factors and depression have led to the “vascular depression hypothesis” (Alexopoulos et al. 1997). According to this hypothesis, subcortical neuronal tracts and in particular periventricular ones are important for the regulation of mood and motivation. Therefore it is reasonable to assume that subcortical lesions in these areas might inflict mood disorders and depression. Indeed, white-matter changes (WMC) are common among the elderly and are thought to result from damage to small blood vessels due to hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, and diabetes mellitus (de Leeuw et al. 2002). WMC occur in both VaD and AD (Hirono et al. 2000; O’Brien et al. 1996) but were described by Chen and colleagues (2006) also in depressed patients. De Groot and colleagues (2000) showed a relationship between the presence of depressive symptoms and WMC among 1077 nondemented elderly subjects. Subjects with WMC presented more depressive symptoms and their depressive symptoms were more frequent. Subjects with severe WMC had a higher risk (3 to 5 times more) to develop depression compared with subjects who had only lesser degrees of WMC. In summary, the link between dementia and LLD may be mediated by neuropathologic changes including greater hippocampal amyloid plaque neurofibrillary tangle pathology in AD patients with a lifetime history of depression. Additional evidence supporting the notion that vascular disease contributes to depression comes from structural MRI studies that show an association between ischemic brain lesions and depression diagnosis in older persons. These studies suggest a relationship between AD, ischemic WMHs, and depression, as well as AD changes. This raises the possibility that ischemic structural changes in the brain are a common etiologic factor of both the depression and the associated cognitive dysfunction. The data described so far about the shared brain changes suggest that it is reasonable to assume that LLD by itself can predict subsequent development of dementia in individuals with cerebrovascular or AD pathology (Thomas, Kalaria, and O’Brien 2004).
Aspects in Neuropsychology
199
DEPRESSION IN VASCULAR BRAIN DISEASE The proposed link between depression and vascular disease is not new and was originally proposed as “arteriosclerotic depressive disease” in Gaupp’s article, “Depressive States in Old Age” (1905). Some studies have suggested that cardiovascular risk factors may enhance the risk for depression. However, depression may in turn increase the risk for cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease (Kales, Maixner, and Mellow 2005). Carney and colleagues (1999) reported that half of the patients suffering from cardiovascular diseases had a history of depressive episodes prior to the diagnosis of cardiovascular disease. Also, one of every five patients had previously been diagnosed as having major depression at the time of diagnostic cardiac catheterization or acute myocardial infarction (Hance et al. 1996). Another one in five had minor depression at these times. Approximately one in three patients developed major depression at some time during the 12 months after a coronary event (FrasureSmith et al. 1999). Mood disturbances may be a specific complication of stroke, and it has been estimated that between 18% of a local community’s elders and 78% of hospital admission poststroke patients suffer from depression (Morris, Robinson, and Raphael 1990; Stern and Bachman 1991), with the period of greatest risk being the 2 years following the stroke event (Parikh et al. 1990). At present, support for vascular brain disease as an underlying etiology of LLD includes the high rate of depression in patients with vascular disease and the frequency of “silent stroke” and white-matter hyperintensities (WMH) in LLD. Structural neuroimaging and neuropathology studies have shown a relationship between frontostriatal impairment and late life depression. Bilateral WMH are prevalent in geriatric (Kumar et al. 2000) and mainly occur in subcortical structures and their frontal projections (Lesser et al. 1996). Subcortical WMH have been found to be associated with executive dysfunction (Boone et al. 1992). Lesions localized in the basal ganglia and their frontal projections are also associated with high incidence of depression and executive dysfunction (Rajkowska et al. 1999). Neuropathological studies identified abnormalities related to frontal impairment. Reduction in glia of the subgenual prelimbic anterior cingulate gyrus has been demonstrated in unipolar depressed patients (Rajkowska et al. 1999; Lai et al. 2000). Abnormalities in neurons of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex have also been documented in unipolar disorder (Ongur, Drevets, and Price 1998). Yet, the exact relationship between depression and vascular brain disease still remains ambiguous.
200
Dementia
Depression may itself predispose to vascular disease. Musselman and colleagues (1998) proposed a link between depression and cardiovascular disease that included (1) the effects of hypercortisolemia; (2) immune activation; and (3) depression-related platelet aggregation leading to increased thrombosis. Others included (4) depression-induced impairment of arterial endothelial functioning (Broadley et al. 2002) or (5) abnormal folate or homocysteine metabolism (Godfrey et al. 1990). Although these mechanisms have been proposed to relate depression to cardiovascular disease, several could pertain to depression-cerebrovascular disease linkages as well. The association between depression and vascular brain disease could also represent the consequences of a shared underlying etiology such as atherosclerosis. Atherosclerosis could lead to events such as cerebral lesions, which could trigger depression either through disruption of critical pathways implicated in mood regulation or accumulation of lesions exceeding a certain threshold (Alexopoulos et al. 1997). Vascular disease may also be linked to depression via shared genetic risk factors. Although the apolipoprotein ε4 (APOE ε4) allele is an established risk factor for coronary artery disease and for AD, the finding of an increased frequency in LLD is less clear (Krishnan et al. 1996; Steffens et al. 2003). Finally, nonbiological factors may be involved in the brain depression– vascular disease linkage, including the effects of depression on decreasing adherence to treatment regimens for vascular diseases such as hypertension, heart disease, or diabetes.
SHARED CHANGES IN THE NEUROTRANSMITTER SYSTEM BETWEEN DEMENTIA AND DEPRESSION Changes in the Serotonergic System Neurotransmitter changes appear during aging. Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) is widely distributed in the human nervous system. Most of the brain cells containing serotonin are located in the raphe nuclei (RN) at the brain stem and in the hypothalamus. In the brain serotonin has many functions, including involvement in memory and affective processes (Korczyn and Blum 1976). Most of the studies that explore the relationship between depression and the serotonergic system were performed in young adults and animal models, and only a few were done on elderly populations. These few studies suggest that mood disorders in old age are accompanied by disruption in serotonin metabolism (Gareri, De Fazio, and De Sarro 2002).
Aspects in Neuropsychology
201
Suicide is traditionally considered an extreme response to stress, with the most frequent stress being depressive illness. A biological role for the serotonergic system, possibly associated with a genetic risk factor, has been postulated (Mann et al. 1989). Low levels of 5-HT and 5-HIAA (the 5HT metabolite) have been found in postmortem examinations of brainstem tissues of suicide victims. An increased number of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2 receptors was found in the pre-frontal cortex of suicide victims, implying up-regulation, induced by 5-HT deficiency (Rao et al. 1998). On the other hand, Stockmeier (1997) examined various indices of serotonergic neurotransmission in axonal projection areas such as prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and cell bodies originated within the dorsal RN. All samples were obtained from postmortem studies of depression and suicide victims. The author concluded that there were no significant differences between suicide victims with major depression and psychiatrically normal control subjects in serotonin-1A or serotonin-2A receptors in the right prefrontal cortex or the hippocampus. However there were region-specific alterations in suicide victims with major depression in G-protein-induced activation of the phosphoinositide signal transduction system and in the levels of G-protein alpha subunits involved in cyclic AMP synthesis. Jones and colleagues (1990) showed lower levels of 5HIAA in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with a history of suicide attempts. Antidepressant drugs are thought to work by limiting serotonin and noradrenaline reuptake, raising neurotransmitter concentrations in the synapse. Evidence supporting serotonergic involvement in AD stem from findings of reduced reuptake of serotonin in the temporal cortex (Reinikainen, Soininen, and Riekkinen 1990). Other studies focused on the RN itself. The dorsal part of the nucleus contains a large number of dense serotonergic neurons which project to the basal ganglia, thalamus, hypothalamus, and cortex. In AD there is depopulation of neurons in RN and that nucleus is also affected by many neurofibrillary tangles (Curcio and Kemper 1984). However, serotonin reuptake inhibitor drugs do not have a beneficial cognitive effect in AD (Grau-Veciana 2006). Changes in the Noradrenergic System The origin of the noradrenergic system in the brain is the locus ceruleus (LC) located at the brainstem. Axons from the LC reach many brain areas such as the hippocampus, amygdala, and neocortex. Activation of the noradrenergic system enhances the reaction to sensory information by amplifying the response to relevant stimuli.
202
Dementia
McGaugh and colleagues (1996) discovered that applying β noradrenergic receptor agonists enhances memory consolidation while antagonists had an opposite effect. In their study Ordway and colleagues (2003) studied the binding of the radioactive ligand P[125] iodoclonidine to α2– adrenoceptors at the LC of patients suffering from major depression and showed significant elevation in major depression patients compared to matched controls. These are consistent with deficiency of noradrenaline in the LC in subjects with major depression. Typical AD changes such as neurofibrillary degeneration were also found in the LC. During normal aging neurofibrillary tangles develop in the LC but they are more prominent in AD (Grudzien et al. 2007). In this study the investigators also made a comparison between the extent of the cytopathology in the LC to the cognitive state prior to the death of the subjects. Other studies (Chen et al. 2000; Rub et al. 2000) confirmed the presence of neurofibrillary tangles in the LC and RN in AD. From the evidence presented so far it seems that the concentrations of noradrenaline in the brains of AD patients and depressed patients is decreased due to degeneration of the LC (Syed et al. 2005). In summary, there are several similarities between neurotransmitter changes in brains of AD patients and elderly depressed patients. It is not likely that the neurotransmitter changes mentioned above are responsible for dementia, since drugs that increase the activity of such neurotransmitters do not have a strong beneficial cognitive effect in AD despite being useful in treating depression. The presence of such changes in AD should therefore be seen as an expression of the disease and not as a direct cause for the cognitive decline. Changes in Hormonal System The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is an important feedback system controlling the release of the stress hormone cortisol. Hypercortisolism is typical for depressed patients, probably because of poor feedback inhibition (Holsboer 2000). Abundant glucocorticoid receptors exist in the hippocampus and frontal cortex, the function of which is still unclear. However, prolonged exposure to high cortisol levels can damage neurons in these brain areas. Animal models have shown death of hippocampal neurons following exposure to high concentrations of glucocorticoid hormones (Sapolsky 2000). Hence it was speculated that high cortisol levels can induce cognitive impairments (Van de Pol et al. 2006). This last fact might explain the limited antidepressant effect on cognitive functioning (Butters et al. 2000).
Aspects in Neuropsychology
203
If a patient’s response to antidepressants is poor, than it is possible that the cognitive symptomatology is related to hypercortisolism and neurodegenerative processes. Elderly individuals may be more vulnerable to this process since hypercortisolism is more common in older as opposed to younger people (O’Brien 1997). If high glucocorticoid levels can cause hippocampal atrophy, this can be of significant importance in the pathogenesis of AD. Wolf and colleagues (2005) studied the relation between subjective cognitive complaints and high levels of cortisol in a sample of healthy middle aged and older subjects (mean age 62) with and without cognitive complaints. Results indicated a significant correlation between high cortisol levels and cognitive impairments, particularly in memory. High cortisol levels with disrupted sensitivity to steroids feedback is also described in VaD (Maeda et al. 1991). Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its derivative dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate (DHEAS) are produced by both the brain and the pituitary glands. These hormones have an important role in activating the immune system, particularly under stress. In contrast to only small changes in cortisol levels with aging, there is marked reduction in DHEAS with aging. Cortisol and DHEAS have opposite effects. While exposure to cortisol promotes neuronal death, DHEAS has protective properties. Therefore a change in the ratio between cortisol and DHEAS (in favor to cortisol) is a neurotoxic factor (Ferrari et al. 2001). In summary, corticosteroid changes that occur in depression may cause degenerative changes in the hippocampus, possibly similar to AD.
IS THERE A SPECIAL PROFILE OF DEPRESSION OR COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT THAT IS A RISK FACTOR FOR DEMENTIA? Depression-Executive Dysfunction Lockwood and colleagues (2002) have suggested that executive dysfunctions (i.e., abstract thinking, planning, executing complex behavior, or difficulties in shifting mental sets) can be seen as central features of LLD. This idea was highlighted again by Alexopoulos and colleagues who introduced the term “depression-executive dysfunction” (DED) (Alexopoulos 2001; Alexopoulos, Kiosses, et al. 2002). DED has been described and conceptualized as an entity with pronounced frontostriatal dysfunction. On a clinical level, DED is characterized by psychomotor retardation, reduced interest in activities, and impaired instrumental activities of daily living (Alexopoulos et al. 2001).
204
Dementia
Krishnan and colleagues (1997) showed that brain infracts affecting frontal-subcortical circuitry would be more common in patients with DED than in patients without DED, and also more common than in patients with depression but without executive dysfunction. These findings support the DED hypothesis presuming that many patients with LLD have an underlying cerebrovascular disease with ischemic WMC, which in turn disrupt both mood regulation and executive function pathways (Alexopoulos, Kiosses, et al. 2002). Alexopoulos, Kiosses, and colleagues (2002) describe the unique clinical presentation of DED in comparison to LLD. Depressive symptomatology and especially psychomotor retardation and loss of interest in activities were observed in DED patients, whereas the latter symptoms did not influence the functioning of depressed patients without executive impairment. Is There a Special Kind of Depression That Is a Risk Factor for Preceding Development of Dementia? Longitudinal studies (Sheline et al. 1999; Chodosh et al. 2007; Devanand et al. 1996; Zubenko, Mossy, and Koop 1990) pointed out that it is the severity of depressive symptoms at the baseline measurement that is associated with future cognitive decline. However it is still not known whether severity of the depressive symptoms over time is also associated with cognitive decline. Rovner and colleagues (2009) tested the hypothesis that variability in Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) scores over time can predict cognitive decline in a study of 160 participants over 65 years old. Cognitive evaluation and GDS were administrated every six months. Results showed that 23 (14.4%) declined cognitively. Age, low education, baseline GDS score (>5), and also variability in GDS scores (i.e., fluctuations between time points) were associated with cognitive decline. Interestingly, the GDS item “Do you feel you have more problems with memory than most?” was significantly more often endorsed by subjects who later declined cognitively than by those who did not (21% versus 2.6% respectively). These findings suggest that not only the severity of depressive symptoms at baseline, but also that cognitive decline followed by mood variability over time (i.e., fluctuations in GDS scores) may reflect damage of neurobiological mechanisms. Another study (Alexopoulos et al. 2005) examined the relationship of executive impairment to the course of depressive symptoms among elderly patients with major depression. A total of 112 nondemented elderly patients with major depression participated in an eight-week citalopram
Aspects in Neuropsychology
205
(a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor [SSRI] trial). Executive functions were assessed with the initiation/perseveration subscale of the Dementia Rating Scale and the Stroop Color-Word test. Medical burden was rated with the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. The main finding of this study was that both abnormal initiation/perseveration and abnormal Stroop Color-Word scores were associated with an unfavorable response of SSRI. These findings confirm earlier results suggesting that impairment in executive functions predicts adverse outcomes of geriatric depression (Potter et al. 2004; Simpson et al. 1998). The theoretical significance of this finding is that it provides a rationale for CI that appears to be a part of depression rather than an indirect behavioral consequence of depressive symptoms. Moreover, most demented disorders have high rates of depressive symptoms. As such, cohort studies (Devanand et al. 1996; Buntinx et al. 1996) showed that a history of depression increases the risk for later development of dementia. These findings suggest that elderly subjects with MCI are at increased risk for future development of dementia. MDD is associated with specific cognitive deficits including poor attention and concentration and slow mental-processing speed (Boone et al. 1995). However, when cerebrovascular disease is present, it is the unique profile of DED that puts older people at increased risk. DED can thus be considered as the phenotype for the underlying brain vascular pathology predisposing to dementia. It appears that frontostriatal–limbic abnormalities predispose to both depressive symptoms and executive dysfunction in older adults (Alexopoulos, Buckwalter et al. 2002). This view is supported by clinical as well as by brain structure and brain function studies. Degenerative disorders of the basal ganglia, as well as stroke of the caudate head and the left frontal pole, often present with depression and executive dysfunction (Sobin and Sacheim 1997). Structural neuroimaging studies (Krishnan, Hays, and Blazer 1997) have shown that MRI hyperintensities are common in subcortical structures and their frontal and limbic connections of depressed elderly patients with executive dysfunction, and impaired metabolism of the caudate nucleus and the frontal regions accompany the depressive state. POSTDEPRESSION COGNITIVE DECLINE Knowledge is sparse concerning whether recovery from a depressive episode also entails recovery from the accompanying cognitive dysfunction. Available evidence on this issue has mostly focused on the effects of antidepressant treatment of cognitive functioning in the elderly, suggesting
206
Dementia
that cognitive dysfunction may persist after recovery from depression (Weiland-Fiedler et al. 2004). Taylor and colleagues (2002) investigated the relationship of depression severity and CI following antidepressant treatment in 52 elderly depressed patients following a one-year treatment period. The study demonstrated that greater severity of depression at baseline treatment was associated with less improvement in cognitive deficits, even when medication therapy had been successful. In a recent study Bhalla and colleagues (2009) examined both the characteristics and frequency of CI among 109 depressed subjects at remission after treatment, compared to an age-matched group of never depressed elderly subjects. The results showed that despite adequate antidepressant treatment and relative to the control group, twice as many depressed subjects had CI and were diagnosed with MCI or dementia (48% versus 28% respectively). Also, older subjects (age>74, 65%) were more likely to be diagnosed with MCI compared to the younger subjects (age<74, 31%). Subjects with LLD were more likely to be diagnosed with MCI or dementia (56%) than those with EOD (35%). Another study (Butters et al. 2000) indicated that elderly LLD patients with CI may experience improvement in some cognitive domains following antidepressant treatment but may not necessarily reach normal cognitive level of performance particularly in memory and executive functions. Airaksinen and colleagues (2004) examined in a three-year retrospective study the performance of elderly depressed patients. At follow-up, despite the symptomatic improvement and improved social functioning, episodic memory and social functioning did not follow this general recovery trend. Cognitive functions that fail to be normalized after remission from depression can thus be termed as postdepression cognitive decline (PDCD). PDCD represents an increased risk for the development of dementia (Halperin and Korczyn 2009). In Figure 7.1 we present a model for the relation between subtypes of MCI that persist after recovery from LLD and prediction about which kind of dementia will develop over time. CONCLUSIONS Depression and dementia are common in old age. Accumulating epidemiological evidence in recent years found that depression is a risk factor for the appearance of dementia. Yet it is unclear whether MDD in patients with dementia is caused by a mental reaction to the disease, or, do aging and disease related brain abnormalities predispose to late-life depression.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
207
Figure 7.1
Subtypes of Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) persist after recovery from Late-Life Depression (LLD) and subsequent development of specific dementia kind.
In the first case, chronic and negative daily events affect thoughts and emotions adjunct to environmental, physiologic stressor triggers somatic response in the CNS leading to excessive secretion of neurotoxic corticosteroids. In the second case, it might also be postulated that older persons are especially sensitive to stress since their brains respond by excessive secretion of corticosteroids. Whichever interaction of these causes might be, either one of them (and both combined) may causes neurotoxic damage and abnormalities, thus suggesting a possible direct pathophysiologic link between depression and the neuropathologic hallmarks of AD and VaD. A proposed predominant mechanism by which depression increases risk for dementia is depicted in Figure 7.2. Medical intervention and care management calls for HPA normalization. This will presumably have positive effects in the treatment of depression and the risk for future development of dementia. Cerebrovascular disease might predispose, precipitate, or perpetuate some late-life depressive syndromes. This notion is based on the
208
Dementia
Figure 7.2
Pathways linking depression to dementia.
comorbidity of depressive syndromes with cerebrovascular lesions, cerebrovascular risk factors, and the fact that depression often develops after a stroke. Elderly people with vascular depression have greater disability and cognitive impairment than those who are depressed but do not have vascular stigmata. The vascular depression hypothesis is of clinical importance because drugs used for the prevention of cerebrovascular disease might, for example, reduce the risk for vascular depression. Late-life depressive disorders also often arise in the context of psychosocial adversity. Low economic status, poor physical health, disability, social isolation, and relocation often lead to an adjustment disorder with depressed mood or trigger more severe depressive syndromes than previously present.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
209
Cognitive impairment and disability that accompany late-life depression are becoming the focus of pharmacological and nonpharmacological interventions. The available treatments are as effective for the treatment of depression in elderly individuals as they are in younger adults. However, late-life depression is underrecognized and undertreated. Depression care management strategies may improve the outcomes of elderly individuals who are depressed and who are treated in primarycare settings. This may improve their QoL on the short run but possibly also reduce the development of dementia. REFERENCES Abas, M. A., B. J. Sahakian, and R. Levy. 1990. Neuropsychological deficits and CT scan changes in elderly depressives. Psychol Med 20: 507–520. Abellan van Kan, G., Y. Rolland, F. Nourhashémi, N. Coley, S. Andrieu, and B. Vellas. 2009. Cardiovascular disease risk factors and progression of Alzheimer ’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 27: 240–246. Airaksinen, E., A. Wahlin, M. Larsson, and Y. Forsell. 2006. Cognitive and social functioning in recovery from depression: Results from a population-based three-year follow-up. J Affect Disord 96: 107–110. Alexopoulos, G. S. 2001. “The depression-executive dysfunction syndrome of late life”: A specific target for D3 agonists? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 9: 22–29. Alexopoulos, G. S., K. Buckwalter, J. Olin, R. Martinez, C. Wainscott, and K. R. Krishnan. 2002. Comorbidity of late-life depression: An opportunity for research in mechanisms and treatment. Biol Psychiatry 52: 543–558. Alexopoulos, G. S., I. R. Katz, C. F. Reynolds III, D. Carpenter, and J. P. Docherty. 2001. Pharmacotherapy of depressive disorders in older patients. The Expert Consensus Guideline Series. Postgrad Med (Special Report) (Oct.): 1–86. Alexopoulos, G. S., D. N. Kiosses, M. Heo, C. F. Murphy, B. Shanmugham, and F. Gunning-Dixon. 2005. Executive dysfunction and the course of geriatric depression. Biol Psychiatry 58: 204–210. Alexopoulos, G. S., B. S. Meyers, R. C. Young, S. Campbell, D. Sibersweig, and M. Charlson. 1997. “Vascular depression” hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54: 915–922. Alexopoulos, G. S., B. S. Meyers, R. C. Young, S. Mattis, and T. Kakuma. 1993. The course of geriatric depression with “reversible dementia”: A controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 150: 1693–1699. Alexopoulos, G. S., R. C. Young, and R. Shindledecker. 1992. Brain computed tomography in geriatric depression and primary degenerative dementia. Biol Psychiatry 31: 591–599. Alexopoulos, G. S., D. N. Kiosses, S. Klimstra, B. Kalayam, and B. L. Bruce. 2002. Clinical presentation of the “Depression-Executive Dysfunction Syndrome” of late life. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 10: 98–106.
210
Dementia
Allin, S., C. Masseria, and E. Mossialos. 2009. Measuring socioeconomic differences in use of health care services by wealth versus by income. 2009. Am J Public Health 99: 1849–1855. American Psychiatric Association. 2000. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR). 4th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. Andrews, G., M. Clark, and M. Luszcz. 2002. Successful aging in the Australian longitudinal study of aging: Applying the MacArthur model cross-nationally. J Soc Issues 58: 749–765. Arber, S., and J. Ginn. 1993. Gender and inequalities in health in later life. Soc Sci Med 36: 33–46. Backman, L., B. J. Small, and L. Fratiglioni. 2001. Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit in Alzheimer ’s disease. Brain 124: 96–102. Baltes, M. M. 1998. The psychology of the oldest-old: the fourth age. Curr Opin Psychiatry 11: 411–415. Baltes, M. M., and L. L. Carstensen. 1996. The process of successful aging. Ageing Society 16: 397–422. Baltes, M. M., and F. R. Lang. 1997. Everyday functioning and successful aging: The impact of resources. Psychol Aging 12: 433–443. Baumann, B., H. Bielau, D. Krell, M. W. Agelink, S. Diekmann, C. Wurthmann, et al. 2002. Circumscribed numerical deficit of dorsal raphe neurons in mood disorders. Psychol Med 32: 93–103. Beck, A. T., and R. Steer. 1987. The Beck depression inventory manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation and Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Belmaker, R. H., and G. Agam. 2008. Major depressive disorder. N Engl J Med 358: 55–68. Bhalla, R. K., M. A. Butters, J. T. Becker, P. R. Houck, B. E. Snitz, O. L. Lopez, et al. 2009. Patterns of mild cognitive impairment after treatment of depression in the elderly. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17: 308–316. Blane, D., P. Higgs, M. Hyde, and D. R. Wiggins. 2004. Life course influences on quality of life in early old age. Soc Sci Med 58: 2171–2179. Blane, D., G. Netuveli, and M. S. Montgomery. 2008. Quality of life, health and physiological status and change at older ages. Soc Sci Med 66: 1579–1587. Bond, F., and D. Bunce. 2003. The role of acceptance and job control in mental health, job satisfaction, and work performance. J Appl Psychol 88: 1057–1067. Bondi, M. W., W. S. Houston, D. P. Salmon, J. Corey-Bloom, R. Katzman, L. J. Thal, et al. 2003. Neuropsychological deficits associated with Alzheimer ’s disease in the very old: Discrepancies in raw vs. standardized scores. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 9: 783–795. Boone, K. B., I. M. Lesser, B. L. Miller, M. Wohl, N. Berman, A. Lee, et al. 1995. Cognitive functioning in older depressed outpatients: Relationship of presence and severity of depression to neuropsychological test scores. Neuropsychology 9: 390–398.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
211
Boone, K. B., B. L. Miller, I. M. Lesser, C. M. Merhinger, E. Hill-Gutierrez,. M. Goldberg, et al. 1992. Neuropsychological correlates of white- matter lesions in healthy elderly subjects. A threshold effect. Arch Neurol 49: 549–554. Boustani, M., M. C. Callahan, F. W. Unverzagt, M. G. Austrom, A. J. Perkins, B. A. Fultz, et al. 2005. Implementing a screening and diagnosis program for dementia in primary care. J Gen Intern Med 20: 572–577. Braak, H., and E. Braak. 1991. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 82: 239–259. Brayne, C., L. Gao, M. Dewey, and E. F. Matthews. 2006. Dementia before death in ageing societies—The promise of prevention and the reality. PLoS Med 3: 1922–1930. Breeze, E., A. E. Fletcher, D. A. Leon, M. G. Marmot, R. J. Clarke, and M. J. Shipley. 2001. Do socioeconomic disadvantages persist into old age? Self-reported morbidity in a 29-year follow-up of the Whitehall Study. Am J Public Health 91: 277–283. Bremner, J. D., M. Narayan, E. R. Anderson, L. H. Staib, H. L. Miller, and D. S. Charney. 2000. Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 157: 115–118. Broadley, A. J., A. Korszun, C. J. Jones, and M. P. Frenneaux. 2002. Arterial endothelial function is impaired in treated depression. Heart 88: 521–523. Brown, S. L., M. E. Salive, J. M. Guralnik, M. Pahor, D. P. Chapman, and D. Blazer. 1995. Antidepressant use in the elderly: Association with demographic characteristics, health related factors, and health care utilization. J Clin Epidemiol 48: 445–453. Buntinx, F., A. Kester, J. Bergers, and J. A. Knottnerus. 1996. Is depression in elderly people followed by dementia? A prospective cohort study based in general practice. Age Aging 25: 231–233. Burns, A., and M. Zaudig. 2002. Mild cognitive impairment in older people. Lancet 360: 1963–1965. Butler, R. N. 1969. Age-ism: Another form of bigotry. Gerontologist 9: 243–246. Butters, M. A., J. T. Backer, R. D. Nebes, M. D. Zmuda, B. H. Mulsant, B. G. Pollock, et al. 2000. Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. Am J Psychiatry 157: 1949–1954. Carlson, M. C., M. J. Helms, D. C. Steffens, J. R. Burke, G. G. Potter, and B. L. Plassman. 2008. Midlife activity predicts risk of dementia in older male twin pairs. Alzheimers Dement 4: 324–331. Carney, R. M., K. E. Freedland, R. C. Veith, and A. S. Jaffe. 1999. Can treating depression reduce mortality after an acute myocardial infarction? Psychosom Med 61: 666–675. Chen, C. P., S. L. Eastwood, T. Hope, B. McDonald, P. T. Francis, and M. M. Esiri. 2000. Immunocytochemical study of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease prospectively assessed for behavioural changes. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 26: 347–355.
212
Dementia
Chen, P. S., D. R. McQuoid, M. E. Payne, and D. C. Steffens. 2006. White matter and subcortical gray matter lesion volume changes and late-life depression outcome: A 4-year magnetic resonance imaging study. Int Psychogeriatr 18: 445–456. Cherbuin, N., K. J. Anstey, and D. M. Lipnicki. 2008. Screening for dementia: A review of self- and informant-assessment instruments. Int Psychogeriatr 20: 431–458. Chobanian, A. V., G. L. Bakris, H. R. Black, W. C. Cushman, L. A. Green, J. L. Izzo, et al. 2003. The seventh report of the Joint National Committee on prevention, detection, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure: The JNC 7 report. JAMA 289: 2560–2572. Chodosh, J., D. M. Kado, T. E. Seeman, and A. S. Karlamangla. 2007. Depressive symptoms as predictor of cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Am J Psychiatry 115: 406–415. Coyne, J., R. Thompson, S. Palmer, A. Kagee, and E. Maunsell. 2000. Should we screen for depression? Caveats and potential pitfalls. Appl Prevent Psychol 9: 101–121. Curcio, C. A., and T. Kemper. 1984. Nucleus raphe dorsalis in dementia of the Alzheimer type: Neurofibrillary changes and neuronal packing density. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 43: 359–368. Dahl, E. 1994. Social inequalities in ill-health: The significance of occupationalstatus, education and income—results from a Norwegian survey. Sociol Health Illness 16: 644–667. Davis, N. C., and D. Friedrich. 2004. Knowledge of aging and life satisfaction among older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev 59: 43–61. de Groot, J. C., F. E. de Leeuw, M. Oudkerk, A. Hofman, J. Jolles, and M. Breteler. 2000. Cerebral white matter lesions and depressive symptoms in elderly adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 1071–1076. de Haan, E. H., G. M. Nys, and M. J. Van Zandvoort. 2006. Cognitive function following stroke and vascular cognitive impairment. Curr Opin Neurol 19: 559–564. de Leeuw, F. E., J. C. de Groot, M. Oudkerk, J. C. Witteman, A. Hofman, J. van Gijn, et al. 2002. Hypertension and cerebral white matter lesions in a prospective cohort study. Brain 125 (pt. 4): 765–772. Dean, A., B. Kolody, P. Wood, and G. E. Matt. 1992. The influence of living alone on depression in elderly persons. J Aging Health 4: 3–18. Devanand, D. P., M. Sano, M. X. Tang, S. Taylor, B. J. Gurland, D. Wilder, et al. 1996. Depressed mood and the incidence of Alzheimer ’s disease in the elderly living in the community. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53: 175–182. DiMatteo, M. R., H. S. Lepper, and T. W. Croghan. 2000. Depression is a risk factor for non-compliance with medical treatment. Meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Arch Intern Med 160: 2101–2107. Emery, V. O., and T. E. Oxman. 1992. Update on the dementia spectrum of depression. Am J Psychiatry 149: 305–317.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
213
Fernández Martínez, M., J. Castro Flores, S. Pérez de Las Heras, A. Mandaluniz Lekumberri, M. Gordejuela Menocal, and J. J. Zarranz Imirizaldu. 2008. Risk factors for dementia in the epidemiological study of Munguialde County (Basque Country-Spain). BMC Neurol 8: 39. Ferrari, E., L. Cravello, B. Muzzoni, D. Casarotti, M. Paltro, S. B. Solerte, et al. 2001. Age-related changes of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: Pathophysiological correlates. Eur J Endocrinol 144: 319–329. Filakti, H., and J. Fox. 1995. Differences in mortality by housing tenure and by car access from the OPCS Longitudinal Study. Popul Trends 81: 27–30. Frasure-Smith, N., F. Lesperance, M. Juneau, M. Talajic, and M. G. Bourassa. 1999. Gender, depression, and one-year prognosis after myocardial infarction. Psychosom Med 61: 26–37. Fratiglioni, L., L. J. Launer, K. Andersen, M. M. Breteler, J. R. Copeland, J. F. Dartigues, et al. 2000. Incidence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. Neurology 54 (11 Suppl 5): 10–15. Gallo, J. J., and P. V. Rabins. 1999. Depression without sadness: Alternative presentations of depression in late life. Am Fam Physician 60: 820–826. Gareri, P., P. De Fazio, and G. De Sarro. 2002. Neuropharmacology of depression in aging and age-related diseases. Ageing Res Rev 1: 113–134. Gaupp, R. 1905. Depressive states in old age. Wochenschrift 1531–1537. Godfrey, P. S., B. K. Toone, M. W. Carney, T. G. Flynn, T. Bottiglieri, M. Laundy, et al. 1990. Enhancement of recovery from psychiatric illness by methylfolate. Lancet 336: 392–395. Goldberg, D. 1978. Manual of the General Health Questionnaire. Windsor, England: National Foundation for Educational Research. Goldman, L. S. 1997. Psychiatry in primary care: Possible roles for organized medicine. Psychiatr Ann 27: 1–5. Grau-Veciana, J. M. 2006. Treatment of noncognitive symptoms of Alzheimer ’s disease. Rev Neurol 42: 482–488. Grudzien, A., P. Shaw, S. Weintraub, E. Bigio, D. C. Mash, and M. M. Mesulam. 2007. Locus coeruleus neurofibrillary degeneration in aging, mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 28: 327–335. Grundy, E., and G. Holt. 2001. The socioeconomic status of older adults: How should we measure it in studies of health inequalities? J Epidemiol Community Health 55: 895–904. Gruneir, A., T. F. Smith, J. Hirdes, and R. Cameron. 2005. Depression in patients with advanced illness: An examination of Ontario complex continuing care using the Minimum Data Set 2.0. Palliat Support Care 3: 99–105. Halperin, I., and A. D. Korczyn. 2007. Late-life depression as a risk factor for dementia. Future Neurol 2: 201–208. Halperin, I., and A. D. Korczyn. 2009. Depression and dementia. J Neurol Sci 283: 139–142. Hamilton, M. 1960. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 23: 56–62.
214
Dementia
Hance, M., R. M. Carney, K. E. Freedland, and J. Skala. 1996. Depression in patients with coronary heart disease. A 12-month follow-up. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 18: 61–65. Hanninen, T., M. Hallikainen, S. Tuomainen, M. Vanhanen, and H. Soininen. 2002. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment: A population-based study in elderly subjects. Acta Neurol Scand 106: 148–154. Hayes, S., K. Strosahl, K. Wilson, R. Bissett, J. Pistorello, D. Toarmino, et al. 2004. Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. Psychol Record 54: 553–578. Hedden, T., and J. D. Gabrieli. 2004. Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 87–96. Heun, R., M. Kockler, and A. Papassotiropoulos. 2000. Distinction of early- and late-onset depression in the elderly by their lifetime symptomatology. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15: 1138–1142. Hirono, N., H. Kitagaki, H. Kazui, M. Hashimoto, and E. Mori. 2000. Impact of white matter changes on clinical manifestation of Alzheimer ’s disease: A quantitative study. Stroke 31: 2182–2188. Holsboer, F. 2000. The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 23: 477–501. Jack, C. R., Jr., R. C. Petersen, Y. C. Xu, S. C. Waring, P. C. O’Brien, E. G. Tangalos, et al. 1997. Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 49: 786–794. Jacoby, R. J., and R. Levy. 1980. Computed tomography in the elderly. 3. Affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry 136: 270–275. Jellinger, K. A. 2007. The enigma of vascular cognitive disorder and vascular dementia. Acta Neuropathol 113: 349–388. Jones, J. S., B. Stanley, J. J. Mann, A. J. Frances, J. R. Guido, L. Traskman-Bendz, et al. 1990. CSF 5-HIAA and HVA concentrations in elderly depressed patients who attempted suicide. Am J Psychiatry 147: 1225–1227. Jorm, A. F., C. M. van Duijn, V. Chandra, L. Fratiglioni, A. B. Graves, and A. Heyman. 1991. Psychiatric history and related exposures as risk factors for Alzheimer ’s disease: A collaborative re-analysis of case-control studies. Int J Epidemiol 20 (Suppl 2): 43–47. Kales, H. C., D. F. Maixner, and A. M. Mellow. 2005. Cerebrovascular disease and late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13: 88–99. Kang, S. K., and A. L. Chasteen. 2009. The moderating role of age-group identification and perceived threat on stereotype threat among older adult. Int J Aging Hum Dev 69: 201–220. Kawas, C., S. Gray, R. Brookmeyer, J. Fozard, and A. Zonderman. 2000. Agespecific incidence rates of Alzheimer ’s disease: The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology 54: 2072–2077. Kessing, L. V., and F. M. Nilsson. 2003. Increased risk of developing dementia in patients with major affective disorders compared to patients with other medical illnesses. J Affect Disord 73: 261–269. Kiloh, L. G. 1961. Pseudo-dementia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 37: 336–351.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
215
Korczyn, A., and B. Blum. 1976. The physiological action of serotonin in the brain. Harefuah 90: 120–123. Korczyn, A. D. 1991. The clinical differential diagnosis of dementia: Concept and methodology. Psychiatr Clin North Am 14: 237–249. Korczyn, A. D. 2002. Mixed dementia—the most common cause of dementia. Ann NY Acad Sci 977: 129–134. Korczyn, A. D., and V. Vakhapova. 2007. The prevention of the dementia epidemic. J Neurol Sci 257: 2–4. Kral, V. A., and O. B. Emery. 1989. Long-term follow-up of depressive pseudodementia of the aged. Can J Psychiatry 34: 445–446. Krishnan, K. R., J. C. Hays, and D. G. Blazer. 1997. MRI-defined vascular depression. Am J Psychiatry 154: 497–501. Krishnan, K. R., J. C. Hays, L. A. Tupler, L. K. George, and D. G. Blazer. 1995. Clinical and phenomenological comparisons of late-onset and early-onset depression. Am J Psychiatry 152: 785–788. Krishnan, K. R., L. A. Tupler, J. C. Ritchie Jr., W. M. McDonald, D. L. Knight, C. B. Nemeroff, et al. 1996. Apolipoprotein E-ε4 frequency in geriatric depression. Biol Psychiatry 40: 69–71. Kumar, A., W. Bilker, Z. Jin, and J. Udupa. 2000. Atrophy and high intensity lesions: Complementary neurobiological mechanisms in late life major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 22: 264–274. La Gory, M., and K. Fitpatrick. 1992. The effects of environmental context on elderly depression. J Aging Health 4: 459–479. Lahelma, E., P. Martikainen, O. Rahkonen, and K. Silventoinen. 1999. Gender differences in ill health in Finland: Patterns, magnitude and change. Soc Sci Med 48: 7–19. Lai, T., M. E. Payne, C. E. Byrum, D. C. Steffens, and K. R. Krishnan. 2000. Reduction of orbital frontal cortex volume in geriatric depression. Biol Psychiatry 48: 971–975. Lesser, I., K. B. Boone, C. M. Mehringer, M. A. Wohl, B. L. Miller, and N. G. Berman. 1996. Cognition and white matter hyperintensities in older depressed adults. Am J Psychiatry 153: 1280–1287. Lobo, A., L. J. Launer, L. Fratiglioni, K. Andersen, A. Di Carlo, M. M. Breteler, et al. 2000. Prevalence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. Neurology 54 (11 Suppl. 5): 4–9. Lockwood, K. A., G. S. Alexopoulos, and W. G. Van Gorp. 2002. Executive dysfunction in geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry 159: 1119–1126. Lorentz, W. J., J. M. Scanlan, and S. Borson. 2002. Brief screening tests for dementia. Can J Psychiatry 47: 723–733. Lynch, M. E. 2001. Antidepressants as analgesics: A review of randomized controlled trials. J Psychiatry Neurosci 26: 30–36. Lyness, J. M., M. Heo, C. J. Datto, T. R. Ten Have, I. R. Katz, R. Drayer, et al. 2006. Outcomes of minor and subsyndromal depression among elderly patients in primary care settings. Ann Intern Med 144: 496–504.
216
Dementia
Lyness, J. M., J. L. Pearson, B. D. Lebowitz, and D. J. Kupfer. 1994. Age at onset or late-life depression: A research agenda report of a MacArthur Foundation– NIMH Workshop. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2: 4–8. Lyubomirsky, S., K. Sheldon, and D. Schkade. 2005. Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Rev Gen Psychol 9: 111–131. Lyubomirsky, S. 2001. Why are some people happier than others? The role of cognitive and motivation processes in well-being. Am Psychol 56: 239–249. Maeda, K., K. Tanimoto, T. Terada, T. Shintani, and T. Kakigi. 1991. Elevated urinary free cortisol in patients with dementia. Neurobiol Aging 12: 161–163. Maher, E., and R. A. Cummins. 2001. Subjective quality of life, perceived control and dispositional optimism among older people. Aust J Ageing 20: 139–146. Mann, J. J., V. Arango, P. M. Marzuk, S. Theccanat, and D. J. Reis. 1989. Evidence for the 5-HT hypothesis of suicide. A review of post-mortem studies. Br J Psychiatry (Suppl): 7–14. Martelin, T., S. Koskinen, and T. Valkonen. 1998. Sociodemographic mortality differences among the oldest old in Finland. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 53: 83–90. Masliah, E., M. Mallory, L. A. Hansen, R. DeTeresa, and R. D. Terry. 1993. Quantitative synaptic alterations in the human neocortex during normal aging. Neurology 43: 192–197. McAllister-Williams, R. H., I. N. Ferrier, and A. H. Young. 1998. Mood and neuropsychological function in depression: The role of corticosteroids and serotonin. Psychol Med 28: 573–584. McGaugh, J. L., L. Cahill, and B. Roozendaal. 1996. Involvement of the amygdala in memory storage: Interaction with other brain systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: 13508–13514. McGuire, M. H., and P. V. Rabins. 1994. Mood disorders. In Textbook of geriatric neuropsychiatry, ed. C. E. Coffey and J. L. Cummings, 246–260. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. McMunn, A., E. Breeze, A. Goodman, J. Nazroo, and Z. Oldfield. 2006. Social determinants of health in older age. In Social determinants of health, 2nd ed., ed. M. G. Marmot and R. G. Wilkinson, 267–296. New York: Oxford University Press. Meltzer, C. C., G. Smith, S. T. DeKosky, B. G. Pollock, C. A. Mathis, R. Y. Moore, et al. 1998. Serotonin in aging, late-life depression, and Alzheimer ’s disease: The emerging role of functional imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 18: 407–430. Minicuci, N., and M. Noale. 2005. Influence of level of education on disability free life expectancy by sex: The ILSA study. Exp Gerontol 40: 997–1003. Modrego, P. J., and J. Ferrandez. 2004. Depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment increases the risk of developing dementia of Alzheimer type: A prospective cohort study. Arch Neurol 61: 1290–1293. Morris, P. L., R. G. Robinson, and B. Raphael. 1990. Prevalence and course of depressive disorders in hospitalized stroke patients. Int J Psychiatry Med 20: 349–364.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
217
Mulrow, C. D., J. W. Williams Jr., M. B. Gerety, G. Ramirez, O. M. Montiel, and C. Kerber. 1995. Case-finding instruments for depression in primary care settings. Ann Intern Med 122: 913–921. Musselman, D. L., D. L. Evans, C. B. Nemeroff. 1998. The relationship of depression to cardiovascular disease: Epidemiology, biology, and treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55: 580–592. Netuveli, G., R. D. Wiggins, Z. Hildon, S. M. Montgomery, and D. Blane. 2006. Quality of life at older ages: Evidence from the English longitudinal study of aging (wave 1). J Epidemiol Community Health 60: 357–363. Nilsson, F. M., L. V. Kessing, T. M. Sørensen, P. K. Andersen, and T. G. Bolwig. 2002. Enduring increased risk of developing depression and mania in patients with dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73: 40–44. O’Brien, J., P. Desmond, D. Ames, I. Schweitzer, S. Harrigan, and B. Tress. 1996. A magnetic resonance imaging study of white matter lesions in depression and Alzheimer ’s disease. Br J Psychiatry 168: 477–485. O’Brien, J. T. 1997. The “glucocorticoid cascade” hypothesis in man. Br J Psychiatry 170: 199–201. Oeppen, J., and J. W. Vaupel. 2002. Broken limits to life expectancy. Science 296: 1029–1031. Ohnishi, T., H. Matsuda, T. Tabira, T. Asada, and M. Uno. 2001. Changes in brain morphology in Alzheimer disease and normal aging: Is Alzheimer disease an exaggerated aging process? Am J Neuroradiol 22: 1680–1685. Ongur, D., W. C. Drevets, and J. L. Price. 1998. Glial reduction in the subgenual prefrontal cortex in mood disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 13290–13295. Ordway, G. A., J. Schenk, C. A. Stockmeier, W. May, and V. Klimek. 2003. Elevated agonist binding to alpha2-adrenoceptors in the locus coeruleus in major depression. Biol Psychiatry 53: 315–323. Ownby, R. L., E. Crocco, A. Acevedo, V. John, and D. Loewenstein. 2006. Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 530–538. Parikh, R. M., R. G. Robinson, J. R. Lipsey, S. E. Starkstein, P. Federoff, and T. R. Price. 1990. The impact of poststroke depression on recovery in activities of daily living over a 2-year follow-up. Arch Neurol 47: 785–789. Pfefferbaum, A., D. H. Mathalon, E. V. Sullivan, J. M. Rawles, R. B. Zipursky, and K. O. Lim. 1994. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol 51: 874–887. Potter, G. G., J. D. Kittinger, H. R. Wagner, D. C. Steffens, and K. R. Krishnan. 2004. Prefrontal neuropsychological predictors of treatment remission in late-life depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 2266–2271. Potts, M., M. Burnam, and K. B. Wells. 1991. Gender differences in depression detection: A comparison of clinical diagnosis and standardized assessment. Psychol Assess 3: 609–615.
218
Dementia
Rajkowska, G., L. L. Miguel-Hidalgo, J. Wei, G. Dilley, S. D. Pittman, H. Y. Meltzer, et al. 1999. Morphometric evidence for neuronal and glial prefrontal cell pathology in major depression. Biol Psychiatry 45: 1085–1098. Rao, M. L., B. Hawellek, A. Papassotiropoulos, A. Deister, and C. Frahnert. 1998. Upregulation of the platelet serotonin2A receptor and low blood serotonin in suicidal psychiatric patients. Neuropsychobiology 38: 84–89. Rapp, M. A., M. Schnaider-Beeri, H. T. Grossman, M. Sano, D. P. Perl, D. P. Purohit, et al. 2006. Increased hippocampal plaques and tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease with a lifetime history of major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 161–167. Reinikainen, K. J., H. Soininen, and P. J. Riekkinen. 1990. Neurotransmitter changes in Alzheimer ’s disease: Implications to diagnostics and therapy. J Neurosci Res 27: 576–586. Robert, S. A., D. Cherepanov, M. Palta, N. C. Dunham, D. Feeny, and D. G. Fryback. 2009. Socioeconomic status and age variations in health-related quality of life: Results from the national health measurement study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 64: 378–389. Rothschild, A. J. 1996. The diagnosis and treatment of late-life depression. J Clin Psychiatry 57 (Suppl 5): 5–11. Rovner, B. W., R. J. Casten, and B. E. Leiby. 2009. Variability in depressive symptoms predicts cognitive decline in age-related macular degeneration. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17: 574–581. Rub, U., K. Del Tredici, C. Schultz, D. R. Thal, E. Braak, and H. Braak. 2006. The evolution of Alzheimer ’s disease-related cytoskeletal pathology in the human raphe nuclei. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 26: 553–567. Sapolsky, R. M. 2000. Glucocorticoids and hippocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 925–935. Schulberg, H. C., B. H. Mulsant, R. Schulz, B. L. Rollman, P. R. Houck, C. F. Reynolds III. 1998. Characteristics and course of major depression in older primary care patients. Int J Psychiatry Med 28: 421–436. Sheline, Y. I., M. Sanghavi, M. A. Mintun, and M. H. Gado. 1999. Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume in mentally healthy woman with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci 19: 5034–5043. Shulman, K. I., N. Herrmann, H. Brodaty, H. Chiu, B. Lawlor, K. Ritchie, et al. 2006. IPA survey of brief cognitive assessment instruments. Int Psychogeriatr 18: 281–294. Silventoinen, K., and E. Lahelma. 2002. Health inequalities by education and age in four Nordic countries, 1986 and 1994. J Epidemiol Community Health 56: 253–258. Simpson, S., R. C. Baldwin, A. Jackson, and A. S. Burns. 1998. Is subcortical disease associated with poor response to antidepressants? Neurological, neuropsychological and neuroradiological findings in late-life depression. Psychol Med 28: 1015–1026. Skoog, I., L. Nilsson, B. Palmertz, L. A. Andreasson, and A. Svanborg. 1993. A population-based study of dementia in 85-year-olds. N Eng J Med 328: 153–158.
Aspects in Neuropsychology
219
Small, B. J., L. Fratiglioni, M. Viitanen, B. Winblad, and L. Backman. 2000. The course of cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer disease: Three- and six-year follow-up of a population-based sample. Arch Neurol 57: 839–844. Smith, J. P., and R. Kington. 1997. Demographic and economic correlates of health in old age. Demography 34: 159–170. Sobin, C., and H. A. Sacheim. 1997. Psychomotor symptoms of depression. Am J Psychiatry 154: 4–17. Steffens, D. C., M. C. Norton, A. D. Hart, I. Skoog, C. Corcoran, and J. C. Breitner. 2003. Apolipoprotein E genotype and major depression in a community of older adults. Psychol Med 33: 541–547. Stern, R. A., and D. L. Bachman. 1991. Depressive symptoms following stroke. Am J Psychiatry 148: 351–356. Stock, W. A., M. A. Okun, M. J. Haring, and R. A. Witte. 1983. Age and subjective well-being: A meta analysis. Evaluation study. Rev Ann 8: 279–302. Stockmeier, C. A. 1997. Neurobiology of serotonin in depression and suicide. Ann NY Acad Sci 836: 220–321. Sulander, T., T. Martelin, P. Sainio, O. Rahkonen, A. Nissinen, and A. Uutela. 2006. Trends and educational disparities in functional capacity among people aged 65–84 years. Inter J Epidemiol 35: 1255–1261. Syed, A., M. Chatfield, F. Matthews, P. Harrison, C. Brayne, and M. M. Esiri. 2005. Depression in the elderly: Pathological study of raphe and locus ceruleus. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 31: 405–413. Taylor, W. D., H. R. Wagner, and D. C. Steffens. 2002. Greater depression severity associated with less improvement in depression-associated cognitive deficits in older subjects. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 10: 632–635. Thomas, A. J., R. N. Kalaria, and J. T. O’Brien. 2004. Depression and vascular disease: What is the relationship? J Affect Disord 79: 81–95. Tomlinson, B. E., G. Blessed, and M. Roth. 1970. Observations on the brains of demented old people. J Neurol Sci 11: 205–242. Ueda, K., H. Kawano, Y. Hasuo, and M. Fujishima. 1992. Prevalence and etiology of dementia in a Japanese community. Stroke 23: 798–803. Unutzer, J., D. L. Patrick, G. Simon, D. Grembowski, E. Walker, C. Rutter, et al. 1997. Depressive symptoms and the cost of health services in HMO patients aged 65 years and older: A 4-year prospective study. JAMA 277: 1618–1623. Van de Pol, L. A., A. Hensel, F. Barkhof, H. J. Gertz, P. Scheltens, W. M. van der Flier. 2006. Hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer disease: Age matters. Neurology 66: 236–238. Verbrugge, L. M. 1989. The twain meet: Empirical explanations of sex-differences in health and mortality. J Health Soc Behav 30: 282–304. Von Faber, M., A. Bootsma–van der Wiel, E. Van Exel, J. Gussekloo, A. M. Lagaay, E. Van Dongen, et al. 2001. Successful aging in the oldest old: Who can be characterized as successfully aged? Arch Intern Med 161: 2694–2700. Weiland-Fiedler, P., K. Erickson, T. Waldeck, D. A. Luckenbaugh, D. Pike, O. Bonne, et al. 2004. Evidence of continuing neuropsychological impairments in depression. J Affect Disord 82: 253–258.
220
Dementia
Weytingh, M. D., P. M. Bossuyt, and H. van Crevel. 1995. Reversible dementia: More than 10% or less than 1%? A quantitative review. J Neurol 242: 466–471. WHOQOL Group. 1995. The World Health Organization Quality of Life assessment (WHOQOL): Position paper from the World Health Organization. Soc Sci Med 41: 1403–1409. Wilson, R. S., J. A. Schneider, J. L. Bienias, S. E. Arnold, D. A. Evans, and D. A. Bennett. 2003. Depressive symptoms, clinical AD, and cortical plaques and tangles in older persons. Neurology 61: 1102–1107. Winkleby, M. A., D. E. Jatulis, E. Frank, and S. P. Fortmann. 1992. Socioeconomicstatus and health—how education, income, and occupation contribute to risk-factors for cardiovascular-disease. Am J Public Health 82: 816–820. Wolf, O. T., I. Dziobek, P. McHugh, V. Sweat, M. J. de Leon, E. Javier, et al. 2005. Subjective memory complaints in aging are associated with elevated cortisol levels. Neurobiol Aging 26: 1357–1363. Yesavage, J. A., T. L. Brink, T. L. Rose, O. Lum, V. Huang, M. Adey, et al. 1983. Development and validation of a geriatric depression scale: A preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 17: 37–49. Youdim, M. B. 2006. The path from anti Parkinson drug selegiline and rasagiline to multifunctional neuroprotective anti Alzheimer drugs ladostigil and m30. Curr Alzheimer Res 3: 541–550. Zubenko, G. S., J. Mossy, and U. Koop. 1990. Neurochemical correlates of major depression in primary dementia. Arch Neurol 47: 209–214. Zung, W. W. 1965. A self rating depression rating scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 12: 63–70.
Chapter 8
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease Erica Harris
Apathy is often defined simply as a general loss of motivation. Marin (1996) added to this definition a reduction in goal-directed behavior and thought with few displays of emotion. Although a consensus within the literature has not been reached regarding whether apathy is a neuropsychiatric syndrome or if it is a syndrome by itself, this will not be covered in this chapter. The reader is referred to previous debates of this topic (Starkstein and Leentjens 2008; Marin 1996). Patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) are not the only clinical group that manifests apathy. Other groups include stroke patients, Alzheimer ’s disease patients, patients with dementia, major depression (Marin, Biedrzycki, and Firinciogullari 1991), and other disorders involving the basal ganglia such as parkinsonism (Sockeel et al. 2006). The prevalence of apathy in patients with PD is found to vary between 16.5% (Aarsland et al. 1999) to 45% (Isella et al. 2002), with one study even reporting apathy in as many as 51% of their sample of patients with PD (Kirsch-Darrow et al. 2006). In addition, apathy has been found to be the most distressing neuropsychiatric problem to caregivers (Leiknes et al. 2010). In this chapter, we will review the neurobiology of PD. Then we will provide a literature review about studies that have previously been conducted to evaluate apathy in PD and relate apathy to the development of dementia. We will then describe three subtypes of apathy that have been linked to basal ganglia and fronto-subcortical circuits and how a patient
222
Dementia
with PD would present with the respective type of apathy. Is one subtype of apathy predictive of progressive dementia over another? Finally, we will explore how these patients’ quality of life can be increased through better diagnosis and treatment. NONMOTOR SYMPTOMS OF PARKINSON’S DISEASE WITH A FOCUS ON APATHY Parkinson’s disease (PD) is classically regarded as a degenerative movement disorder of unknown etiology due to a loss of dopaminergic neurons in the basal ganglia, particularly within an area known as the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc). The basal ganglia are a set of structures within the midbrain that are responsible for learning and coordinating movement such as swinging a baseball bat, walking, initiating, maintaining, or carrying out a particular behavior. The loss of dopamine (DA) within the SNc contributes to a variety of motor and nonmotor problems that are associated with PD. The gamut of such nonmotor problems include changes in personality (Poewe et al. 1983; Menza et al. 1993; Menza 2000; McNamara, Durso, and Harris 2008), sleep disorders (Chaudhuri et al. 2002; Lauterbach 2007; Stavitsky et al. 2008), pain (Chaudhuri, Healy, and Schapira 2005), falls (Chaudhuri et al. 2005), anosmia (Cramer, Friedman, and Amick 2010), impulse control disorders related to gambling, sexual paraphilias, buying addictions, or binge eating (Ferrara and Stacy 2008; Lim, Evans, and Miyasaki 2008; Wolters, van der Werf, and van den Heuvel 2008), deficits in executive cognitive dysfunction (McNamara, Durso, and Brown 2003), and mood changes including depression (Lew 2007) and apathy (Lew 2007; Simuni and Sethi 2008; Aarsland, Marsh, and Schrag 2009). More than likely, some of these nonmotor symptoms are present before the actual display of motor symptoms (Siderowf and Stern 2008; Tolosa et al. 2009). In this chapter, we will focus on apathy. Clinicians and neurologists are taking the nonmotor symptoms of PD, including apathy, more seriously since the revised Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS; Goetz et al. 2008), part 1, which addresses nonmotor aspects of daily living in an interview format between the clinician and the patient. One of the questions is about apathy and assesses if the patient feels indifferent to doing activities or to being with people. The clinician and patient decide what the best response is for the patient, 0 indicating there have been no changes in the last week to 4, there is a complete loss of initiative and the patient is passive and withdrawn. Kirsch-Darrow et al. (2009) evaluated this new apathy question on the UPDRS against the Apathy Scale (AS)
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
223
used by Starkstein et al. (1992). Kirsch-Darrow and colleagues found that this one apathy item has low specificity if subjects indicate a 1 and it has low sensitivity with a cut-off of 2 or better. As a result, they recommend that users of this apathy screener question exercise prudence when evaluating patients as there is a high probability of missing patients who need to be managed for clinical apathy. Apathy mimics some of the symptoms of depression but can be differentiated from depression (Marin 1996). Approximately 40% of PD patients evidence some degrees of a depression (Cummings 1992; Cummings and Masterman 1999). Depression in patients with PD is different from depression in psychiatric populations because patients with PD have greater anxiety and less negative self-reflecting, or self-ideation, fewer feelings of guilt, and less suicidal ideation. Moreover, patients with lower levels of 5-HIAA (5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, a metabolite of serotonin), a past history of depression, and more functional disability usually have a greater risk for developing depression in PD. Being female, having an early age of onset of PD, and greater left-brain involvement may all be risk factors for depression in PD as well (Cummings and Masterman 1999). What is more, those patients who exhibit more bradykinesia and gait instability than tremor are more likely to develop depression (Cummings 1992) whereas the apathy studies have reported there is more tremor and akinesia as the predominant motor symptoms in patients with PD (Kirsch-Darrow et al. 2006). In addition, depressed patients with PD have greater frontal-lobe dysfunction and greater involvement of dopaminergic and noradrenergic systems than nondepressed patients (Cummings 1992; Starkstein et al. 1992). NEUROBIOLOGY OF PARKINSON’S DISEASE In the initial stages of PD, the loss of dopamine within the brain is chiefly localized to either the left or right hemisphere. Depending on which hemisphere has a greater depletion of dopamine, the display of motor symptoms is on the contralateral side of the body (Amick, Grace, and Chou 2006; Djaldetti, Ziv, and Melamed 2006). For example, if there is a greater depletion of dopamine in the left hemisphere, then tremors may first begin in the right hand and vice versa. There are two major dopaminergic pathways within the brain that are affected in PD. These include the nigrostriatal—or movement—pathway and the mesolimbic—or reward— pathway. The nigrostriatal pathway sends dopamine from the substantia nigra to the basal ganglia (Björklund and Lindvall 1975) and the mesolimbic pathway sends dopamine from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to
224
Dementia
the striatum, the limbic system, and the frontal cortex (Lindvall et al. 1974; Swanson 1982). Apathy can result from lesions to the basal ganglia and to these fronto-subcortical circuits (Levy and Dubois 2006; Dujardin et al. et al. 2007). Understanding the connections and projections of the dopaminergic neurotransmitter system will help us to learn how these neuroanatomical areas can be disrupted and produce apathy. PREVIOUS RESEARCH ON APATHY IN PARKINSON’S DISEASE As described above, apathy is a behavioral syndrome that is the result of a loss of motivation and incentives, a lack of emotion, and no exhibition of goal-directed behavior or thought, with apathy not being attributable to a loss of consciousness (Marin 1996). Levy and Dubois (2006) extend this definition by stating that apathy is the result of a loss of voluntary and intentional behavior on behalf of the self; this behavior can be measured and observed. What are the signs of a patient who is apathetic? This is the patient who does not comply with medication regimens, who says he is going to show up for appointments but does not, and who, in general, just does not do what he is asked to do (Marin 1996). In addition, patients who display apathy are more likely to not complete questionnaires about themselves and exhibit negative personality traits. These patients may make statements such as “I do not care about being around people” or “I do not have a desire to learn anything new.” Several types of scales have been developed to measure apathy, but none have been established as the gold standard (van Reekum, Stuss, and Ostrander 2005). One such scale is the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES; Marin 1996) with a patient and an informant version. There are four subscales in addition to a Global Severity Score: Cognitive, Behavior, Emotional, and Other. The AES has been validated in Alzheimer ’s disease patients, right hemisphere stroke patients, patients with major depression, and healthy elderly controls, with internal consistency reliability ranging from 0.86 to 0.94 and test-retest reliability fluctuating between 0.76 and 0.94. The convergent validity coefficients range from 0.43 to 0.72 (Marin, Biedrzycki, and Firinciogullari 1991). Starkstein et al. (1992) evaluated a 14-item version of the Apathy Scale (a brief form of the AES) in patients with Parkinson’s disease. They found interrater reliability to be r = 0.81 and internal validity to be alpha = 0.76. Another scale that has been used to measure apathy is the Lille Apathy Rating Scale (LARS; Sockeel et al. 2006). The LARS focuses on four of the core features of apathy including intellectual curiosity, self-awareness, emotion, and action initiation. Between-items reliability was established to be 0.80 whereas between-subscales reliability
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
225
was 0.74. The test-retest coefficient was found to be 0.95 with split-half reliability at 0.84. Concurrent validity was r = 0.87 (comparing the global scores of the AES and the LARS). Moreover, the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI; Cummings 1997) is a tool that is used to measure neuropsychiatric behavioral changes in patients with dementia, but there is only one question that assesses global apathy. And finally, Robert et al. (2002) developed the Apathy Inventory (AI) specifically for those patient groups who have suffered brain damage. Like the NPI, it can be used with a caregiver or next-of-kin. The AI is a global measure of apathy that consists of patient and caregiver versions to measure the core features of apathy: lack of emotion, no initiative, and a loss of interest in previous activities and people. Internal consistency for reliability was found to be alpha = 0.84, but this applies only to the caregiver version of the AI. Interrater reliability was high with Kappa coefficient = 0.99. It has not yet been established whether apathy is related to a stage of disease or to greater motor impairment. There also is not a consensus on whether there is an overall decline in Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores and general cognitive functioning in patients with PD who suffer from apathy (Levy et al. 1998; Pluck and Brown 2002; Starkstein et al. 1992; Starkstein and Leentjens 2008). On the other hand, some studies have shown that degrees of apathy predict risk for dementia, but these studies have not always specified exact subtypes of apathy. Below is a review of apathy studies previously conducted with patients with PD. Starkstein et al. (1992) assessed apathy in a group of 50 patients with idiopathic PD using the Apathy Scale (AS). They found that only 12% of their sample exhibited apathy without depression whereas 30% had both apathy and depression; patients with apathy exhibited greater difficulty with timed neuropsychological tests and verbal memory tests. Pedersen and colleagues (2009) conducted a longitudinal study with a communitybased sample of 139 patients with PD in western Norway. They found that patients with apathy were demented at baseline and often at follow-up (79 patients of the original 139 patients were reassessed four years later). At follow-up, 29.1% of their sample was demented with 63.6% of patients displaying apathy to also be demented. They concluded that apathy may be a result of the natural, biological course of PD for those patients with more advanced disease but that the emergence of dementia may be related to the nondopaminergic circuits and the fact that the patient sample was older. Pluck and Brown (2002) evaluated apathy in 45 PD patients and in a control group of 17 patients with osteoarthritis. They found that the patients with PD had higher levels of apathy. Those patients with PD
226
Dementia
who were classified as having high apathy performed worse on verbal fluency and the Stroop test than those who scored low on apathy. They hypothesized that those patients, 7.1% of the sample, could possibly be demented since they did so poor on the cognitive battery. Cognitive impairment was also found to be related to apathy, but disease progression or stage of disease was found to be unrelated to levels of apathy, possibly indicating a subtype of apathy. Furthermore, apathy was not related to anxiety scores or to depression and was not predicted by stage of disease or duration of disease. Kirsch-Darrow et al. (2006) investigated apathy in 80 idiopathic patients with PD and in 20 patients with adult-onset dystonia using the short version of the AES. They found that 51% of patients with PD experienced apathy with almost 29% experiencing apathy and depression together. None of the patients with dystonia experienced apathy. They also found a small positive correlation between age and apathy score, which previous studies have not. They concluded that apathy is more than likely a defining feature of PD and that apathy is not a symptom of depression and occurs independently of depression since the anterior cingulate-mesial frontal cortex is more damaged in PD. Finally, Cramer, Friedman, and Amick (2010) examined olfaction in apathy in patients with PD by administering the AES. They found that greater apathy indicated greater olfactory impairment. Their reasoning was that smell is intricately tied to emotions and since olfaction areas are anatomically close to limbic areas within the brain, studying the overlap of these areas is important to understanding how apathy can be a part of the physiological changes that emerge during the course of PD. They further recommend that more research be tailored to deciphering the link between dementia and motor-symptom severity to be sure apathy scores or olfactory test scores are not influenced by these factors. Sockeel et al. (2006) developed the Lille Apathy Rating Scale (LARS). They found that this new measure is a useful tool in separating apathy from depression in patients with PD. Of 159 patients with probable PD, 32.1% were found to have apathy in a study conducted by Dujardin et al. (2007) to validate the LARS using the same patient sample from the Sockeel group. Dujardin and colleagues found that having a low level of cognitive functioning was the main contributing factor to more severe apathy. Motor-symptom severity did not contribute to apathy, and the occurrence of apathy is not dependent on depression. Overall, the LARS was reliable in differentiating various types of apathy among PD patients, the types of apathy including none, slight, moderate, and severe. Dujardin et al. (2009) examined dementia in 20 apathetic patients with PD and
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
227
in 20 nonapathetic patients with PD using the LARS. They administered various tests of executive function including the Stroop, word generation, a letter and number sequencing test, and the oral version of the Symbol Digit Modalities test. Apathetic patients with PD performed much more poorly on this cognitive battery with reduced response inhibition and action initiation. At 18 months follow-up, dementia was much more common in patients who were apathetic, suggesting that apathy can predict later dementia. Only one case of dementia was seen at follow-up in the nonapathetic group. Dujardin et al. (2009) allude to a subtype of apathy known as cognitive-affective apathy (Levy and Dubois 2006) that is present in this sample of patients. More about this subtype of apathy is explained below. Levy et al. (1998) used the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) to assess apathy in PD patients. They found that depression and apathy were uncorrelated. PD patients had greater cognitive dysfunction as evidenced by the MMSE and this was correlated with apathy. They also had greater levels of depression. Oguru et al. (2010) found in their study that 60% of Japanese patients with PD exhibited apathy and 56% of these patients exhibited depression. Of their sample, 43% displayed apathy and depression together, and 8% displayed dementia. After removing patients with depression and dementia, they found that only 15% of their sample displayed apathy as the sole behavioral disorder. They concluded that it is possible that apathy can occur independent of depression in PD. Those patients with apathy had higher levels of depression and worse cognitive functioning. Oguru and colleagues hypothesize as did Kirsch-Darrow et al. (2006) that impairment to the mesial frontal-anterior cingulate circuits underlie the production of apathy. Pedersen et al. (2010) administered the NPI to a group of communitydwelling PD patients who had never begun taking medicine for their PD. Of their sample, 25 patients were diagnosed solely with apathy whereas 14 patients were diagnosed with depression and apathy together. Apathy in patients with PD was significantly associated with male gender, worse motor scores, high depression, and impairments in attention/executive deficits. No correlation was found between apathy and increased cognitive impairment as has been found in other studies, and no controls were diagnosed with apathy. Newly diagnosed PD patients who had never taken medicine and had apathy indicated that their apathy was more than likely the result of a neurobiological change. Aarsland et al. (1999) found that 4.3% of patients with untreated PD who were assessed with the NPI had apathy and no depression whereas Pedersen et al. (2010) found that 8.3% of untreated patients with PD did.
228
Dementia
Finally, Robert et al. (2002) used the AI to rate apathy in Alzheimer ’s disease patients, mild cognitive impairment patients, and patients with PD. They found that the patients with PD rated their global apathy higher than the other two neurological groups and showed that they were aware of their apathy. This was not seen in the nonapathetic patient group. Robert et al. (2002) recommend that future research try to unravel the cognitive, behavioral, and emotional components of apathy. In summary, it is inconclusive from this previous research if apathy is a result solely of the disability of PD or if it is a neurobiological problem resulting from PD. Most of the researchers conclude that apathy is a multifactorial neuropsychiatric problem and is probably a result of the underlying neurobiology in PD. Apathy, furthermore, appears to be a strong predictor for development of a later dementing process. However, PD is a complicated neurodegenerative disease. There are different ages of onset of disease with different disease courses. Even diagnosing the disease can be difficult with up to 25% of patients being misdiagnosed (Tolosa, Wenning, and Poewe 2006). In addition, it is impossible gauge how much loss of dopamine has occurred in the brainstem and progressed to the midbrain and to the cortex. As a result, even though we have these data on apathy, research groups are still trying to establish the best method to measure apathy in addition to consistently detailing the specific subtypes of apathy we would see in patients with PD based on the type of lesion in the basal ganglia and fronto-subcortical circuits and resulting cognitive batteries. SUBTYPES OF APATHY IN PATIENTS WITH PARKINSON’S DISEASE Levy and Dubois (2006) discuss three models of apathy (based on Stuss, van Reekum, and Murphy 2000) including emotional-affective, cognitive-affective and auto-activation deficit, that are linked with profiles of dysfunction within the basal ganglia and the frontal lobes. Even though these specific subtypes of apathy are described, the occurrence of dementia along with the presentation of apathy in patients with PD is not uniform (Starkstein et al. 1992; Levy and Dubois 2006; Pluck and Brown 2002). However, these models can serve as heuristics to evaluate apathy while dementia is concurrently assessed not only in patients with PD but in other patient populations as well. We revisit these models of apathy for two purposes: (1) to redirect the research community to these subtypes, and (2) to begin thinking of ways by which these subtypes of apathy are expressed in patients with PD.
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
229
Emotional-Affective Apathy Emotional-affective apathy is linked with dysfunction in the orbitalmedial prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the limbic regions of the basal ganglia including the ventral striatum and ventral pallidum (Levy and Dubois 2006). Lesions to the orbital-medial PFC often result in emotional blunting, poor judgment related to actions and consequences, and the performance of actions based on feelings and emotions alone. Robert et al. (2002) report that in their study that mild cognitively impaired (MCI) patients differed on the lack of initiative domain, but this could not be adequately investigated due to the small sample size. They suggest that future studies should investigate whether this domain is indicative of a progressive change toward dementia. In patients with PD who exhibit this type of apathy, we would see the emergence of impulsive behaviors and a reduction in self-initiated behaviors, possibly increased novelty seeking and a lack of response to rewarding behaviors. There would be a lack of disregard for any consequences of behavior. Many of these types of behaviors are seen after these patients have been medicated with dopamine agonists over a long period of time (Weintraub 2008). The neurotransmitter dopamine responds to reward, but these patients are less likely to recognize the reward; therefore, there is no recognition of a rewarding and beneficial outcome (Daw and Shohamy 2008). When we are surprised by a new outcome and our dopamine receptors respond, it is considered that we have learned from this response. Daw and Shohamy (2008) argue that the striatum is where this learning occurs in the basal ganglia. One type of test that can evaluate this form of apathy is the Gambling Task developed by Bachara (Levy and Dubois 2006). This form of apathy is difficult for caregivers to manage because these patients may perform activities that can be quite embarrassing to the family. In addition, there is also a loss of interaction with the family on behalf of the patient. Activities of daily living such as maintaining good hygiene are also neglected.
Cognitive-Affective Apathy Cognitive-affective apathy involves lesions of the dorsolateral PFC and the dorsal caudate nucleus, the internal portion of the globus pallidus, the lateral substantia nigra pars reticulata, and the anterior thalamic nuclei (Levy and Dubois 2006). Patients with PD who suffer from this type of apathy perform poorly on tests of executive cognitive functioning and planning including the Stroop, Trails, categorical verbal fluency, the Tower
230
Dementia
of London, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Levy and Dubois 2006). Each of these tasks requires the patient to plan, to establish a set of rules, to maintain the set of rules, and then be able to shift the rules to exhibit cognitive flexibility. These patients also have difficulties with working memory in that they forget the mini goals they are working towards in these various tasks (Levy and Dubois 2006). An as example, patients with PD who have this type of apathy are those who more than likely exhibit the greatest deficits on the Tower of London (TOL) planning task and who become frustrated because they cannot do it. From personal testing experience, some apathetic patients with PD have not gotten farther than part five of nine of the TOL task and do extremely poor on word-generation tasks. Much of the research as discussed in the literature review indicates that this subtype of apathy is common. Auto-Activation Deficit Auto-activation deficit is the most debilitating form of apathy. The lesions are located in the medial PFC and may be large frontal lesions or white-matter lesions and in the cognitive and limbic areas of the basal ganglia, particularly in the internal portion of the globus pallidus (Levy and Dubois 2006). In this form of apathy, whatever information is gleaned from the external environment does not elicit any behavior from the basal ganglia to reach the output systems. The basal ganglia and frontal lobes need input to communicate with each other. When this input cannot reach the desired brain areas, such as the pallidum, there is no feedback loop providing information to the frontal lobes about what behavior to perform. Patients with this type of apathy remain motionless and respond to needing a “little” push each day or having someone tell them what to do. If they are given some form of external environmental stimuli, they can move and carry out behaviors for a short period of time, but this is not sustained (Levy and Dubois 2006). As a result, there is no voluntary action that is carried out by the patient. Patients with PD who exhibit this type of apathy would exhibit all of these symptoms. It will be especially important to identify those patients who suffer from this type of apathy early, with treatment interventions aimed towards the patient and the caregiver. SUGGESTIONS FOR TREATMENT AND FUTURE RESEARCH Any treatments for apathy thus far seem to be targeted only to global apathy and not to any specific subtype of apathy. Dopaminergic agents such as bromocriptine and amantadine, in addition to amphetamines,
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
231
atypical antipsychotics, and acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, might be beneficial for some patients with apathy. However, which drug is effective for which type of apathy has yet to be described (van Reekum, Stuss, and Ostrander 2005). It could be possible that one type of medication may not be suitable for emotional-affective apathy but is ideal for cognitive-affective apathy and vice versa. As Stuss, van Reekum, and Murphy (2000) point out, until we have a better grasp as to the underlying neurobiological and neurochemical manifestations of apathy, any pharmacological or behavioral therapy will not be useful. As several authors recommend (Kirsch-Darrow et al. 2006, 2009; Starkstein and Leentjens 2008), better measurement and methodologies of studying apathy can assist pharmaceutical companies in developing better medications to assist with the problems associated with apathy including motivation and emotion. What are the risk factors for apathy? How can we better predict those patients who will develop apathy versus those who do not? As we have discussed above, however, there are various subtypes of apathy that patients may endure. It is problematic that there are no specific treatments tailored toward these individual subtypes of apathy. Since the definition of apathy is still not ideally defined, what we may be categorizing as apathy within PD may be something entirely different (Starkstein and Leentjens 2008). However, we need to find other avenues of treatment that are nonpharmacologic, especially for patients with PD since they are already taking very detailed medication regimens. One of the first steps is for clinicians to become better educated about the signs and symptoms of apathy (van Reekum, Stuss, and Ostrander 2005). Furthermore, educating caregivers and patients that apathy is a symptom of PD will help them to be better prepared in the event the patient becomes apathetic (Kirsch-Darrow et al. 2006). Better education can then possibly lead to rehabilitative programs of which there currently are none specifically tailored to patients with PD and apathy. Stuss et al. (2000) report on a study that attempted therapy for apathetic patients who were not patients with PD. Kopelowicz and colleagues (1997) enrolled a total of six subjects with schizophrenia, three with and without deficit syndrome. In other words, they exhibited the amotivational syndrome that is typically found in schizophrenia. The authors implemented a social-skills training program that lasted for 12 weeks. The schizophrenic patients who did not have the deficit syndrome improved, but those with the deficit syndrome were not influenced by the social-skills training. Other types of rehabilitation that were discussed refer to teaching compensatory strategies and training executive cognitive functions (Stuss, van Reekum, and Murphy 2000). The paucity of research on rehabilitation of patients with
232
Dementia
apathy indicates that better emotional and cognitive therapies need to be developed (Aarsland et al. 1999). In the end, though, it will take dedicated caregivers and clinicians to work with these patients. One factor not addressed in current studies of apathy in patients with PD is the impact of PD subtype on apathy. Patients with left-onset disease are known to significantly differ along an array of clinical variables from PD patients with right-onset disease. For example, there are differences in language production among left-versus right-onset PD patients. Could the language production among these patients be linked to specific types of apathy (Pluck and Brown 2002)? What type of apathy is seen in patients with left-onset versus right-onset PD? Knowing this information may tell us something special about the various forms of apathy that are seen in these patients. More than likely, there is a different neurobiology and neurochemical basis depending upon the side of onset exhibiting a different presentation of apathy. Additionally, although it is clear that apathy is a strong predictor of later dementia, it is uncertain as to whether any particular subtype of apathy more strongly predicts dementia than the other subtypes of apathy. Researchers investigating apathy in patients with PD should report their data with respect to side of onset to provide additional information by which the tangled web of apathy can be addressed. Although not all patients with PD will have apathy, this neuropsychiatric symptom is not to be ignored. As Cummings (1997) indicates, those patients who suffer from dementia may be a subgroup with neurobiological changes occurring at a different rate than those without dementia. This equally applies to our patients with PD who have apathy. They are a special subgroup of patients with PD who need to be treated as such. These patients are an interesting group to study to learn more about how such symptoms can develop from a neurodegenerative disease and can help us eventually find better ways to treat and to manage apathy in patients with PD and to possibly predict whether apathy definitely predicts later dementia. REFERENCES Aarsland, D., J. P. Larsen, N. G. Lim, C. Janvin, K. Karlsen, E. Tandberg, and J. L. Cummings. 1999. Range of neuropsychiatric disturbances in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 67: 492–496. Aarsland, D., L. Marsh, and A. Schrag. 2009. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 24 (15): 2175–2186.
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
233
Amick, M. M., J. Grace, and K. L. Chou. 2006. Body side of motor symptom onset in Parkinson’s disease is associated with memory performance. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 12: 736–440. Björklund, A., and O. Lindvall. 1975. Dopamine in dendrites of substantia nigra neurons: Suggestions for a role in dendritic terminals. Brain Research 83: 531–537. Chaudhuri, K. R., D. G. Healy, and A. H. V. Schapira. 2005. Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: Diagnosis and management. The Lancet Neurology 5 (3): 235–245. Chaudhuri, K. R., S. Pal, A. DiMarco, C. Whatley-Smith, K. Bridgman, R. Mathew, F. R. Pezzela, B. Hogl, and C. Trenkwalder. 2002. The Parkinson’s disease sleep scale: A new instrument for assessing sleep and nocturnal disability in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 73: 629–635. Cramer, C. K., J. H. Friedman, and M. M. Amick. 2010. Olfaction and apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 16: 124–126. Cummings, J. L. 1992. Depression and Parkinson’s disease: A review. American Journal of Psychiatry 149 (4): 443–454. Cummings, J. L. 1997. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Assessing psychopathology in dementia patients. Neurology 48 (Suppl. 6): S10–16. Cummings, J. L., and D. L. Masterman. 1999. Depression in patients with Parkinson’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14: 711–718. Daw, N. D. and D. Shohamy. 2008. The cognitive neuroscience of motivation and learning. Social Cognition 26: 593–620. Djaldetti, R., I. Ziv, and E. Melamed. 2006. The mystery of motor asymmetry in Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurology 5: 796–802. Dujardin, K., P. Sockeel, M. Delliaux, A. Destée, and L. Defebvre. 2009. Apathy may herald cognitive decline and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 24 (16): 2391–2397. Dujardin, K., P. Sockeel, D. Devos, M. Delliaux, P. Krystkowiak, A. Destée, and L. Defebvre. 2007. Characteristics of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 22 (6): 778–784. Ferrara, J. M., and M. Stacy. 2008. Impulse-control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. CNS Spectrums 13: 690–698. Goetz, C. G., B. C. Tilley, S. R. Shaftman, G. T. Stebbins, S. Fahn, P. Martinez-Martin, W. Poewe, et al. 2008. Movement Disorder Society–sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): Scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Movement Disorders 23 (15): 2129–2170. Isella, V., P. Melzi, M. Grimaldi, S. Iurlaro, R. Pioliti, C. Ferrarese, L. Frattola, and I. Appollonio. 2002. Clinical, neuropsychological and morphometric correlates of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 17: 366–371. Kirsch-Darrow, L., H. F. Fernandez, M. Marsiske, M. S. Okun, and D. Bowers. 2006. Dissociating apathy and depression in Parkinson disease. Neurology 67: 33–38.
234
Dementia
Kirsch-Darrow, L., L. B. Zahodne, C. Hass, A. Mikos, M. S. Okun, H. H. Fernandez, and D. Bowers. 2009. How cautious should we be when assessing apathy with the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale? Movement Disorders 24 (5): 684–688. Kopelowicz, A., R. P. Liberman, J. Mintz, and R. Zarate. 1997. Comparison of efficacy of social skills training for deficit and nondeficit negative symptoms in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 154 (3): 424–425. Lauterbach, E. C. 2007. The neuropsychiatry of Parkinson’s disease. Minerva Medica 96 (3): 155–173. Leiknes, I., O-B. Tysnes, D. Aarsland, and J. P. Larsen. 2010. Caregiver distress associated with neuropsychiatric problems in patients with early Parkinson’s disease: The Norwegian ParkWest study. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 122 (6): 418–424. Levy, M. L., J. L. Cummings, L. A. Fairbanks, D. Masterman, B. L. Miller, A. H. Craig, J. S. Paulsen, and I. Litvan. 1998. Apathy is not depression. Journal of Neuropsychiatry 10 (3): 314–319. Levy, R., and B. Dubois. 2006. Apathy and the functional anatomy of the prefrontal cortex-basal ganglia circuits. Cerebral Cortex 16: 916–928. Lew M. 2007. Overview of Parkinson’s disease. Pharmacotherapy 27: 155S–60S. Lim, S.-Y., A. H. Evans, and J. M. Miyasaki. 2008. Impulse control and related disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1142: 85–107. Lindvall, O., A. Björklund, R. Y. Moore, and U. Stenevi. 1974. Mesencephalic dopamine neurons projecting to neocortex. Brain Research 81: 325–331. Marin, R. S. 1996. Apathy: Concept, syndrome, neural mechanisms, and treatment. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry 1 (4): 304–314. Marin, R. S., R. C. Biedrzycki, and S. Firinciogullari. 1991. Reliability and validity of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psychiatry Research 38: 143–162. McNamara, P., R. Durso, and A. Brown. 2003. Relation of “sense of self” to executive function performance in Parkinson’s disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 16: 139–148. McNamara, P., R. Durso, and E. Harris. 2008. Alterations of the sense of self and personality in Parkinson’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23: 79–84. Menza, M. 2000. The personality associated with Parkinson’s disease. Current Psychiatry Reports 2: 421–426. Menza, M. A., L. I. Golbe, R. A. Cody, and N. E. Forman. 1993. Dopamine-related personality traits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 43: 505–508. Oguru, M., H. Tachibana, K. Toda, B. Okuda, and N. Oka. 2010. Apathy and depression in Parkinson disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 23 (1): 35–41. Pedersen, K. F., G. Alves, D. Aarsland, and J. P. Larsen. 2009. Occurrence and risk factors for apathy in Parkinson disease: A 4-year prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 80: 1279–1282.
Relation of Apathy to Dementia in Patients
235
Pedersen, K. F., G. Alves, K. Brønnick, D. Aarsland, O-B. Tysnes, and J. P. Larsen. 2010. Apathy in drug-naïve patients with incident Parkinson’s disease: The Norwegian ParkWest study. Journal of Neurology 257: 217–223. Pluck, G. C., and R. G. Brown. 2002. Apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 73: 636–642. Poewe, W., E. Karamat, G. W. Kemmler, and F. Gerstenbrand. 1983. The premorbid personality of patients with Parkinson’s disease: A comparative study with health controls and patients with essential tremor. Advances in Neurology 53: 339–342. Robert, P. H., S. Clairet, M. Benoit, J. Koutaich, C. Bertogliatic, O. Tible, H. Caci, M. Borg, P. Brocker, and P. Bedoucha. 2002. The Apathy Inventory: Assessment of apathy and awareness in Alzheimer ’s disease, Parkinson’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 1099–1105. Siderowf, A., and M. B. Stern. 2008. Premotor Parkinson’s disease: Clinical features, detection, and prospects for treatment. Annals of Neurology 64: S139–147. Simuni, T., and K. Sethi. 2008. Nonmotor manifestations of Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology 64: S65–80. Sockeel, P., K. Dujardi, D. Devos, C. Denève, A. Destée, and L. Defebvre. 2006. The Lille apathy rating scale (LARS), a new instrument for detecting and quantifying apathy: Validation in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 77: 579–584. Starkstein, S. E., and A. F. G. Leentjens. 2008. The nosological position of apathy in clinical practice. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 79: 1088–1092. Starkstein, S. E., H. S. Mayberg, T. J. Preziosi, P. Andrezejewski, R. Leiguarda, and R. G. Robinson 1992. Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry 4 (2): 134–139. Stavitsky, K., P. McNamara, R. Durso, E. Harris, S. Auerbach, and A. Cronin-Golomb. 2008. Hallucinations, dreaming and frequent dozing in Parkinson’s disease: Impact of right-hemisphere neural networks. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 21: 143–149. Stuss, D. T., R. van Reekum, and K. J. Murphy. 2000. Differentiation of states and causes of apathy. In The neuropsychology of emotion, ed. J. C. Borod, 340-63. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Swanson, L. W. 1982. The projections of the ventral tegmental area and adjacent regions: A combined fluorescent retrograde tracer and immunofluorescence study in the rat. Brain Research Bulletin 9: 321–353. Tolosa, E., C. Gaig, J. Santamaria, and Y. Compta. 2009. Diagnosis and the premotor phase of Parkinson disease. Neurology 72: S12–20. Tolosa, E., G. Wenning, and W. Poewe. 2006. The diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurology 5: 75–86. van Reekum, R., D. T. Stuss, and L. Ostrander. 2005. Apathy: Why care: Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 17: 7–19.
236
Dementia
Weintraub, D. 2008. Dopamine and impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology 64: S93–100. Wolters, Ech., Y. D. van der Werf, and O. A. van den Heuvel. 2008. Parkinson’s disease-related disorders in the impulsive-compulsive spectrum. Journal of Neurology 255: 48–56.
Chapter 9
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia Howard S. Kirshner
Vascular disease in the brain is a common cause of late-life cognitive impairment. It is estimated that up to 30% of stroke survivors have disabling cognitive deterioration at one year after stroke. Vascular risk factors, transient ischemic attacks, both silent and clinically evident strokes, and “white-matter ischemic changes” on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies all correlate with dementia. Vascular dementia, or mixed vascular and degenerative dementia, may be second only to Alzheimer ’s disease in prevalence of dementing illnesses. Historically, vascular diseases were thought to represent the majority of dementia cases; senile dementia was commonly referred to as “cerebral arteriosclerosis” or “hardening of the arteries.” Vascular dementia has been recognized since the writings of Thomas Willis (Roman 2002). The German physician Otto Binswanger wrote about vascular lesions associated with dementia, in the same period that Alois Alzheimer described the plaques and tangles associated with dementia, that led to the name “Alzheimer ’s disease” (AD). In the 1960s, the pendulum of thought about dementing illnesses shifted away from vascular disease, toward the concept of primary neuronal degeneration, unrelated to vascular disease; this has since been the central way of thinking about Alzheimer ’s disease. Over the past decade, the pendulum has shifted back somewhat, toward vascular diseases as prominent causes of dementia. Several lines of evidence, which I will discuss in this chapter, have supported the increased emphasis on vascular diseases. First, in pathological series of dementia cases, vascular diseases are second only to Alzheimer ’s
238
Dementia
disease as causes of dementia, and many cases have both cerebral infarcts and changes of AD. Second, the risk factors for stroke have also been shown to be the principal risk factors for dementia, and even for Alzheimer ’s disease specifically. Third, the pathology of Alzheimer ’s disease, besides the formation of senile plaques containing a central core of amyloid protein in the neuropil and “tangles” in the remaining neurons, includes deposition of amyloid protein in cerebral vessels. Many patients who qualify for the diagnosis of AD have changes on MRI scan and at autopsy of deep whitematter ischemic changes, likely related to the amyloid vasculopathy. Cerebral amyloidosis also results in brain hemorrhages (Zhang-Nunes et al. 2006). Fourth, many types of cerebrovascular disease cause or contribute to the development of dementia. This heterogeneity of vascular disorders makes vascular dementia a complex topic. How do vascular lesions contribute to dementia? The presence of ischemic infarctions in the brain undoubtedly affects cognitive function. The relationship between vascular lesions and cognitive impairment, however, is complex. The degree of impairment likely relates to both the quantitative amount of tissue damage and to the strategic locations of the infarctions. These relationships will be discussed in some detail. In this chapter I shall discuss the definition and diagnostic criteria for vascular cognitive impairment; the evidence for the association of vascular risk factors and vascular lesions with dementia; and finally, the prevention and treatment of vascular dementia. To date, only a limited number of clinical trials have addressed the prevention and treatment of vascular dementia, many fewer than those concerning Alzheimer ’s disease. DEFINITION AND DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR VASCULAR DEMENTIA Dementia can be defined as a loss of cognitive functions significant enough to cause functional disability in everyday life. Vascular dementia is a dementia caused at least in part by vascular lesions such as infarctions. The difficulty implicit in this definition lies in teasing out the contributions of strokes, or vascular lesions, and neurodegenerative changes in a patient with dementia. The presence of AD changes in elderly stroke survivors cannot be excluded by imaging studies alone. Hachinski originally coined the term “multi-infarct dementia” to reflect cases of dementia caused by an accumulation of ischemic stroke damage (Hachinski et al. 1975). This term has not remained useful, for at least two reasons. First, single, strategic infarctions can be associated with dementia, as in the case of a patient with a large left middle cerebral artery territory stroke and
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
239
global aphasia, or a posterior cerebral artery territory stroke with damage to the hippocampus and loss of short-term memory. Second, strokes can affect cognitive functions without producing dementia. In 1975 Hachinski advanced the “Hachinski ischemic score,” originally for use in research studies. This score remains the only quantitative measure of vascular dementia, used in numerous research trials and verified with pathology (Rosen et al. 1980). Table 9.1 shows the Hachinski ischemic score. Note that vascular risk factors such as hypertension, evidence of atherosclerosis, or a history of strokes are important, as are the mode of presentation (acute onset of cognitive difficulties or a stepwise rather than gradually progressive deterioration) and associated features. The terms “vascular dementia” (VaD) (Hachinski 1994, 5) or “vascular cognitive impairment” have largely replaced “multi-infarct dementia.” Tables 9.2 and 9.3 summarize two sets of diagnostic criteria for vascular dementia, the California (Chui et al. 1992) and NINDS-AIREN (Roman et al. 1993) criteria. These criteria are similar, in that they require evidence of strokes, both clinically and by imaging studies (and not just white matter changes on MRI), and also evidence of cognitive impairment. Both make clear that definite vascular dementia can be diagnosed only with neuropathology, usually an autopsy study, so that the most a clinician can diagnose is “probable” or “presumed” vascular dementia. Both sets Table 9.1 Hackinski Ischemic Score Feature Abrupt onset Stepwise deterioration Fluctuating course Nocturnal confusion Relative preservation of personality Depression Somatic complaints Emotional incontinence History of hypertension History of strokes Evidence of associated atherosclerosis Focal neurological symptoms Focal neurological signs
Score 2 1 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 1 2 2
Source: Hachinski et al. 1975 Note: Patients with a total score of ≥ 7 are considered to have multi-infarct dementia; those scoring ≤ 4 have primary degenerative dementia.
240
Dementia
Table 9.2 California Criteria for Ischemic Vascular Dementia (IVD) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
Dementia established by clinical examination Progressive worsening of cognitive function Evidence of ≥ 2 strokes by clinical or neuroradiological criteria Evidence of ≥ 1 hemisphere infarct by CT or MRI (T1-weighted) Diagnosis of definite IVD requires neuropathology
The following factors support the diagnosis of IVD: history of TIA’s, hypertension, or other risk factors for cerebrovascular disease; early gait disorder; extensive deep white matter disease; focal abnormalities on PET or SPECT functional brain imaging. Against ischemic vascular dementia were: absence of focal neurological signs other than cognitive abnormalities; and presence of aphasia, apraxia, or agnosia without appropriate lesions on CT or MRI scans. Source: Chui et al. 1992.
Table 9.3 NINDS-AIREN Criteria 1. 2. 3.
Documented dementia Evidence of cerebrovascular disease by clinical history, clinical examination, or brain imaging The dementia and cerebrovascular disease must be “reasonably related.”
The diagnosis of vascular dementia, by these criteria, also must include a decline in memory and at least two other domains of intellectual ability, with resultant impairment of activities of daily living. Single strokes are permitted, if the other criteria apply. The NINDS-AIREN criteria also emphasize typical clinical features of impairment of multiple cognitive domains, usual presence of focal neurological signs, gait abnormalities, mood changes, psychomotor slowing, and extrapyramidal signs. Against vascular dementia were early onset and progressive worsening of a deficit in memory or other cognitive functions, in the absence of focal lesions on CT or MRI scans; absence of focal neurological signs, other than cognitive ones; and absence of infarcts on brain imaging studies. Source: Roman et al. 1993.
of criteria include either supporting and contravening factors, in the case of the California criteria (e.g., aphasia without an infarct on MRI in the language area would favor Alzheimer ’s disease) or “typical features” in the NINDS-AIREN criteria; both listings contain clinically useful items. There are some important differences. The California criteria utilize only ischemic strokes, whereas the NINDS-AIREN criteria allow both infarctions and hemorrhages. The California criteria also include more explicit rules for imaging evidence of strokes, and they require progressive cognitive dysfunction, whereas the NINDS-AIREN criteria specify only that the
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
241
dementia and cerebrovascular disease must be “reasonably related,” usually meaning onset of cognitive problems within 3 months of a stroke. The diagnosis of vascular dementia, by these criteria, also must include a decline in memory and at least two other domains of intellectual ability, with resultant impairment of activities of daily living. Single strokes are permitted if the other criteria apply. The NINDS-AIREN criteria also emphasize typical clinical features of impairment of multiple cognitive domains, usual presence of focal neurological signs, gait abnormalities, mood changes, psychomotor slowing, and extrapyramidal signs. Against vascular dementia were early onset and progressive worsening of a deficit in memory or other cognitive functions, in the absence of focal lesions on CT or MRI scans; absence of focal neurological signs, other than cognitive ones; and absence of infarcts on brain imaging studies. VASCULAR DISORDERS AND DEMENTIA As mentioned earlier, not only strokes, but also other vascular factors are risk factors for dementia. Five separate lines of evidence support the association between vascular disease and dementia: (1) vascular risk factors; (2) transient ischemic attacks (TIAs); (3) strokes, both clinically evident and “silent”; (4) white-matter ischemic changes on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); and (5) neuropathological studies. All of these vascular disease markers are associated with a higher incidence of dementia in older people. Vascular Risk Factors Risk factors for stroke are also the principal risk factors for dementia. The strongest evidence comes from studies of hypertension, but diabetes mellitus, hyperlipidemia, and the metabolic syndrome have all been found to be associated with an increased risk of dementia. Knopman and colleagues (2001) performed neuropsychological assessments at six-month intervals in 10,963 individuals and found that both hypertension and diabetes correlated with cognitive decline, while smoking and hyperlipidemia did not. In the Syst-Eur hypertension study, a placebo-controlled trial of treatment of systolic hypertension in elderly persons, active antihypertensive treatment with a calcium channel blocker (and other drugs as indicated), reduced the degree of cognitive decline compared to the placebo-arm subjects (Forette et al. 1998). In the PROGRESS trial (Hanon and Forette 2004), an angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) inhibitor, perindopril, usually combined with a diuretic, indapamide, for the treatment of hypertension
242
Dementia
after stroke, reduced cognitive decline, though most of the association was with cognitive decline related to recurrent strokes. Both diabetes mellitus and the “metabolic syndrome” of insulin resistance, central obesity, hypertension, and central (“apple-shaped” as opposed to “pear-shaped”) obesity were associated with an increased risk of dementia (Yaffe et al. 2004). Elevated cholesterol appears to predict dementia early in life, but a pattern of dropping cholesterol later in life is also associated with the onset of dementia (Solomon, Kareholt, and Ngandu 2007). In the Women’s Health Initiative (Shumaker et al. 2003; Rapp, Espeland, and Shumaker 2003), hormone replacement therapy with estrogens in postmenopausal women has been correlated with both an increased risk of stroke and increased cognitive decline and dementia. In general, most of the stroke risk factors are also risk factors for dementia and cognitive impairment. Transient Ischemic Attacks and Cognitive Impairment The second line of evidence linking vascular disease with dementia involves transient ischemic attacks (TIAs), which also represent a potent risk factor for stroke. A recent study of “transient neurological attacks,” including both focal attacks and also nonfocal symptoms such as confusion or dizziness, showed that these attacks predict both risk of stroke and risk of dementia (Bos et al. 2007). In this study, patients with both focal and nonfocal episodes had the highest risk of dementia. Strokes and Cognitive Impairment Third, patients who suffer strokes are at greatly increased risk of developing dementia. In studies by Tatemichi and colleagues (1994), in the 6–12 months after any stroke, 30% of survivors developed significant cognitive impairment or dementia. Age at the time of the stroke and lack of education were associated with higher risk of dementia. In a population study from Olmstead County, Minnesota, Kokmen and colleagues (1996) found that a recent history of stroke conferred a ninefold increased risk of dementia, as compared to the incidence of dementia in control subjects who had not suffered a stroke. Pohjasvaara and colleagues (2000) studied MRI and clinical features in 273 first clinical stroke patients, of whom 28.9% had dementia at three months post-stroke, a figure very close to that reported by Tatemichi and colleagues (Tatemichi et al. 1994). Several studies have examined the locations and sizes of strokes as risk markers for dementia. In the study of Pohjasvaara et al. (2000), features differentiating demented from nondemented subjects included both
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
243
volume and number of infarctions, degree of atrophy, and also location in the left hemisphere corona radiata. In studies from the University of Illinois RMDAS (Risk Markers for Dementia after Stroke) project, stroke survivors with lesions or atrophy in the thalamus were most likely to have cognitive impairment in at least one of seven domains; lesser predictive value was found with lesions in the cingulate gyrus and in the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices (Stebbins et al. 2008). Strategic lesions in the thalamus may be associated with dementia because of disruption of thalamocortical circuits (Szirmai et al. 2002). Williamson and colleagues (2008) reported that frontal white-matter hyperintensities on MRI were associated with persistent cognitive changes after stroke. All of these studies suggest that both “strategic locations” of ischemic strokes and the volume of the infarctions are predictors of cognitive impairment and vascular dementia. MRI Findings: Silent Strokes, White-Matter Disease “Silent” strokes are an important factor in the loss of memory and cognitive function in the elderly. A survey of MRI scans in older people suggested that the annual incidence of “silent” small infarcts may be as high as 22 million infarcts in 11 million persons, though the reported incidence of clinical strokes is approximately 780,000 cases annually in the United States (Leary and Saver 2003). In the Rotterdam aging study, subjects entering the study with evidence of silent infarctions by CT scan had more than a twofold increased risk of developing dementia (Vermeer et al. 2003; Vermeer, Longstreth, and Koudstaal 2007) during the follow-up period. Ischemic white-matter disease, or “leukoaraiosis” (Hachinski, Potter, and Merskey 1987), has been a controversial topic. These lesions do not represent definite strokes, but they correlate with age, vascular risk factors, and cognitive impairment (Steingart et al. 1987; Kertesz, Polk, and Carr 1990; Breteler et al. 1994). Not all studies, however, confirm that ischemic white-matter lesions always indicate cognitive impairment. In a small series, Hershey and colleagues (1987) found that cerebral atrophy or enlargement of the cerebrospinal fluid spaces correlated with cognitive impairment, but white-matter lesions did not. Fein and colleagues (1990) documented two patients who had extensive white-matter lesions, but no increase in cognitive impairment over as long as seven years of follow-up. Rao and colleagues (1989) compared neuropsychological test performance in patients with and without white-matter lesions, and they found no overall differences. On the other hand, several studies have indicated that ischemic white-matter changes are associated with disturbance of executive
244
Dementia
function (Moser et al. 2001; Bombois et al. 2007). I will return to the subject of executive dysfunction as an important symptom of vascular cognitive impairment later in this chapter. Perhaps the most important point about leukoaraiosis is that, when the lesions progress over time, progression of cognitive dysfunction is likely (Schmidt et al. 2007). Leukoaraiosis has remained a subject of debate because MRI scans also show ischemic whitematter changes in patients who would otherwise meet clinical criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease, and they occur in normal older persons, especially those with vascular risk factors such as hypertension. They are thus not an invariant sign of vascular cognitive impairment. Neuropathology Neuropathogy is the only “gold standard” for the diagnosis of dementing illnesses. The contribution of vascular lesions to dementia can perhaps best be sorted out in autopsy studies. Rosen and colleagues (1980) found that the Hachinski ischemic score reliably distinguished patients with pure AD from those with vascular or mixed dementias, but the latter two groups could not be distinguished from each other. A number of recent neuropathological studies have attempted to discern the contribution of vascular pathology to the degree of dementia, even in patients who also had neuropathological changes of Alzheimer ’s disease. In a series of 89 autopsies of dementia patients, Knopman and colleagues (2003) reported that 34% had pure AD, 12% had both multiple infarcts and AD pathology, 17% had at least one infarct and AD pathology, and 13% had pure vascular disease, without AD changes. Schneider and colleagues (2007) reported that the presence of small infarctions increased the odds of dementia by more than fivefold, even allowing for changes of Alzheimer ’s disease. Microinfarcts and white-matter demyelination were also associated with dementia in the study of Kovari and colleagues (2007). Chui et al (2006) investigated the contributions of subcortical infarctions, Alzheimer ’s disease pathology, and hippocampal sclerosis in dementia; all three factors had important effects. In effect, vascular pathology lowers the threshold for the development of dementia or increases the impact of aging changes on cognitive function. Amyloid Angiopathy and Dementia Amyloid angiopathy is a disorder in which amyloid proteins are deposited in cerebral vessels. The usual result of this disorder is the development of a lobar hemorrhage (Zhang-Nunes et al. 2006; Rosand and Greenberg 2000). Tiny hemorrhages, called “microbleeds,” often associated with
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
245
amyloid angiopathy, may also contribute to dementia (Zhang-Nunes et al. 2006; Viswanathan and Chabriat 2006). These microbleeds were present in over 80% of brains from a series of subcortical vascular dementia cases (Seo et al. 2007). Amyloid angiopathy, or amyloid deposition in vessels, may also lead to ischemic lesions and may relate to the deep white-matter lesions or leukoaraiosis seen in both AD and vascular dementia (Cadavid et al. 2000). NEUROBIOLOGY OF VASCULAR DEMENTIA Strokes can clearly affect cognitive function by damaging brain areas important for cognitive function. There are other, more complex interactions, however, between vascular disease and cognitive function. For example, activation of the Angiotensin II system appears to increase the production of amyloid peptides, involved in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer ’s disease. This interaction is thought to occur at the endothelium of brain microvessels, where amyloid peptide is manufactured (Vagnucci and Li 2003). This mechanism could explain at least part of the reason why hypertension is a risk factor for dementia. Another factor under study is the MEOX2, or GAX gene, a regulator of vascular differentiation, whose expression is low in brains of patients with AD. This gene also regulates angiogenesis and reduces apoptosis and increases production of a protein involved in the clearance of amyloid-beta peptide (Abeta) (Wu et al. 2005). The interaction of vascular factors with amyloid deposition and the pathogenesis of AD is an active field of research. Symptoms of Vascular Dementia The neuropsychology of vascular dementia is complex. Symptoms of vascular disease vary according to the size and location of lesions, and likely other factors as well (McPherson and Cummings 1996; Reed et al. 2007). Deficits are often referred to as “patchy,” but this would clearly relate to the locations of infarctions. We shall return later to syndromes associated with large infarctions. One common theme of vascular dementia, however, is the prominent involvement of deficits in executive function in patients with ischemic white-matter lesions, subcortical infarctions, and vascular dementia in general (Moser et al. 2001; Bombois et al. 2007). Although the neuropsychological patterns in vascular dementia vary considerably, it is rare for vascular dementia to present with isolated memory loss, and most patients with vascular dementia have executive function deficits (Reed et al. 2007). On the other hand, a pure amnestic presentation
246
Dementia
would be typical of Alzheimer ’s disease and its precursor, mild cognitive impairment (Kirshner 2005). VARIETIES OF VASCULAR DEMENTIA Vascular dementia is complex and heterogeneous because it does not consist of only one pattern of vascular diseases. Lumping the various vascular diseases together under a single rubric of “vascular dementia” is a major source of diagnostic confusion. The following is a brief, simplified categorization of vascular diseases associated with dementia. Multiple, Large Infarcts Patients with multiple, large ischemic strokes represent the classic syndrome of “multi-infarct dementia.” As discussed earlier, dementia develops in stroke patients, related to the location and amount of infarcted tissue; even single strokes in strategic sites may cause major cognitive impairment, sometimes in association with age-related brain pathology. McPherson and Cummings (1996) list four sites of focal infarction as causes of dementia: the left angular gyrus (Benson, Cummings, and Tsai 1982), the caudate nucleus (Mendez, Adams, and Lewandowski 1989), the globus pallidus, and the thalamus (Schmahmann 2003). I would add that single, large cortical strokes in either middle cerebral artery territory, large frontal lobe infarctions, and posterior cerebral artery infarctions with involvement of the medial temporal region, especially the hippocampus, all produce cognitive syndromes that can cause permanent cognitive disability. Another vascular syndrome is the amnestic syndrome following rupture of an anterior communicating artery aneurysm, often with infarction in the deep medial frontal regions (Diamond, DeLuca, and Kelley 1997). In all of these focal stroke examples, the clinical stroke event is usually quite obvious, and the dementia is not a diagnostic mystery. Occasionally, however, infarcts may occur in “silent” areas of the brain, and dementia may then develop more insidiously. Lacunar State Patients with chronic hypertension are at risk for the development of “lacunar” strokes, small infarcts deep in the white matter of the cerebral hemispheres, the internal capsule, the basal ganglia, and the brainstem. These strokes are frequently multiple and lead to the syndrome of “lacunar state,” or “etat lacunaire.” Patients typically manifest increased reflexes,
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
247
positive Babinski signs, spastic tone, pseudobulbar emotional lability, and frontal release signs, as well as cognitive impairment (Roman 2002; Corbett, Bennett, and Kos 1994). CADASIL Families with multiple members with migraine-like headaches, multiple small territory ischemic strokes, and dementia are typical of CADASIL (Cerebral Autosomal Dominant Arteriopathy with Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy) (Chabriat et al. 1997; Dichgans et al. 1998). Migraine headaches may begin early in life, strokes in adult life, and dementia in midlife or the elderly period. Dementia appears to be correlated with brain atrophy and with the burden of subcortical infarctions (Jouvent et al. 2007). Most reports of CADASIL have come from Europe; if CADASIL is as common in the United States as in Europe, then many cases go unrecognized. The disease is related to a mutation of the Notch3 gene on chromosome 19 (Jouvent et al. 2007). The disorder can be diagnosed by a genetic test or by arterial biopsy. Binswanger ’s Disease Binswanger ’s disease, or subcortical arteriosclerotic encephalopathy (Caplan and Schoene 1978; Babikian and Ropper 1987), is diagnosed in elderly patients with dementia related to longstanding hypertension and a history of acute strokes. As in the lacunar state, patients often manifest pseudobulbar palsy, emotional lability, pyramidal tract signs, gait difficulty, and often bladder urgency or incontinence. CT and MR imaging reveal extensive cerebral white-matter disease, without obvious cortical infarcts, a severe version of the leukoaraiosis discussed earlier in this chapter. The pathology of Binswanger ’s disease includes lacunar infarcts and areas of white-matter demyelination with gliosis (Babikian and Ropper 1987). The diagnosis of Binswanger ’s disease depends on typical clinical features, confirmed at autopsy, and not on MRI features alone; the use of this term to describe the white-matter lesion of leukoaraiosis is not in keeping with the original description of the disease. Binswanger ’s disease, as classically defined, is a rare diagnosis (Mast and Mohr 1996). Miscellaneous Other Vascular Cognitive Syndromes Many less common vascular disorders lead to dementia. Patients with advanced atherosclerosis of the great vessels may develop a syndrome of
248
Dementia
multiple-organ involvement from cholesterol emboli. Confusion, transient ischemic attacks, strokes, slowly progressive renal failure, retinal emboli, and ischemia of the distal extremities (“blue toes”) are all clinical signs associated with multiple cholesterol emboli (Beal et al. 1981). Sneddon’s syndrome (1965) is a combination of multiple ischemic strokes and livedo reticularis, a mottling of the skin. Some but not all cases have had associated anticardiolipin antibodies, and a hypercoagulable state is the presumed mechanism of the strokes. Vascular dementia has been reported in this syndrome (Wright and Kokmen 1999). Collagen vascular diseases are another complex topic within the rubric of vascular dementia. Only a brief summary will be provided here. These diseases produce neurological damage by vascular inflammation, or vasculitis. Most vasculitic illnesses involve inflammation in vessels not only in the brain, but in systemic organs as well. Neurological syndromes include stroke syndromes, confusional states, psychosis, dementia, affective disorders, and seizures (Moore and Richardson 1998; Fieschi et al. 1998). The most common collagen vascular disease associated with dementia is systemic lupus erythematosis (Kirk, Kertesz, and Polk 1991), but only a minority of patients with CNS lupus actually has vasculitis. Antiphospholipid antibodies and nonbacterial endocarditis are other mechanisms underlying the stroke symptoms, and direct immunological attack on the nervous system may produce the psychosis and seizures. Other diseases involving vasculitis include Churg-Strauss vasculitis with eosinophilia, mixed connective tissue disease, periarteritis nodosa, Wegener ’s granulomatosis, Sjogren’s syndrome, giant cell or temporal arteritis, thrombotic thrombocytopenic purpura (TTP), and Susac’s syndrome, a triad of central nervous system vasculitis, deafness, and retinal infarcts (Susac 1994). These diseases are diagnosed by elevated erythrocyte sedimentation rates and abnormal antibody tests such as ANA, anti-DNA and ANCA (Fieschi et al. 1998). The most difficult to diagnose of the vasculitic illnesses is isolated cerebral vasculitis, or granulomatous angiitis. This disease presents with headaches and multiple small infarctions in the brain, usually with small lesions on MRI and inflammatory changes in the CSF such as pleocytosis and elevated protein. Systemic antibody tests are typically negative, and diagnosis requires arteriography and, often, brain biopsy. Treatment with corticosteroids and immunosuppressive therapy appears to help in many cases (Moore 1989). Arteriovenous malformations usually present with either bleeding or epileptic seizures. Rarely, an AVM can present with dementia. These lesions may produce cerebral symptoms by mass effect, by hemorrhage, by production of seizures, and occasionally by “stealing” blood from
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
249
adjacent tissue (Perret and Nishioka 1966). Subacute diencephalic angioencephalopathy is a rare disorder characterized by rapidly progressive memory loss, emotional changes, dementia, and myoclonus, mimicking Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. Pathological studies indicate inflammatory vascular degeneration restricted to the thalamus (DeGirolami, Haas, and Richardson 1974). PREVENTION OF VASCULAR DEMENTIA The most important aspect of the prevention of vascular dementia, or at least stabilization and avoidance of further progression, is the aggressive management of risk factors. If one wants to prevent dementia, whether Alzheimer ’s disease or vascular dementia, the best advice is to carry out the same preventive lifestyle and medical measures adopted to prevent heart attack and stroke: exercise, healthy diet, smoking cessation, and management of blood pressure, cholesterol, and diabetes. As stated earlier, under risk factors, treatment of hypertension is associated with reduced decline in cognitive function over time. Presumably, healthy diet, exercise, smoking cessation, avoidance of excessive alcohol, and optimal management of blood pressure, glucose, and lipids all serve to prevent vascular dementia (Gorelick et al. 1999). Jick and colleagues (2000), in a case-control study, reported a 71% reduced risk of developing dementia among statin users. Prevention of vascular dementia by risk factor interventions is a fruitful area of current research. TREATMENT OF VASCULAR DEMENTIA Clinical trials have shown some efficacy for the acetylcholinesterase inhibitors galantamine (Erkinjuntti et al. 2002) and donepezil (Black et al. 2003; Wilkinson et al. 2003; Aguilar et al. 2006) in patients with vascular dementia, but one study reported excess deaths in the donepezil treatment group (Aguilar et al. 2006). The third cholinesterase inhibitor, rivastigmine, has not been tested in a large clinical trial in vascular dementia, but one study of AD patients with vascular risk factors suggested equal or greater benefit, as compared to AD patients without vascular risk factors (Kumar et al. 2000). The FDA has not approved these drugs for vascular dementia, principally because the definition of vascular dementia has been considered imprecise, but the finding of increased deaths with donepezil in one study has surely not advanced the case for these agents. A recent metaanalysis by Kavirajan and Schneider (2007) of cholinesterase inhibitors in vascular dementia showed only small benefits in cognition, without
250
Dementia
significant improvement in activities of daily living or behavioral abnormalities. Use of these drugs cannot be routinely recommended. It is possible that some of the benefit seen in the clinical trials of vascular dementia relates to underlying Alzheimer pathology, since “mixed” AD and vascular dementia is more common than pure vascular dementia. Theoretically, however, the interruption of cholinergic fibers by subcortical vascular lesions could provide a rationale for the efficacy of these drugs. A recent trial involved donepezil in patients with CADASIL. This group of patients would seem to have the advantage of a much more uniform pathophysiology than the numerous types of strokes represented in a typical vascular dementia cohort. Donepezil did not improve overall cognitive function or memory, though some improvement in executive function was noted (Dichgans et al. 2008). Memantine, a reversible blocker of the NMDA subtype of the glutamate receptor, has been approved for moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease, but the drug has had only limited testing in vascular dementia. Memantine has shown some promise in clinical trials including patients with vascular dementia (Wilcock et al. 2002; Orgogozo et al. 2002). This drug, too, is FDA approved only for AD, not for vascular dementia. CONCLUSION Vascular dementia has a long history; it was the earliest recognized cause of dementia, and it has become increasingly clear in recent years that vascular risk factors and disorders contribute commonly to cognitive decline in the elderly. Vascular dementia has frustrated simple statements because of the variability in vascular diseases, and in the locations and size of lesions. Diagnosis is difficult, even using diagnostic criteria such as the Hachinski ischemic score, the NINDS-AIREN, or California criteria. Exclusion of underlying neurodegenerative disease is almost impossible. Advances in neurobiology may lead to a greater understanding of these disorders. The principal treatments at present are preventive, in the form of aggressive management of vascular risk factors. REFERENCES Aguilar, M., G. Roman, S. Black, et al. 2006. Efficacy and safety of donepezil in vascular dementia: Results from the largest double-blind trial in vascular dementia. Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Alzheimer ’s disease and related disorders, Madrid, Spain, July 15–20, 4–439.
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
251
Babikian, V., and A. H. Ropper. 1987. Binswanger ’s disease: A review. Stroke 18: 1–12. Beal, M. F., R. S. Williams, E. P. Richardson, and C. M. Fisher. 1981. Cholesterol embolism as a cause of transient ischemic attacks and cerebral infarction. Neurology 31: 860–865. Benson, D. F., J. C. Cummings, and S. I. Tsai. 1982. Angular gyrus syndrome simulating Alzheimer ’s disease. Arch Neurol 39: 616–620. Black, S., G. C. Roman, D. S. Geldmacher, et al. 2003. Efficacy and tolerability of donepezil in vascular dementia: Positive results of a 24-week, multicenter, international, randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Stroke 34: 2323–2330. Bombois, S., S. Debette, X. Delbeuck, et al. 2007. Prevalence of subcortical vascular lesions and association with executive dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment subtypes. Stroke 38: 2595–2597. Bos, M. J., M. J. Van Rijn, J. C. Witteman, et al. 2007. Incidence and prognosis of transient neurological attacks. JAMA 298: 1877–2785. Breteler, M. M. B., J. C. van Swieten, M. L. Bots, et al. 1994. Cerebral white matter lesions, vascular risk factors, and cognitive function in a population-based study: The Rotterdam Study. Neurology 44: 1246–1252. Cadavid, D., H. Mena, K. Koeller, and R. A. Frommelt. 2000. Cerebral beta amyloid angiopathy is a risk factor for cerebral ischemic infarction. A case control study in human brain biopsies. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 59: 768–773. Caplan, L. R., and W. C. Schoene. 1978. Clinical features of subcortical arteriosclerotic encephalopathy (Binswanger disease). Neurology 28: 1206–1215. Chabriat, H., A. Joutel, K. Vahedi, et al. 1997. CADASIL: Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy. Rev Neurol (Paris) 153: 376–385. Chui, H., C. Zarow, W. J. Mack, et al. 2006. Cognitive impact of subcortical vascular and Alzheimer ’s disease pathology. Ann Neurol 60: 677–687. Chui, H. C., J. I. Victoroff, D. Margolin, et al. 1992. Criteria for the diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the State of California Alzheimer ’s Disease Diagnostic and Treatment Centers. Neurology 42: 473–480. Corbett, A. J., H. Bennett, and S. Kos. 1994. Cognitive dysfunction following subcortical infarction. Arch Neurol 51: 999–1007. DeGirolami, V., M. L. Haas, and E. P. Richardson Jr. 1974. Subacute diencephalic angioencephalopathy: A clinicopathological case study. J Neurol Sci 22: 197–210. Diamond, B. J., J. DeLuca, and S. M. Kelley. 1997. Memory and executive functions in amnestic and nonamnestic patients with aneurysms of the anterior communicating artery. Brain 120: 1015–1025. Dichgans, M., H. S. Markus, S. Salloway, et al. 2008. Donepezil in patients with subcortical vascular cognitive impairment: A randomized double-blind trial in CADASIL. Lancet Neurol 7: 310–318.
252
Dementia
Dichgans, M., M. Mayer, I. Uttner, et al. 1998. The phenotypic spectrum of CADASIL: Clinical findings in 102 cases. Neurology 44: 731–739. Erkinjuntti, T., A. Kurz, S. Gauthier, et al. 2002. Efficacy of galantamine in probable vascular dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease combined with cerebrovascular disease: A randomised trial. Lancet 359: 1283–1290. Fein, G., C. Van Dyke, L. Davenport, et al. 1990. Preservation of normal cognitive functioning in elderly subjects with extensive white-matter lesions of long duration. Arch Gen Psychiat 47: 220–223. Fieschi, C,, M. Rasura, A. Anzini, and M. Beccia. 1998. Central nervous system vasculitis. J Neurol Sci 8: 159–171. Forette, F., M. L. Seux, J. A. Staessen, et al. 1998. Prevention of dementia in randomised double-blind placebo-controlled Systolic Hypertension in Europe (Syst-Eur) trial. Lancet 352: 1347–1351. Gorelick, P. B., T. Erkinjuntti, A. Hofman, W. A. Rocca, I. Skoog, and B. Winblad. 1999. Prevention of vascular dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 13 (Suppl. 3): S131–139. Hachinski, V. 1994. Vascular dementia: A radical redefinition. Dementia 5: 130–132. Hachinski, V. C., L. D. Iliff, E. Zilkha. G. H. DuBoulay, V. L. McAllister, J. Marshall, R. W. Ross Russell, and L. Symon. 1975. Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Arch Neurol 32: 632–637. Hachinski, V. C., P. Potter, and H. Merskey. 1987. Leuko-araiosis. Arch Neurol 44: 21–23. Hanon, O., and F. Forette. 2004. Prevention of dementia: Lessons from SYST-EUR and PROGRESS. J Neurol Sci 226: 71–74. Hershey, L. A., M. T. Modic, P. G. Greenough, and D. F. Jaffe. 1987. Magnetic resonance imaging in vascular dementia. Neurology 37: 29–36. Jick, H., G. L. Zornberg, S. S. Jick, S. Seshadri, and D. A. Drachman. 2000. Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet 356: 1627–1631. Jouvent, E., A. Viswanathan, J. F. Mangin, et al. 2007. Brain atrophy is related to lacunar lesions and tissue microstructural changes in CADASIL. Stroke 38: 1786–1790. Kavirajan, H., and L. S. Schneider. 2007. Efficacy and adverse effects of cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine in vascular dementia: A meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. Lancet Neurol 6: 782–792. Kertesz, A., M. Polk, and T. Carr. 1990. Cognition and white matter changes on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia. Arch Neurol 47: 387–391. Kirk, A., A. Kertesz, and M. J. I. Polk. 1991. Dementia with leukoencephalopathy in systemic lupus erythematosus. Can J Neurol Sci 18: 344–348. Kirshner, H. S. 2005. Mild cognitive impairment: To treat or not to treat. Current Neurol Neurosci Rep 5: 455–457. Knopman, D., L. L. Boland, T. Mosley, et al., for the Atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study investigators. 2001. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults. Neurology 56: 42–48.
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
253
Knopman, D. S., D. S. Parisi, B. F. Boeve, et al. 2003. Vascular dementia in a population-based autopsy study. Arch Neurol 60: 569–575. Kokmen, E., J. P. Whisnant, W. M. O’Fallon, C. P. Chu, and C. M. Beard. 1996. Dementia after ischemic stroke: A population-based study in Rochester, Minnesota (1960–1984). Neurology 46: 154–159. Kovari, E., G. Gold, F. R. Herrmann, et al. 2007. Cortical microinfarcts and demyelination affect cognition in cases at high risk for dementia. Neurology 68: 927–931. Kumar, V., R. Anand, J. Messina, et al. 2000. An efficacy and safety analysis of Exelon® in Alzheimer ’s disease patients with concurrent vascular risk factors. Eur J Neurol 7: 159–169. Leary, M. C., and J. L. Saver. 2003. Annual incidence of first silent stroke in the United States: A preliminary estimate. Cerebrovasc Dis 16: 280–285. Mast, H., and J. P. Mohr. 1996. Otto Binswanger and the role of chronic ischemia in vascular dementia. Cerebrovasc Dis 6: 255–263. McPherson, S. E., and J. L. Cummings. 1996. Neuropsychological aspects of vascular dementia. Brain and Cognition 31: 269–282. Mendez, M. F., N. L. Adams, and K. S. Lewandowski. 1989. Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions. Neurology 39: 349–354. Moore, P. M. 1989. Diagnosis and management of isolated angiitis of the central nervous system. Neurology 39: 167–1173. Moore, P. M., and B. Richardson. 1998. Neurology of the vasculitides and connective tissue diseases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 65: 10–22. Moser, D. J., R. A. Cohen, R. H. Paul, et al. 2001. Executive function and magnetic resonance imaging subcortical hyperintensities in vascular dementia. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 14: 89–92. Orgogozo, J. M., A. S. Rigaud, A. Stoffler, et al. 2002. Efficacy and safety of memantine in patients with mild to moderate vascular dementia: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial (MMM 300). Stroke 33: 1834–1839. Perret, G., and H. Nishioka. 1966. Report on the cooperative study of intracranial aneurysms and subarachnoid hemorrhage. Section VI. Arteriovenous malformations. J Neurosurg 25: 467–490. Pohjasvaara, T., R. Mantyla, O. Salonen, H. J. Aronen, R. Ylikoski, M. Hietanen, M. Kaste, and T. Erkinjuntti. 2000. How complex interactions of ischemic brain infarcts, white matter lesions, and atrophy relate to poststroke dementia. Arch Neurol 57: 1295–1300. Rao, S. M., W. Mittenberg, L. Bernardin, et al. 1989. Neuropsychological test findings in subjects with leukoaraiosis. Arch Neurol 46: 40–44. Rapp, S. R., M. A. Espeland, and S. A. Shumaker. 2003. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women. The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 289: 2663–2672. Reed, B. R., D. M. Mungas, J. H. Kramer, et al. 2007. Profiles of neuropsychological impairment in autopsy-defined Alzheimer ’s disease and cerebrovascular disease. Brain 130: 731–739.
254
Dementia
Roman, G. C. 2002. On the history of lacunes, etat crible, and the white matter lesions of vascular dementia. Cerebrovasc Dis 13 (Suppl. 2): 1–6. Roman, G. C., T. K. Tatemichi, T. Erkinjuntti, et al. 1993. Vascular dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Work Group. Neurology 43: 250–260. Rosand, J., and S. M. Greenberg. 2000. Cerebral amyloid angiopathy. The Neurologist 6: 315–325. Rosen, W. G., R. D. Terry, P. A. Fuld, R. Katzman, and A. Peck. 1980. Pathological verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias. Ann Neurol 7: 486–488. Schmahmann, J. D. 2003. Vascular syndromes of the thalamus. Stroke 34: 2264–2278. Schmidt, R., K. Petrovic, S. Ropele, et al. 2007. Progression of leukoaraiosis and cognition. Stroke 38: 2619–2625. Schneider, J. A., P. A. Boyle, Z. Arvanitakis, et al. 2007. Subcortical infarcts, Alzheimer ’s disease pathology, and memory function in older persons. Ann Neurol 62: 59–66. Seo, S. W., B. H. Lee, E-J. Kim, et al. 2007. Clinical significance of microbleeds in subcortical vascular dementia. Stroke 38: 1949–1951. Shumaker, S. A., C. Legault, S. R. Rapp, et al. 2003. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women. The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 289: 2651–2652. Sneddon, I. B. 1965. Cerebro-vascular lesions and livedo reticularis. Br J Dermatol 77: 180–185. Solomon, A., I. Kareholt, and T. Ngandu. 2007. Serum cholesterol changes after mid-life and late-life cognition: Twenty-one-year follow-up. Neurology 68: 751–756. Stebbins, G. T., D. L. Nyenhuis, C. Wang, et al. 2008. Gray matter atrophy in patients with ischemic stroke with cognitive impairment. Stroke 39: 785–793. Steingart, A., V. C. Hachinski, C. Lau, A. J. Fox, F. Diaz, R. Cape, D. Lee, D. Inzitari, and H. Merskey. 1987. Cognitive and neurologic findings in subjects with diffuse white matter lucencies on computed tomographic scan (leuko-araiosis). Arch Neurol 44: 32–35. Susac, J. 1994. Susac’s syndrome: The triad of microangiopathy of the brain and retina with hearing loss in young women. Neurology 44: 591–593. Szirmai, I., I. Vastagh, E. Szombathelyi, and A. Kamondi. 2002. Strategic infarcts of the thalamus in vascular dementia. J Neurol Sci 203–204: 91–97. Tatemichi, T. K., M. Paik, E. Bagiella, D. W. Desmond, Y. Stern, M. Sano, W. A. Hauser, and R. Mayeux. 1994. Risk of dementia after stroke in a hospitalized cohort: Results of a longitudinal study. Neurology 44: 1885–1891. Vagnucci, A. H., Jr, and W. W. Li. 2003. Alzheimer ’s disease and angiogenesis. Lancet 361: 605–608.
Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia
255
Vermeer, S. E., W. T. Longstreth Jr., and P. J. Koudstaal. 2007. Silent brain infarcts: A systematic review. Lancet Neurol 6: 611–619. Vermeer, S. E., N. D. Prins, T. den Heijer, et al. 2003. Silent brain infarcts and the risk of dementia and cognitive decline. New Engl J Med 348: 1215–1222. Viswanathan, A., and H. Chabriat. 2006. Cerebral microhemorrhages. Stroke 37: 550–555. Wilcock, G., H. J. Mobius, A. Stoffler, and MMM 500 Group. 2002. A double-blind, placebo-controlled multicentre study of memantine in mild to moderate vascular dementia (MMM 500). Int Clin Psychopharmacol 17: 297–305. Wilkinson, D., R. Doody, R. Helme, et al. 2003. Donepezil in vascular dementia: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Neurology 61: 479–486. Williamson, J. B., D. L. Nyenhuis, L. Pedelty, et al. 2008. Baseline differences between vascular cognitive impairment no dementia reverters and nonreverters. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 79: 1208–1214. Wright, R. A., and E. Kokmen. 1999. Gradually progressive dementia without discrete cerebrovascular events in a patient with Sneddon’s syndrome. Mayo Clin Proc 74: 57–61. Wu, Z., H. Guo, N. Chow, et al. 2005. Role of the MEOX2 homeobox gene in neurovascular dysfunction in Alzheimer ’s disease. Nat Med 11: 899–904. Yaffe, K., A. Kanaya, K. Lindquist, et al. 2004. The metabolic syndrome, inflammation, and rise of cognitive decline. JAMA 292: 2237–2242. Zhang-Nunes, S. X., M. J. Maat-Schieman, S. G. van Duinen, R. A. Roos, M. P. Frosch, and S. M. Greenberg. 2006. The cerebral beta-amyloid angiopathies: Hereditary and sporadic. Brain Pathol 16: 30–39.
Chapter 10
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies Haruhiko Oda, Yasuji Yamamoto, and Kiyoshi Maeda
Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is considered to be the second most common form of neurodegenerative dementia after Alzheimer ’s disease (AD). It is characterized neuropathologically by the presence of Lewy bodies; those are detectable in post-mortem brain biopsies. The clinical core features of DLB are fluctuation in cognitive function, recurrent visual hallucinations, and the spontaneous features of parkinsonism. Those core features, however, demonstrate a low frequency in patients with AD. If those features were clearly present, then it would be simple to differentiate DLB from AD. However, not all patients with DLB manifest such core features early in the course of the disease. It is therefore often difficult to accurately diagnose DLB, especially at the initial presentation. The need for an early and accurate diagnosis of DLB in order to administer the proper clinical treatment has been emphasized by reports of severe neuroleptic sensitivity and the preferential response to cholinesterase inhibitors in these patients. Therefore, the ability to accurately diagnose the cause of dementia could be of great medical benefit. An analysis of the neuropsychological profile was carried out, while various brain imaging techniques were also investigated with the aim of establishing an early diagnosis of DLB. The neuropsychological profile of DLB consists of a poor attentional, executive, and constructional function and a better short- and medium-term recall than those of AD. In the present chapter,
258
Dementia
the history of research, pathology, clinical symptoms, cognitive profiles, and differential clinical diagnosis of DLB is reviewed. HISTORY OF RESEARCH Lewy bodies were first seen and linked to Parkinson’s disease (“paralysis agitans”) in 1912 by the German neurologist Frederic Lewy. Lewy bodies appear as spherical masses that displace other cell components. There are two morphological types of Lewy bodies, namely, brainstem and cortical types. A brainstem Lewy body is an eosinophilic cytoplasmic inclusion that consists of a dense core surrounded by a halo, measuring approximately 10 nm in diameter, of radiating fibrils, the primary structural component of which is alpha-synuclein. Hematoxylin and eosin staining is not sufficient for the detection of cortical Lewy bodies and it is also not capable of detecting Lewy neurites. Initially, Lewy did not ascribe any neurobehavioral significance to the Lewy bodies observed in postencephalitic parkisonian patients in 1912. Okazaki et al. reported two cases of elderly patients of European extraction who exhibited progressive dementia and quadriparesis in flexion (Okazaki et al. 1961). The neuropathology of these two patients was characterized by the presence of numerous Lewy bodies in the cerebral cortex as well as in the brainstem. Although these inclusion bodies lacked the distinctive halo of brainstem Lewy bodies, this group of investigators established an association between these cerebral inclusions and dementia. Their report did not receive much academic attention for approximately 15 years. However, Ikeda et al. reported a dementia case involving a young person with parkinsonism (Ikeda et al. 1975). Kosaka et al. reported an autopsied case with progressive dementia and parkinsonism, and the neuropathologic features demonstrated the widespread presence of Lewy bodies thoroughout the central nervous system as well as Alzheimer ’s changes (Kosaka et al. 1976). Kosaka described three cases with a distribution of cortical Lewy bodies (Kosaka 1978). Thereafter, many similar cases were reported in Japan. In addition, Kosaka and Mehraein reported German cases with progressive dementia and parkinsonism (Kosaka and Mehraein 1979). The neuropathology of these cases was characterized by the widespread occurrence of Lewy bodies. These were the first cases reported in Europe. Kosaka et al. examined 20 cases with “Lewy body disease” and classified this disease pathologically into three types: brainstem, transitional, and diffuse (Kosaka et al. 1980). Yoshimura confirmed their findings and proposed the term “diffuse Lewy body disease” (DLBD) (Yoshimura et al. 1983). Kosaka et al. suggested that DLBD had
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
259
been overlooked in both European and American countries, since only a few DLBD cases had been reported in those countries (Kosaka et al. 1984). Since 1985, a number of DLBD cases have been reported in both North American and European countries. Similar terminology has been proposed by several researchers, such as diffuse cortical Lewy body dementia, senile dementia of Lewy body type, and Lewy body variant of Alzheimer ’s disease. To resolve the confusion in naming this disease, the generic term “dementia with Lewy bodies” (DLB) was proposed to include those disorders in the first International Workshop on Lewy Body Dementia held in Newcastle-upon-Tyne in 1995. The results of this workshop were reported in 1996 (McKeith et al. 1996). Consensus criteria for clinical and pathologic diagnosis of DLB were thereafter published. The clinical criteria for probable DLB showed sufficient specificity, but poor sensitivity. From this standpoint, the revised criteria for the clinical diagnosis of DLB were thereafter proposed at the third international workshop meeting on DLB held in Newcastle-upon-Tyne in 2003 and later were published in 2005 (McKeith et al. 2005). Recently DLB, Parkinson’s disease (PD), and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD) have been identified as belonging to the spectrum of Lewy body disease. PATHOLOGY From a pathological standpoint, DLB is a common disorder of the alpha-synuclein metabolism characterized by the development of abnormal cytoplasmic inclusions, called Lewy bodies, throughout the brain. A Lewy body is the pathologic aggregation of alpha-synuclein. It is also associated with intermediate filaments, chaperone proteins, and elements of the ubiquitin-proteasome system. DLB was originally defined as a clinicopathologic entity with a specific constellation of the clinical features, and a descriptive approach was proposed for assessing the neuropathology of this disease (McKeith et al. 1996). The only neuropathologic requirement for DLB was the presence of Lewy bodies somewhere in the brain of a patient with a clinical history of dementia. Other pathologic features, such as senile plaques and neuron loss (frequently seen in AD), could also occur; however, they are not either inclusive or exclusive to the diagnosis of DLB. As increasingly sensitive methods for detecting Lewy bodies have been developed, as many as 60% of AD cases may thus be considered to meet the pathologic criteria for DLB based on the 1996 criteria. In addition, none of these patients normally demonstrated the clinical symptoms of DLB.
260
Dementia
New recommendations have thus been proposed which take into account both the extent of Lewy-related pathology and AD-type pathology in assessing the degree of certainty that the neuropathologic findings explain the DLB clinical symptoms. Lewy bodies are present in DLB as well as in PD. In addition, there is a loss of dopamine-producing neurons (in the substantia nigra) similar to that seen in PD and a loss of acetylcholine (ACh)-producing neurons (in the nucleus basalis of Meynert [NBM]and elsewhere) similar to that seen in AD. Cerebral atrophy (or shrinkage) also occurs as the cerebral cortex degenerates. Autopsy series have revealed the pathology of DLB to often be concomitant with the pathology of AD. Namely, when Lewy body inclusions are found in the cortex, they often co-occur with the AD pathology found primarily in the hippocampus, including neurofibrillary tangles (abnormally phosphorylated tau protein), senile plaques (amyloid protein deposits), and granulovacuolar degeneration. Kosaka and colleagues proposed two distinct pathological subtypes of DLB: (1) the common form, found in approximately 75% of cases, with a mixed Lewy body and amyloid pathology; and (2) the pure form, with only the Lewy body pathology (Kosaka 1990). Within DLB, the loss of cholinergic (ACh-producing) neurons is thought to account for the degradation of cognitive and emotional functioning, as in AD, whereas the loss of dopaminergic (dopamine-producing) neurons is thought to account for the degradation of motor control, as is observed in PD. Therefore, DLB is similar to the dementia resulting from both AD and PD. In fact, DLB is often confused in its early stages with AD and/or vascular dementia (multi-infarct dementia). The overlap of neuropathologies and presenting symptoms (cognitive, emotional, and motor) may therefore make an accurate differential diagnosis difficult to make. CLINICAL SYMPTOMS According to the revised criteria for the clinical diagnosis of DLB (McKeith et al. 1996), the clinical diagnosis of probable or possible DLB essentially requires a progressive disabling mental impairment. At least two of three core features are sufficient for the diagnosis of probable DLB, and one for possible DLB. The three core features consist of fluctuation, visual hallucinations, and parkinsonism. The suggestive clinical features include rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder, severe neuroleptic sensitivity, and a low dopamine transporter uptake in the basal ganglia as demonstrated by either single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) or positron emission tomography (PET) imaging.
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
261
In the absence of two core features, the diagnosis of probable DLB can also be made if dementia plus at least one suggestive feature is present with one core feature. Possible DLB can be diagnosed with the presence of dementia plus one core or suggestive feature. Progressive Disabling Mental Impairment A progressive disabling mental impairment is a mandatory requirement for the diagnosis of DLB. This leads to the development of global dementia, sometimes over a period of months but more commonly over a period of several years. The rate of longitudinal cognitive decline for DLB is equivalent to that for AD; however, the risk of mortality for DLB is higher than that for AD. The greater risk for a progression of noncognitive symptoms (e.g., parkinsonism) for DLB than for AD is considered to result in the clinically meaningful differences in these two disorders. The demonstration of cognitive impairment by formal testing of the mental status such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is an essential component in establishing the diagnosis. However, prominent or persistent memory impairment may not necessarily occur in the early stages because of the relative preservation of confrontation naming and short- and medium-term recall as well as recognition for patients with DLB. In such cases, a prominent or persistent memory impairment become usually evident with progression over time. Parkinson’s Disease Dementia (PDD) and DLB Parkinson’s disease (PD) is one of the most common neurodegenerative diseases. It is characterized by the progressive degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra and the accumulation of Lewy bodies in the surviving neurons. PD belongs to a group of conditions called movement disorders. It is characterized by muscle rigidity, tremor, a slowing of the physical movement (bradykinesia), and, in extreme cases, a loss of physical movement (akinesia). The primary symptoms are the results of a decreased stimulation of the motor cortex by the basal ganglia, normally caused by the insufficient formation and action of dopamine, which is produced in the dopaminergic neurons of the brain. Secondary symptoms may include a high-level cognitive dysfunction and subtle language problems. PD is both chronic and progressive. Many patients with PD develop dementia, typically 10 years or more after the onset of motor symptoms. Such patients are diagnosed to have Parkinson disease dementia (PDD). The term PDD should be used to
262
Dementia
describe dementia that occurs in the context of well-established PD. The term DLB should be diagnosed when dementia occurs either before or concurrently with parkinsonism. When a patient developed parkinsonism more than one year before developing dementia, then a diagnosis of PDD is given. When parkinsonism and dementia begin within one year or if the parkinsonism starts after the onset of dementia, then a diagnosis of DLB is made. This is called the “one year rule.” The clinical distinction between DLB and PDD is based solely on the temporal sequence in the appearance of symptoms. No major differences between DLB and PDD have been found in any variable examined, including the cognitive profile, attentional performance, neuropsychiatric features, sleep disorders, autonomic dysfunction, type and severity of parkinsonism, neuroleptic sensitivity, and responsiveness to cholinesterase inhibitors. Upon autopsy, DLB and PDD are also difficult to distinguish because abnormal neuronal alpha-synuclein inclusions are the common pathologic process of both PDD and DLB. Fluctuation Fluctuation in the cognitive function is common in DLB. In the earliest stages, patients may show deficits in their cognitive function and global performance that alternate with periods of normal or near-normal performance. Fluctuation may be based on pronounced variations in attention and alertness. DLB patients may show improved performance in response to environmental novelty and increased arousal (sometimes confounding formal cognitive testing), but these effects are usually only short-lived. The periodicity and amplitude of fluctuations are variable, both between the subjects and within the same individual. They are described as occurring rapidly (lasting minutes or hours), as well as slower (weekly or monthly) variations. Substantial changes in the mental status and behavior may therefore be seen both within the duration of a single interview and/or between consecutive examinations. No typical diurnal pattern of fluctuation has been identified in DLB. Some patients identify the variable cognitive state themselves, but generally the most productive approach for identifying such fluctuation is via a reliable informant. The fluctuations resemble signs of delirium without any identifiable precipitants of such mental-status changes. The report of fluctuations in DLB is widely discrepant and it ranges from 13% to 85% with a low clinician inter-rater reliability. There are inconsistencies among studies regarding what is considered sufficient to constitute the designation of fluctuations. To some degree, most people can experience some variability
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
263
in cognition, abilities, or alertness. Therefore, for clinical purposes, it is imperative to identify those aspects of fluctuations that are particularly prominent to DLB and that can be consistently elicited from informant reports. The differential diagnosis of fluctuating cognition may include several conditions, including delirium due to medication toxicity or intercurrent illness. There are substantial difficulties inherent in defining and quantifying fluctuating cognition, particularly later in the illness when variability may become submerged in progressive cognitive deterioration. Questions such as “are there episodes when his/her thinking seems to be quite clear and then later becomes muddled?” were previously suggested as useful probes, but a recent study found 72% of both AD and DLB care givers respond positively. The study using the Mayo Fluctuations Composite Scale (Ferman et al. 2004) suggested that 4 of 19 items of structured questionnaire assessing fluctuations were found to significantly differentiate DLB from AD. These four items are as follows: 1. Are there times when the patient’s flow of ideas seems disorganized, unclear, or not logical? 2. How often is the patient drowsy and lethargic during the day? a. All the time or several times a day b. Once a day or less 3. How much time does the patient spend sleeping during the day (before 7: 00 pm)? c. 2 hours or more d. Less than 2 hours” 4. Does the patient stare into space for long periods of time? Three or more “positive” responses from caregivers to these four questions yield a positive predictive value of 83% for the clinical diagnosis of DLB against an alternate diagnosis of AD. Conversely, two or less “positive” responses yield a negative predictive value of 70% for the absence of a clinical diagnosis of DLB in favor of AD. Visual Hallucinations Visual hallucinations, which are typically recurrent, formed, and detailed, have been described by most groups investigating DLB. Visual hallucinations appear to be the only psychotic symptom that reliably discriminates DLB from AD. They are generally present early in the course of illness. Hallucinations in other modalities, particularly auditory, may also occur in DLB but do so less frequently. Informant-based assessment
264
Dementia
tools such as the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) are helpful both to screen for visual hallucinations and to assess their severity and frequency but do not always distinguish them from hallucinations in other sensory modalities. Caregivers tend to underreport visual hallucinations and patients with mild to moderate cognitive impairment can contribute useful information about their presence and quality. Patients with mild to moderate DLB tend to remember their experience of visual hallucinations because of their relatively preserved memory function. Prominent cognitive impairment on visuoperceptual and spatial functions may be one of the causes of visual hallucination. Patients with DLB with visual hallucinations show more profound visuoperceptual dysfunction in comparison to those without hallucinations. There is considerable overlap between true visual hallucinatory symptoms (in the absence of an adequate external stimulus) and other perceptual disorders, including misidentification syndromes and visual agnosias. Patients may describe visual hallucinations, such as seeing faces emerging out of the patterns on chair cushions. Typical themes are animate objects of people or animals intruding into the patient’s home. Inanimate objects can also be seen. Abstract perceptions such as characteristics on walls or ceiling are not unusual. The visual hallucinations are characteristically seen and described in considerable detail. Emotional responses vary through fear, amusement, or indifference, and a degree of insight into their unreality is often present. The precise descriptions of visual hallucination in DLB are similar to those described in association with delirium due to systemic disturbances. Antiparkinsonian medications, such as levodopa or anticholinergics could also be the cause of visual hallucinations as a side effect. Antiparkinsonian medications are often used for parkinsonisan symptoms in patients with DLB. However, their role of the hallucinatory symptoms of DLB has not yet been systematically investigated. Visual hallucinations that do not recede, or vanish very slowly, after the withdrawal of antiparkinsonian medications in PD patients may therefore be predictive of a subsequent progressive cognitive decline and dementia. Increased numbers of Lewy bodies in the anterior and inferior temporal lobe and amygdale at autopsy are associated with the presence and onset of visual hallucinations. Each of these areas is implicated in the generation of complex visual images. Brain perfusion imaging demonstrates a reduced occipital uptake in areas identified as primary and secondary visual cortex in DLB patients. Visual hallucinations are associated with greater deficits in cortical acetylcholine and their presence may predict a good response to cholinergic therapy.
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
265
Parkinsonism The severity of extrapyramidal motor features in DLB is generally similar to that of age-matched patients with PD either with or without dementia with an average 10% annual progression rate. Rigidity and bradykinesia are the usual extrapyramidal symptoms, while other common findings are hypophonic speech, masked faces, a stooped posture, and a slow and shuffling gait. Resting tremors are less common, especially in older individuals. The assessment of motor features may be complicated by the presence of a cognitive impairment. The order of onset of mental and motor symptoms is variable, particularly in older patients who often present with a complex admixture of extrapyramidal and mental symptoms of almost simultaneous onset. In advanced AD and other dementias, parkinsonian signs may also be found. Parkinsonism appearing for the first time late in the course of a dementia is therefore consistent with a diagnosis of DLB, but it is not specific for it. Neuroleptics, even at low doses, may induce parkinsonism in elderly or demented patients. DLB may thus be distinguished from druginduced parkinsonism by the persistence of motor symptoms after the withdrawal of neuroleptics. Levodopa responsiveness in DLB is almost certainly less than that in uncomplicated PD, possibly because of intrinsic striatal degeneration and the fact that a significant proportion of the parkinsonian symptoms may be non-dopaminergic in origin. However, levodopa can be used for the motor disorder of both DLB and PDD. Medication should generally be introduced at low doses and thereafter be increased slowly to the minimum required dose in order to minimize any potential disability without exacerbating the psychiatric symptoms. However, the administration of anticholinergics should be avoided.
REM Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD) There is a clear electroencephalographic (EEG) difference between sleep and the waking state in the human brain. EEG is the recording of electrical activity along the scalp produced by the firing of neurons within the brain. The EEG during sleep is divided into at least two categories. One type of sleep was found to be associated with the occurrence of dreams and the other with nondream sleep. Since dream sleep was found to be accompanied by episodes of REM, this state is often called REM sleep. Nondream sleep is also called non-REM sleep. Another prominent component of REM sleep is the profound paralysis of the skeletal muscles. REM sleep
266
Dementia
paralysis has been shown to be due to a small region of the dorsal pons, the nucleus subcoeruleus. A lesion to this nucleus abolishes the REM sleep paralysis. A very dramatic observation is that during REM sleep, cats with such lesions were observed to become very active and agitated, as if they were acting out an emotionally charged dream episode. Additional nuclei and neurotransmitters of the lower brainstem participate in the process of muscle atonia that occur during REM sleep. Patients with DLB often show the REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), which is manifested by vivid and often frightening dreams during REM sleep without muscle atonia. Patients therefore appear to “act out their dreams” vocalizing, flailing limbs, and moving around the bed sometimes violently. Vivid visual images are often reported, although the patient may have little recall of these episodes. The history is obtained from the bed partner, who may report many years of this sleep disorder prior to the onset of dementia and parkinsonism. RBD is frequently associated with an underlying synucleinopathy—PD, DLB, or multiple system atrophy (MSA)—and only rarely with other neurodegenerative disorders such as AD. Associated sleep disorders in DLB including excessive daytime drowsiness may also contribute to the fluctuating pattern. Screening questions about the presence of day- and nighttime sleep disturbance should always be asked, facilitated by the use of sleep questionnaires, particularly those that query bed partners about a history of repeated episodes of “acting out dreams.” The diagnosis of RBD may also be confirmed by polysomnography. Severe Neuroleptic Sensitivity Neuroleptics (also called antipsychotics) are a group of psychoactive drugs commonly but not exclusively used to treat psychosis, which is typified by schizophrenia, but can also be present in severe bipolar disorder, as well as many other conditions. Neuroleptics were originally developed to treat schizophrenia. Recently, these drugs have also come to be used to treat nonpsychotic disorders. For example, some neuroleptics (haloperidol) are used to treat Tourette syndrome, whereas aripiprazole and risperidone are prescribed in some cases of Asperger syndrome. Some neuroleptics such as quetiapine have multiple uses including acting as an augmentation agent in the treatment of mental illness such as anxiety, insomnia, autism, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuroleptics (antipsychotics) are broadly divided into two groups, the typical antipsychotics and the atypical antipsychotics. Atypical antipsychotics are generally considered to be more effective for the treatment of psychiatric symptoms, such as
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
267
delusion or hallucination, and to have fewer adverse effects, including parkinsonism, in comparison to typical antipsychotics. The behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) are common and problematic in clinical practice and represent a significant part of the day-today workload of the old-age psychiatry teams in hospitals, institutions, and community settings. Atypical antipsychotics may be used off-label to treat BPSD, particularly in care homes for the elderly. A severe adverse reaction to medication with neuroleptics often occurs in patients with DLB. Neuroleptics can bring about the appearance or severe exacerbation of extrapyramidal signs in DLB. Severe neuroleptic reactions include rigidity, reduced consciousness, pyrexia, falling, postural hypotension, and collapse. Approximately 50% of all patients with DLB receiving typical or atypical antipsychotic agents do not react so adversely. Therefore, a history of neuroleptic tolerance does not rule out a diagnosis of DLB. In contrast, a positive history of severe neuroleptic sensitivity is strongly suggestive of DLB. The deliberate use of neuroleptics as a diagnostic tool for DLB should be avoided because a previous study reported that a high morbidity and mortality associated with neuroleptic sensitivity reactions of DLB which are characterized by the acute onset or exacerbation of parkinsonism and impaired consciousness. Dopamine Transporter Imaging Functional imaging of the dopamine transporter (DAT) defines the integrity of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic system and currently has its main clinical application in assisting the diagnosis of DLB. Imaging with specific ligands for DAT provides a marker for presynaptic neuronal degeneration. DAT imaging is abnormal in idiopathic PD, MSA, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Low striatal DAT activity also occurs in DLB but it is normal in AD, thus making DAT scanning particularly useful for distinguishing DLB from AD. Depression Depression is common in both DLB and PDD and there have been no systematic studies of its management to date. Neuroimaging The ability to diagnose the cause of dementia could be of great medical benefit. Magnetic resonance (MR) imaging, SPECT, and PET have been
268
Dementia
investigated with the aim of establishing an early diagnosis. PET has higher sensitivity and higher spatial resolution than SPECT, thus making it more appropriate for the diagnosis of early-stage dementias (Ishii and Minoshima 2005). However, SPECT has the advantage of lower cost and has been widely used in general hospitals, and therefore it is useful in many clinical examinations. During the last several years, a voxel-based analysis of brain PET and SPECT images has been widely applied for the clinical diagnosis of AD and dementia using PET or SPECT. To promote further objective and reproducible data, there has also been recent interest in the development and application of automated algorithms for brain PET/SPECT images. Ishii et al. (2009) developed a fully automated diagnosis system for early AD and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) using the NEUROSTAT program for the analysis of FDG-PET images (Kono et al. 2007). Ishii et al. (2009) further developed this system to provide a fully automated diagnosis of early-stage neurodegenerative dementia. They aimed to distinguish AD/DLB from non-AD/DLB dementia and then DLB from AD for therapeutic decisionmaking. According to their report, diagnoses by experienced neuroradiologists were more accurate in patients with very mild AD than those by radiologists whose subspecialty were not neuroradiology, although their accuracy slightly decreased when diagnosing DLB. The problem is that it is very difficult to distinguish a patient with mild DLB from a patient with mild late-onset AD. On the contrary, this automated system is independent of observer skill and showed good results comparable to those achieved by experienced observers. The automated diagnosis system’s diagnostic value was therefore considered to be comparable to that of experienced neuroradiologists. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE The clinical diagnosis of DLB can be difficult because of the variability and the overlap of symptoms between DLB and other related dementias, notably AD. The clinical manifestation of DLB and AD can be very similar. Both DLB patients and AD patients may initially present with a progressive cognitive decline without any other neurological abnormalities. The clinical diagnosis of DLB is supported and facilitated by the revised criteria for the clinical diagnosis of DLB (McKeith et al. 2005). In most studies examining the clinical criteria for the operational diagnosis of DLB, the specificity of the diagnosis has been high, but the sensitivity has been poor. From a neuropsychological standpoint, patients with DLB tend to manifest greater attentional and visuospatial cognitive impairments than
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
269
those with AD, whereas patients with AD involve more profound episodic memory impairment than those with DLB. Using those neuropsychological differences, a careful cognitive assessment may therefore aid in the differential diagnosis between DLB and AD. Neuropsychological research on DLB can provide theoretical insight into the nature of the underlying impairments. There has been a need for studies examining the neuropsychological profile of DLB and the contribution of a neuropsychological evaluation to the diagnostic workup. In the rest of this chapter, the cognitive functions of patients with DLB including the attentional function, visuoperceptual and executive functions, and memory function will be reviewed. Attentional Function Attention is the cognitive process of selectively concentrating on one aspect of the environment while ignoring other things. Attention has also been referred to as the allocation of processing resources. Attention is a multi-dimensional concept that describes different aspects of processing and responding to information, including automatic processes such as visual orienting and higher-level processes of attentional control. There have been studies demonstrating a greater attentional impairment in DLB than in AD. Hansen et al. (1990) compared nine patients with DLB with nine patients with AD. More severe deficits of attentional function (digit span sub-test from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleRevised [WAIS-R]) were seen in DLB. Sahgal et al. (1992) reported that DLB patients had significantly greater impairment on a computerized delayed matching-to-sample task. Ayre et al. (1998) used the Cognitive Drug Research Computerized Assessment System for Dementia Patients (COGDRAS-D) computerized test battery to compare attention in 46 patients with AD and 24 patients with DLB. The DLB group performed significantly worse on simple reaction time (SRT) and choice reaction time (CRT) tasks and digit vigilance (VIG) in comparison to the AD group. Ballard et al. (2001) compared 85 patients with DLB with 80 patients with AD using the COGDRAS-D. They reported a slowed processing speed, attentional impairments, and fluctuations in attentional impairments to be significantly more severe in DLB than AD patients. The DLB patients were significantly more impaired than the AD patients on all tests of attention and fluctuating attention. In both DLB and AD, most measures of attentional performance and most indices of fluctuating attention were significantly correlated with the MMSE score. The severity and fluctuation of attentional impairments are
270
Dementia
particularly pronounced in DLB patients with MMSE scores of 10 or less. They concluded that their results confirmed that the attentional deficits and fluctuations in attention are substantially more severe in DLB patients than in patients with AD. A number of other factors, such as parkinsonism with a slowed motor speed, depression, or a general slowing of the cognitive processing speed could theoretically have contributed to these findings. They noticed that deficits of attention became more pronounced with increasing dementia severity and, hence, that these deficits need to be interpreted within the context of overall cognitive deficits. Oda et al. (2009) compared 26 patients with DLB with 78 patients with AD and demonstrated that patients with AD had significantly greater scores on the weighted sum score of the attention of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) than did patients with DLB (P = 0.0010). The overall pattern is consistent, with DLB patients thus showing a significantly greater impairment on a range of attentional tasks. Both neuropsychological and clinical observations strongly suggest that DLB patients experience great difficulty in maintaining attention. The neural basis of the attentional impairment in DLB requires further investigation, but it is likely that a dysfunction of the basal forebrain cholinergic system is involved. Several lines of evidence support this proposal. Cholinergic neuronal loss and the depletion of choline acetyltransferase are seen early in DLB (Tiraboschi et al. 2002). The administration of anticholinergic drugs can disturb the attention and cause hallucinations, whereas cholinesterase inhibitors can improve cognition in DLB. Visuoperceptual Function Visuoperceptual function is the ability to perceive an object’s visual properties (such as shape, color, and texture) and apply semantic attributes to the object, which includes the understanding of its use, previous experience with the object and how it relates to others. Numerous studies have observed greater impairments in DLB in comparison to AD on visuoperceptual tasks. Ala et al. (2001) compared 17 patients with autopsy-confirmed DLB and 27 patients with autopsyconfirmed AD by using copies of the double pentagon from the MMSE. They showed that only two patients with DLB drew the pentagon acceptably, in contrast with 16 AD patients, and that an unacceptable copy of the pentagon was associated with DLB with a sensitivity of 88% and a specificity of 59%. They concluded that their results confirmed the greater visuoperceptual impairment of patients with DLB than for the patients with AD and thus suggested that the pentagon copying task of the MMSE
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
271
may be useful in a diagnostic sense. Cormack et al. (2004) reported that patients with DLB were found to draw significantly worse double pentagons than those with AD or PD. In their report, a correlation between the MMSE score and the pentagon drawing score was observed in patients with AD; however, DLB patients did not show any significant correlation between the MMSE scores and the pentagon drawing score. In order to investigate the hypothesis that DLB patients have a different neuropsychological basis to their drawing impairments in comparison to the other dementia groups, the global cognitive performance of subjects was measured using the cognitive section of the Cambridge Mental Disorders in the Elderly Examination (CAMCOG). As a result, the pentagon copying scores were found to correlate significantly with all CAMCOG subscales except for the visual and recent memory in the AD group, whereas the scores of DLB patients’ scores only significantly correlated with Praxis and Perception. This result suggested that constructional disability was proportionate to global cognitive impairment in the AD group, but there was a dissociation of the constructional ability from the global cognitive ability in the DLB group. Mori et al. (2000) addressed problems in visual perception in patients with DLB and compared them with patients with AD. They assessed the visual perception of 24 patients with DLB and 48 patients with DLB using a subset of the object and spatial vision test battery. The discrimination of the object size task was used to examine elementary visual perception, the form discrimination task was used to examine more complex visuoperceptual function that requires the analysis of two-dimensional visual stimuli, the overlapping figure identification task was used to examine the ability to actively extract concrete shapes and to recognize objects, and the visual counting task was used to examine the ability to explore and identify the spatial relationship of visual stimuli to count targets without duplication or omission. They found that DLB subjects performed more poorly than the AD group, not only in discriminating size and form and visual counting, but also in identifying overlapping figures. Moreover, DLB subjects with visual hallucinations performed significantly worse on the overlapping figures task. Oda et al. reported that patients with DLB scored significantly worse on the Block Design, Object Assembly, and Digit Symbol subtests of the WAIS-R than did patients with AD (Oda, Yamamoto, and Maeda 2009). Because the set of the Block Design, Object Assembly, and Digit Symbol is considered to be involved in visual perception/processing meaningful stimuli and visual organization, these results were considered to suggest that patients with DLB have a more severe impairment of both their visual perception of meaningful stimuli and visual organization than do
272
Dementia
AD patients. They also showed that except for Comprehension, Similarities, and Object Assembly, all subtests and IQ of the WAIS-R showed a significant correlation with the MMSE score in the AD group. This suggests that the fall in IQ is proportional to the global cognitive impairment in the AD group. However, in the DLB group, no correlation was found between all subtests of the WAIS-R and MMSE score. There seemed to be a dissociation of the IQ from the global cognitive abilities in the DLB group. They ascribed the lack of any correlation between the global cognitive impairment and the fall in intellectual ability in the DLB group to a selective impairment of the visuoperceptual function in addition to a global cognitive impairment. The fact that visual perceptual disturbances in patients with DLB predispose them to experience visual hallucinations has important clinical implications. First, because visual hallucinations are among the strongest diagnostic predictors of DLB, the neuropsychological assessment of visual perceptual and constructional functions is critical in suspected DLB and its differentiation from AD. Indeed, visuoconstructional tasks, in combination with other tests, can differentiate DLB from normal aging and from AD with high sensitivity and specificity. Furthermore, a poor performance on visuoperceptual and constructional tasks may indicate the need for more careful monitoring regarding the occurrence of hallucinations. It is likely that the occipital dysfunction is implicated in visuoperceptual abnormalities of DLB and both the ventral occipitotemporal and dorsal occipitoparietal streams have been implicated. The visuoperceptual dysfunction in DLB can be attributed to accentuated damage in the occipital lobes. Albin et al. (1996) demonstrated the regional glucose metabolism to decrease in the occipital association cortex and primary visual area in six patients with autopsy proved DLB. In the study of Ishii et al. (1998), using 18F-fluorodeoxyglucose and PET, the glucose metabolic rate in the occipital cortices was found to be significantly lower in patients with probable DLB than in controls with probable AD matched for age, sex, disease duration, and MMSE score, despite similar decreases in the parietotemporal lobe in patients with DLB and AD. Similarly, a SPECT study demonstrated the occipital blood flow to be significantly lower in patients with DLB than in patients with AD. Therefore, in DLB, not only does the parietotemporal damage provoke visuocognitive dysfunctions, but occipital damage also causes disturbances of visual sensations, while also intensifying the higherorder visuocognitive dysfunctions. Defective visual perception, resulting in illusions including distortions of form, size, movement, or color, in combination with general defects such as confusion and mental deterioration may cause a sense of strangeness or inexplicable familiarity.
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
273
The mechanism of occipital involvement and visuoperceptual deficits in DLB is highly speculative. Bashir et al. (1998) reported a unique patient with DLB who initially complained of heaviness in the right upper extremity and then subsequently developed a dense left homonymous hemianopsia during the course of rapidly progressing dementia. Their patient fulfilled all the consensus criteria for the clinical diagnosis of probable DLB: their case exhibited a progressive cognitive decline, parkinsonism, visual hallucinations, and fluctuating agitation, confusion, and depression. The neuropathologic findings in their patient fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for DLB, proposed by the Consortium on Dementia with Lewy Bodies (McKeith et al. 1996). In addition, their patient exhibited a striking predominance of neurofibrillary tangles in the right inferotemporal and occipital cortices. However, in general, the pathologic features of DLB (including Lewy bodies) hardly affect the occipital lobes. In a PET study with (+)-[11C]-dihydrotetrabenazine, a greater reduction of the blood-to-brain ligand transport occurred in occipital cortex in DLB than in AD. Bodis-Wollner (1990) speculated that in patients affected by PD, as well as in the monkey model of this disease, the visual defects may be caused by a systemic dopaminergic deficiency. Conversely, involvement of the occipital cholinergic system also has been assumed. The activity of a cholinergic enzyme, namely choline acetyltransferase, is reportedly lower in the temporoparietal and occipital neocortex in patients with DLB in comparison to those with AD. Memory Memory is one’s ability to remember the information that one has received previously. From the neuropsychological standpoint, there are three main stages in the process of memory: registration, storage, and recall. In the registration stage, one enters new information. In the storage stage, one stores registered information whether one is conscious of it or not. In the recall stage, one draws upon stored information when it is required. Persons with a normal memory function can fail to recall learned information (e.g., an examinee who forgets something he has studied). In many such cases, one is able to recall forgotten information with the aid of a hint or a cue. This means that the main problem of normal forgetfulness is in the recall stage, not in the storage stage. One cannot draw upon the stored information by the aid of a hint or a cue if he or she has failed to store it in the storage stage. Patients with dementia often fail to recall the registered information even if they are given a hint or a cue. This suggests that patients with dementia therefore have problems in the storage stage of the processing memory.
274
Dementia
With regard to memory, in general, DLB subjects perform better on tests of episodic (declarative) memory than do AD patients, and this appears to be particularly true on tests of verbal rather than visual memory. Shimomura et al. (1998) demonstrated that patients with DLB scored significantly better (P < 0.05) on the verbal memory subtest of the Alzheimer Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS) than did AD patients who were comparable in the global severity of dementia and the global assessment of cognitive impairment. To determine the degree to which elementary visual perceptual dysfunction may contribute to a visual memory impairment in DLB, Oda et al. (2009) compared DLB patients with AD patients using the WMS-R. In that study, the DLB group showed significantly better scores than did the AD group on Verbal Memory (P < 0.0001) and Delayed Recall (P < 0.0001) of the WMS-R. However, the DLB and AD groups demonstrated comparable scores on Visual Memory (26.31 ± 12.64 vs. 26.42 ± 9.81, respectively; P = 0.9222). The authors speculated that the selective visuoperceptual impairment in DLB may explain this similarity: namely, the relatively well preserved short- and medium-term recall would compensate for the severe visuoperceptual impairment in the DLB group on the visual memory tasks. Lambon et al. (2001) reported that both DLB and AD groups exhibited impaired performance across a range of tasks designed to assess semantic memory. Whereas patients with AD showed equivalent comprehension of written words and picture stimuli, patients with DLB demonstrated more severe semantic deficits for pictures than words. The major pathological substrate of more severe amnestic deficits in AD relative to DLB likely reflects the burden of neurofibrillary tangles in the entorhinal cortex and surrounding medial temporal lobe regions in AD. Neuropsychological Differentiation from AD The diagnosis of DLB can be difficult, in particular when trying to differentiate it from AD. Neuroimaging techniques such as SPECT and PET have the ability to differentiate DLB from AD with high sensitivity and specificity. Neuroimaging techniques are often extremely costly and require a complex clinical setting whereas neuropsychological examinations tend to have a low cost and are practical in the general clinical setting. In addition, there are many neuropsychological differences between DLB and AD. The third report of the DLB consortium mentioned that a “double discrimination” can help differentiate DLB from AD, with the relative preservation of confrontation naming and short- and medium-term recall as well as recognition, and a greater impairment on verbal fluency, visual perception, and performance tasks (McKeith et al. 2005).
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
275
The MMSE is one of the widely used and validated tests for measuring the level of global cognitive impairment. The MMSE is commonly used in medicine to screen for dementia. It is also used to estimate the severity of cognitive impairment at a given point in time and to follow the course of cognitive changes in an individual over time. In the time span of about 10 minutes it samples various functions, including arithmetic, memory, and orientation. The MMSE test consists of simple questions that cover various cognitive functions, such as orientation, registration, attention, recall, and visual construction. Therefore, the score of each subtest may be useful information for both diagnosing dementia as well as the total score. Ala et al. (2002) reported a retrospective study in which pathologically confirmed cases of AD and DLB could be differentiated on the basis of a subscore derived from the MMSE. Based on the greater impairment of the attentional and visuospatial functions, and the relative preservation of memory function in DLB compared with AD, they derived a weighted score, calculated as follows: Ala score = Attention – 5/3 Memory + Construction An Ala score <5 was associated with a pathological diagnosis of DLB with a sensitivity of 82% and a specificity of 81%. By using the Ala score and the z-score in the medial occipital lobe from a brain SPECT study, Hanyu et al. (2006) derived a combined index of SPECT/MMSE that achieved a high discrimination between DLB and AD with a sensitivity of 81% and a specificity of 85%. According to their report, patients with DLB and AD could be distinguished by their performance on a single dementia instrument of the MMSE. The DLB group performed significantly worse than the AD group on the Attention and Copy design, while the AD group demonstrated poorer performance than the DLB group on the Word Recall. Oda et al. (2009) derived a weighted score consisting of the Object Assembly subtest of the WAIS-R and the Logical Memory II subtest of the WMS-R to differentiate DLB from AD15 that had a sensitivity of 81% and a specificity of 76%. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Given that DLB is a relatively new disease concept, most of the work so far has been concerned with the first step of the characterization and description of DLB as a separate disease. Most of these studies suggest that in the early stages of the disease, DLB patients tend to exhibit pronounced visual-perceptual, attentional, and frontal executive impairments, whereas the memory functions are generally less impaired than in AD patients. However, given the overlap and variability of the symptoms, the
276
Dementia
neuropsychological profile of DLB has not yet been clearly distinguished from that of AD. In the future, the challenge of DLB research will lie in developing a theoretical model that can link evidence from pathophysiological and imaging studies with clinical and neuropsychological data, which will therefore facilitate the treatment of this disease.
REFERENCES Ala, T. A., L. F. Hughes, G. A. Kyrouac, M. W. Ghobrial, and R. J. Elble. 2001. Pentagon copying is more impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies than in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 70: 483–488. Ala, T. A., L. F. Hughes, G. A. Kyrouac, M. W. Ghobrial, and R. J. Elble. 2002. The Mini-Mental State exam may help in the differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 17: 503–509. Albin, R. L., S. Minoshima, C. J. D’Amato, K. A. Frey, D. A. Kuhl, and A. A. Sima. 1996. Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography in diffuse Lewy body disease. Neurology 47: 462–466. Ayre, G., C. Ballard, C. Pincock, I. McKeith, A. Sahgal, and K. Wesnes. 1998. Double dissociation between dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer ’s disease on tests of attentional and mnemonic function: The role of the basal forebrain. J Psychopharmacol A12 (suppl): A64. Ballard, C., J. O’Brien, A. Gray, F. Cormack, G. Ayre, E. Rowan, P. Thompson, et al. 2001. Attention and fluctuating attention in patients with dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 58: 977–982. Bashir, K., R. J. Elble, M. Ghobrial, and R. G. Struble. 1998. Hemianopsia in dementia with Lewy bodies. Arch Neurol 55: 1132–1135. Bodis-Wollner, I. 1990. Visual deficits related to dopamine deficiency in experimental animals and Parkinson’s disease patients. Trends Neurosci 13: 296–302. Cormack, F., D. Aarsland, C. Ballard, and M. J. Tovee. 2004. Pentagon drawing and neuropsychological performance in dementia with Lewy Bodies, Alzheimer ’s disease, Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 19: 371–377. Ferman, T. J., G. E. Smith, B. F. Boeve, R. J. Ivnik, R. C. Petersen, D. Knopman, N. Graff-Radford, J. Parisi, and D. W. Dickson. 2004. DLB fluctuations: Specific features that reliably differentiate DLB from AD and normal aging. Neurology 62 (2): 181–187. Hansen, L., D. Salmon, D. Galasko, E. Masliah, R. Katzman, R. DeTeresa, L. Thal, et al. 1990. The Lewy body variant of Alzheimer ’s disease: A clinical and pathologic entity. Neurology 40 (1): 1–8. Hanyu, H., S. Shimizu, K. Hirao, H. Kanetaka, H. Sakurai, T. Iwamoto, K. Koizumi, and K. Abe. 2006. Differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer ’s disease using Mini-Mental State Examination and brain perfusion SPECT. J Neurol Sci 250: 97–102.
Neuropsychological Profile of Dementia with Lewy Bodies
277
Ikeda, K., T. Yoshimura, and H. Kato. 1975. A case of idiopathic parkinsonism with many Lewy bodies in the cerebral cortex (in Japanese). Brain Nerve 27: 733–742. Ishii, K., T. Imamura, M. Sasaki, S. Yamaji, S. Sakamoto, H. Kitagaki, M. Hashimoto, N. Hirono, T. Shimomura, and E. Mori. 1998. Regional cerebral glucose metabolism in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 51: 125–130. Ishii, K., T. Kanda, T. Uemura, N. Miyamoto, T. Yoshikawa, K. Shimada, S. Ohkawa, and S. Minoshima. 2009. Computer-assisted diagnostic system for neurodegenerative dementia using brain SPECT and 3D-SSP. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 36 (5): 831–840. Ishii, K., and S. Minoshima. 2005. PET is better than perfusion SPECT for early diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 32 (12): 1463–1465. Kono, A. K., K. Ishii, K. Sofue, N. Miyamoto, S. Sakamoto, and E. Mori. 2007. Fully automatic differential diagnosis system for dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer ’s disease using FDG-PET and 3D-SSP. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 34 (9): 1490–1497. Kosaka, K. 1978. Lewy bodies in cerebral cortex: Report of three cases. Acta Neuropathol 42: 127–134. Kosaka, K. 1990. Diffuse Lewy body disease in Japan. J Neurol 237: 197–204. Kosaka, K., M. Matsushita, S. Oyanagi, and P. Mehraein. 1980. A clinicopathological study of Lewy body disease. Psychiat Neurol Jpn 83: 292–311. Kosaka, K., and P. Mehraein. 1979. Dementia–Parkinsonism syndrome with numerous Lewy bodies and senile plaques in cerebral cortex. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 226: 241–250. Kosaka, K., S. Oyanagi, M. Matsushita, et al. 1976. Presenile dementia with Alzheimer-, Pick-, and Lewy-body changes. Acta Neuropathol 36: 221–233. Kosaka, K., M. Yoshimura, K. Ikeda, and H. Budka. 1984. Diffuse type of Lewy body disease: Progressive dementia with abundant cortical Lewy bodies and senile changes of various degree—a new disease? Clin Neuropathol 3: 185–192. Lambon, R., J. Powell, D. Howard, A. Whitworth, P. Garrard, and J. Hodges. 2001. Semantic memory is impaired in both dementia with Lewy bodies and dementia of Alzheimer ’s type: A comparative neuropsychological study and literature review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 70: 149–156. McKeith, I. G., D. W. Dickson, J. Lowe, M. Emre, J. T. O’Brien, H. Feldman, J. Cummings, et al. 2005. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 65 (12): 1863–1872. McKeith, I. G., D. Galasko, K. Kosaka, E. K. Perry, D. W. Dickson, L. A. Hansen, D. P. Salmon, et al. 1996. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. Neurology 47 (5): 1113–1124.
278
Dementia
Mori, E., T. Shimomura, M. Fujimori, N. Hirono, T. Imamura, M. Hashimoto, S. Tanimukai, H. Kazui, and T. Hanihara. 2000. Visuoperceptual impairment in dementia with Lewy bodies. Arch Neurol 57: 489–493. Oda, H., Y. Yamamoto, and K. Maeda. 2009. The neuropsychological profile in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 24: 125–131. Okazaki, H., L. E. Lipkin, and S. M. Aronson. 1961. Diffuse intracy-toplasmic ganglionic inclusions (Lewy type) associated with progressive dementia and quadriparesis in flexion. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 20: 237–244. Sahgal, A., P. H. Galloway, I. G. McKeith, J. A. Edwardson, and S. Lloyd. 1992. A comparative study of attentional deficits in senile dementias of Alzheimer and Lewy body types. Dementia 3: 350–354. Shimomura, T., E. Mori, H. Yamashita, T. Imamura, N. Hirono, M. Hashimoto, S. Tanimukai, H. Kazui, and T. Hanihara. 1998. Cognitive loss in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 55: 1547–1552. Tiraboschi, P., L. A. Hansen, M. Alford, A. Merdes, E. Masliah, L. J. Thal, and J. Corey-Bloom. 2002. Early and widespread cholinergic losses differentiate dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59: 946–951. Williams, M. M., C. Xiong, J. C. Morris, and J. E. Galvin. 2006. Survival and mortality differences between dementia with Lewy bodies vs. Alzheimer disease. Neurology 67 (11): 1935–1941. Yoshimura, M. 1983. Cortical changes in the Parkinsonian brain: A contribution to the delineation of “Diffuse Lewy body disease.” J Neurol 229: 17–32.
Chapter 11
Sleep Disorders in Dementia Kesha Wilford and Sanford Auerbach
The study of sleep in dementia can be quite difficult. Human sleep is a normal complex state characterized by various behavioral and physiologic alterations. Otherwise normal, healthy adults, however, are subject to various disorders of sleep. Aging is accompanied by normal changes that occur in the behavioral and physiologic characteristics of sleep. Abnormal aging of the brain, as seen in dementia, may lead to abnormal derangements in sleep physiology. In order to create a basic understanding, we shall approach the issue of sleep in dementia by reviewing the basic elements of sleep, the normal changes associated with aging, the common disorders of sleep encountered in the general population and, then, the special problem of sleep in dementia. ELEMENTS OF NORMAL SLEEP AND CHANGES ACROSS THE LIFESPAN In the normal adult, sleep is divided into two main stages: Non–rapid eye movement (NREM) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. NREM sleep is further subdivided into three stages: N1 (stage 1), N2 (stage 2), and N3 (stage 3/4). NREM sleep is characterized behaviorally by decreased muscle tone but not complete atonia, and physiologically by a relatively synchronized EEG, sleep spindles and K complexes, slow waves, and slow eye movements, decreased but not absent chin EMG activity, and relative predominance of parasympathetic activity. REM sleep is characterized behaviorally by a loss of muscle tone, and physiologically by a desynchronized EEG, rapid eye movements, absent chin EMG activity, and a
280
Dementia
predominance of parasympathetic activity. NREM and REM sleep alternate and cycle approximately every 90 minutes throughout the night with an average night consisting of five cycles. Normal sleep commences with NREM sleep, which predominates the first half of the night. REM sleep durations progressively lengthen through the night. For a more detailed description of the nomenclature used to describe normal sleep, the reader should refer to the AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events (Ibfar et al. 2007). Sleep architecture changes through the normal lifespan. Infants display two stages of sleep, active (REM) sleep and nonactive (NREM) sleep. Unlike adults, infants enter active sleep and the sleep cycles are approximately forty minutes shorter. Active sleep is about half of total sleep time until about 2 years of age, when it decreases to 20–25% of total sleep time and remains at that percentage throughout the rest of the lifespan. Slow wave sleep (stage 3/4) decreases from adolescence into old age. As sleep physiology changes normally through life, there are a plethora of derangements that may occur in the normal aging brain and particularly in the pathologically aging brain. To understand these derangements, an overview of the anatomy of wakefulness and sleep is warranted. In a recent review, Datta (2007) summarized the current knowledge of the neurobiological mechanisms controlling wakefulness and sleep. The drive to sleep is known to be under homeostatic and circadian control. Homeostatic drive to sleep refers to the increasing need to sleep that begins upon awakening in the morning, increases as the day progresses, is maximal at night, and decreases according to duration of sleep. The circadian control of sleep refers to a 24-hour rhythm that is regulated by the light-dark cycle. Light enters the retina, and through the retinohypothalamic tract it signals the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN). The SCN is known to be the control center of all systems under oscillatory control. The pineal gland, which secretes melatonin, a potent neurochemical sleep promoter, is also under SCN control. Melatonin secretion decreases through inhibitory signals from the SCN in the presence of light. In the absence of light, melatonin is secreted and induces sleep. Wakefulness has been shown to be under neurobiological control with the discovery of wake-promoting cell populations in various areas of the brainstem, comprising the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS), as well as wake-promoting cell groups in the forebrain (which are felt to work by preventing the initiation of sleep). Brainstem wake-promoting cells have been discovered in the locus ceruleus (noradrenergic cells),
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
281
raphe nucleus (serotonergic cells), pedunculo-pontine tegmentum (PPT) containing cholinergic cells, midbrain reticular formation (unknown neurochemical), substantia nigra pars compacta (dopaminergic cells), and ventral tegmental area (dopaminergic cells). Wake-promoting cells in the forebrain include histaminergic cells of the posterior hypothalamus (PH), lateral hypothalamic (LatH) hypocretin containing cells, cholinergic cells in the basal forebrain, and the cells of the SCN. Noradrenergic cells of the locus ceruleus project to the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, thalamus, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain (BF) and have been shown to participate in cortical activation and behavioral arousal. While serotonergic cells of the raphe nucleus are known to project to the same areas as noradrenergic cells, their role in wakefulness has not been clearly delineated. They are known to fire maximally during wakefulness. Cholinergic cells of the PPT promote wakefulness by activating thalamo-cortical, hypothalamo-cortical, basalo-cortical, suprachiasmatic, and amygdaloid wake-promoting systems of the forebrain. The midbrain reticular formation (MRF), though the exact neurochemical involved has yet to be discovered, is felt to be involved in wakefulness based on neuroimaging techniques showing more activity during wakefulness than in slow wave sleep. Dopaminergic cells of the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) and the ventral tegmental area (VTA) project to the frontal cortex, striatum, limbic areas, and basal forebrain. Although it is still unclear the exact role that dopamine (DA) plays in wake promotion and sleep since these cells seem to maintain a relatively constant firing rate during sleep and wake states, studies have shown that extracellular concentrations of DA increase during wakefulness. Brainstem and basal forebrain wake-promoting areas have reciprocal as well as feedback mechanisms that help to maintain wakefulness and wake-promoting behaviors. For instance, cholinergic neurons of the basal forebrain have been shown to be involved in wake-promoting behavior such as attention, learning, and sensory processing. The onset of sleep appears to be related to the inhibition of wakepromoting areas, specifically by the buildup of various neurochemical products of neuronal activity—GABA, glycine, prostaglandin D2, iterleukin-1 beta, and tumor necrosis factor alpha. Slow-wave sleep is generated by the activation of GABA neurons in the preoptic area of the hypothalamus. REM sleep, however, has a vast network of neurotransmitters that generate the complex physiological changes that occur during REM sleep. This circuit of cells has been localized to the midbrain and pons.
282
Dementia
DISORDERS OF SLEEP: OVERVIEW Derangements in sleep may be seen throughout the lifespan and some characteristically occur during specific stages of sleep. A full description of sleep disorders can be found in the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD-2) (American Academy of Sleep Medicine 2005). At times, it may be helpful to consider these disorders in a manner that parallels the description of normal sleep physiology. For instance, there are several problems that may affect the circadian rhythms of sleep. Patients may also suffer from insomnias or hypersomnias (increased need for daily sleep). Other disorders may be more restricted to specific stages of sleep. REM sleep, for instance, may be associated with sleep paralysis (the persistence of the muscle atonia of REM sleep for a brief time after awakening from REM sleep), REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD, the loss of the usual REM atonia and the apparent acting out of REM-related dreams) or nightmare disorder. RBD is often referred to as a parasomnia or abnormal behavior associated with sleep. Other parasomnias may be associated with NREM sleep, such as sleep walking, sleep terrors, and confusional arousals. Since NREM sleep is most prominent in the young, these types of parasomnias are more common in the younger patients. Other parasomnias may occur from any stage of sleep and include sleeprelated dissociative disorders, sleep enuresis, sleep-related groaning, exploding head syndrome, sleep-related hallucinations or sleep-related eating disorder. The incidence of sleep-disordered breathing (obstructive sleep apnea, OSA) increases with age and may be seen more commonly in the elderly. There are still other disorders, such as restless legs syndrome (RLS), which is actually a disorder of wakefulness that interferes with sleep onset. The pathologically aging brain as seen in dementia patients is susceptible to all sleep-related pathology. Certain sleep disorders are seen with more frequency in dementia and will be discussed by individual dementing illness.
SPECIFIC DISORDERS Circadian rhythm disorders (CRDs) occur when the normal 24-hour sleep-wake cycle is shifted. Advanced sleep-phase syndrome describes a normal sleep-wake cycle that begins earlier than usual and ends earlier. It is associated with melatonin secretion and core body temperature decline occurring at an earlier time than usual. In the elderly it is commonly thought to be related to loss of social cues or eye pathology that block normal light cues. Sleep time is normal, but the onset of sleep and the
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
283
end of sleep occur later in the night and later in the morning, respectively. Irregular sleep-wake cycle occurs when there is a normal amount of sleep, but melatonin secretion and core body temperature vary throughout the day. This leads to irregular lengths of sleep time that vary throughout the night and day. Normal aging is usually associated with tendency to advance the circadian rhythm, due in part to biological and social factors. Treatment of CRDs is most successful with chronotherapy, light therapy, and melatonin. Restless legs syndrome (RLS) is characterized by a compelling urge to move the legs during periods of inactivity with worsening that occurs during the evening. Commonly, there are parasthesias with uncomfortable or unpleasant feeling in the legs while immobile that is relieved by movement of the limb. Although technically a disorder of wakefulness, RLS enters this discussion of sleep disorders because of the impact on the ability to transition into sleep. Ferritin levels less than 50 ng/mL, neuropathy, and renal failure are common risk factors. Certain medications like antidepressants or antihistamines may aggravate or trigger this condition. A positive response to dopaminergic agents is oftentimes considered to be supportive evidence. Iron replacement may improve symptoms in individuals with low ferritin levels. Antipsychotics and other dopaminergic antagonists may lead to the development of akasthesias or restlessness that may be quite similar in presentation. Treatment usually consists of addressing possible aggravating factors and then the use of either dopaminergic agents, selected anticonvulsants, benzodiazepines, or opioids. Since the diagnosis is largely based on the description of a subjective experience, there may be particular problems with diagnosis in the cognitively impaired population. Periodic limb movements (PLMs) are brief, repetitive, and stereotyped movements of the limbs that occur every 5 to 90 seconds at a frequency of at least 15 limb movements per hour. These are not problematic unless they are associated with RLS symptoms, causing arousals and daytime symptoms, or affecting the sleep of bed partners. There are no FDA recommended treatments for PLMs at this time. The parasomnia that is of particular interest in the discussion of sleep in dementia is REM behavior disorder (RBD). RBD occurs when there is a loss of the normal REM atonia leading to complex motor activity during REM sleep with an apparent acting out of often violent dreams. This may occur in the normal population, though usually an older population, and it may be triggered by certain medications like antidepressants. Although RBD has also been associated with brainstem lesions, especially those involving the pons, it has been particularly associated with the
284
Dementia
group of neurodegenerative disorders referred to as synucleinopathies. RBD may actually precede the emergence of other symptoms of this group of degenerative disorders by several years. Treatment starts with ensuring the safety of the patient and the bed partner if there is a concern for injury. Removal of aggravating factors or medications may be helpful. Clonazepam and/or melatonin can also be used for the treatment of RBD. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) may be attributed to disrupted or inadequate nocturnal sleep, but may also be related to hypersomnia with an increased need for sleep during a 24-hour period. At times, hypersomnia may be a component of neurodegenerative dementias, but medications and depression need to also be considered as causes of hypersomnia in this population. Depression, in particular, is often overlooked as a cause of hypersomnia in the group of patients. Insomnia, on the other hand, is described as an inability to initiate or maintain sleep with resultant daytime dysfunction, in the setting of appropriate sleep opportunity and sleep environment. Many medical factors need to be considered in the assessment of insomnia, especially in the older patient where medications, pain problems, and urges to urinate through the night need to be considered. Anxiety is another factor that may contribute to insomnia and is one that is often overlooked, especially in a cognitively impaired patient. Many patients suffering from insomnia considered to be idiopathic or primary actually suffer from a degree of anxiety that may not reach criteria for a formal diagnosis of an anxiety disorder. Obviously, these issues may be difficult to delineate in a cognitively impaired patient. Sleep-disordered breathing refers to obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) and central sleep apnea (CSA). Obstructive sleep apnea occurs in the setting of a sleep-related obstruction, either in the nasopharynx or hypopharynx, which causes decreased airflow in spite of breathing effort. The obstruction is usually a function of the anatomy of the individual’s upper airway and the degree of muscle relaxation that occurs during sleep. Central sleep apnea is characterized by a pause in breathing related to absent breathing effort. Diagnosis usually requires confirmation with a formal polysomnogram or overnight sleep study. Sleep-disordered breathing is often associated with hypoxemia, fragmented sleep, and excessive daytime sleepiness. Recently an association with hypertension, impaired glucose, heart failure, pulmonary hypertension, and motor vehicle accidents has been established. The treatment of OSA is commonly with positive airway pressure, oral appliances, surgery (maxilla-mandibular advancement), or tracheostomy. Since obesity and supine sleeping position may be
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
285
aggravating factors, weight reduction and attention to sleep position may also play a role in the treatment of OSA. CSA may be caused by excessive opioid use, congestive heart failure, or other CNS disorders. Treatment of underlying factors, when possible, is recommended. There may also be a role for modifications of the positive airway pressure systems. Sleep derangements can have a detrimental effect on patients with dementia and their caregivers. The effect of poor sleep on an individual’s health and cognition is profound. Poor sleep is also a risk factor for institutionalization of the elderly with dementia because of the negative impact on the care giver. SLEEP IN DEMENTIA Ferri et al. (2005) conducted a consensus study covering the period from 1980 to 2004 and estimated that the prevalence of dementia from any cause is approximately 24 million people. The irreversible dementing illnesses may be subdivided by the main pathologic protein, which predicts the most common neurodegenerative pattern and subsequent characteristic sleep abnormalities. Certainly, it is possible to classify these dementing illnesses in various manners, but here we will be considering them according to the prominent pathological features. Amyloidopathies Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease clinically characterized by progressive memory loss and cognitive decline. Genetically, the APOE4 lipoprotein has been shown to impart risk to carriers. Pathologically, AD is characterized by neuritic plaques comprised of the dysfunctional protein amyloid, and neurofibrillary tangles that are comprised of dysfunctional tau protein. Amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles are found in all association areas of the cerebral cortex. It is of some interest that although the presence of the amyloid plaques are necessary for the diagnosis of AD, it is the density and distribution of the neurofibrillary tangles that best correlate more with dementia severity (Arriagada et al. 1992). Neurofibrillary tangles preferentially affect the hippocampus (CA1 and CA2), entorhinal cortex, subiculum, amygdala, and parietal lobes. The hypothalamus, thalamus, periaqueductal gray, pontine tegmentum, and granule-cell layer of the cerebellum are not particularly targeted (Querfurth and LaFerda 2010) Amyloid plaques occur diffusely in the interstitial space of the cerebrum, cerebellum, and brainstem.
286
Dementia
Sleep abnormalities seen in individuals with Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) are plentiful. Circadian rhythm abnormalities, nocturnal agitation or wondering, restless legs syndrome, and obstructive sleep apnea (possible link of APOE4 and sleep disordered breathing in adults [Kadotani et al. 2001]) are common. These sleep disturbances may, in part, be explained by patterns of neuronal loss, medications, comorbid medical problems, and loss of social cues. Dysfunction of the SCN has been shown to occur even in the earliest stages of AD. Wu et al. (2007) found that pineal melatonin secretion and pineal clock oscillation is dysfunctional in demented AD patients as well as nondemented patients with the earliest signs of AD. A functional disruption of the SCN was seen in the earliest stages of AD and manifested by decreased vasopressin mRNA secretion. MT1 receptors in the SCN were drastically reduced in late AD. The disconnection between the SCN and pineal melatonin secretion seems to underlie the prominent circadian rhythm disorders in AD, which then lead to nocturnal agitation and disorientation, sleep fragmentation, increased daytime sleepiness, and increased nighttime sleep latency. Treatment of circadian rhythm disorders in AD seems to respond, at least in part, to chronobiological treatment, namely bright lights in the morning and melatonin administration in the evening. It should also be kept in mind that sundowning behavior encountered in later stages of AD and other dementing disorders follows a circadian pattern. As in other circadian disorders, it follows a 24-hour rhythm. Although environmental factors and medication effects need to also be considered, it needs to be noted that behavioral/cognitive deterioration may be associated with circadian factors. Oftentimes, these points of deterioration may be associated with the “sleep-permissive” times of the day. Chronobiological treatment improves the intrinsic circadian rhythm, which may also be affected by the loss of social cues that can occur in aging. Social cues lost after retirement from work, loss of friends, loss of family, medical illness with limited mobility, and depression all affect the sleep-wake cycle and contribute to circadian rhythm disorders. Psychiatric and social supports are important resources to minimize these psychosocial impacts on sleep in demented individuals. There have also been some particular sleep changes that have been described in AD. A prominent feature of the degenerative process in AD has been the impact on the central cholinergic system of the basal forebrain. Another cluster of central cholinergic neurons in the brainstem have been implicated in REM sleep. Some studies have demonstrated a progressive deterioration in REM sleep that seems to parallel the progression of dementia. The exact implication of this observation is still speculative.
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
287
Medications used for neuromodulation and to treat behavioral symptoms in AD have side effects that affect sleep and wakefulness. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors are used to slow cognitive decline, but in some patients they may cause insomnia, hallucinations, and vivid dreams. Memantine, an NMDA receptor antagonist, may cause insomnia that may be improved by giving the second dose earlier in the evening. The atypical antipsychotics used to treat behavioral symptoms may cause excessive daytime sleepiness. Depression/anxiety is a common cause of insomnia in the normal population, as well as in demented individuals. Antidepressants used to treat depression may lead to insomnia. Anticholinergic properties of some antidepressants may worsen cognitive dysfunction. They may also precipitate RLS, PLMs, or REM sleep behavior disorder. Periodic limb movements of wakefulness and sleep (PLMs) may be idiopathic, related to underlying medical illness such as iron deficiency, renal disease, neuropathy, or related to medication effect of antidepressants. PLMs may lead to sleep onset difficulty, fragmented sleep, and daytime hypersomnolence. The most effective known treatments are treating the underlying disorder, dopaminergic medications, neurontin, or opiates. Close monitoring is required with treatment, as among other side effects, dopaminergic medications may cause hallucinations, neurontin may increase daytime sleepiness, and opiates may lead to cognitive dysfunction. RLS may also prove to be a problem in recognition in the cognitively impaired patient. Many issues may contribute to the restlessness of a demented patient. Oftentimes, it is important to look for the behavioral patterns that may suggest RLS as a trigger for restless nocturnal behavior. At times a therapeutic trial of a dopaminergic agent may be quite helpful. REM behavior disorder (RBD) has been described with AD, but is more commonly associated with the synucleinopathies. RBD may also occur as a medication side effect of some antidepressants. RBD is problematic if it causes affected individuals to act out dreams in a way that causes injury to themselves or their bed partner. The current recommended treatment is with clonazepam or melatonin. Hypersomnia caused directly by AD or other dementing illnesses has not been systematically studied. The etiology remains unclear but may be related to abnormal hypocretin production and physiology. Insomnia may be seen in AD. It is commonly related to depression, medications, or comorbid health problems. Sleep-disordered breathing is an important consideration in AD. Some of the medications used to treat sleep disorders, such as benzodiazepines,
288
Dementia
may worsen OSA. Left untreated, OSA may lead to daytime sleepiness and worsening of cognitive function. Tauopathies The frontotemporal dementias (FTDs) include Pick’s disease, primary progressive aphasia, semantic dementia, frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism, corticobasal ganglionic degeneration (CBGD), progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), and argyrophilic grain disease. These progressive dementing disorders are all caused by the accumulation of a dysfunctional tau protein associated with the emergence of neurofibrillary tangles. Although these tangles were also noted in AD, it is the amyloid plaque that is often considered the distinguishing feature of that disorder. The FTDs are characterized by degeneration of the frontal and temporal lobes preferentially. Changes in behavior help to distinguish FTD from AD, such as disinhibition, loss of empathy, changes in eating preference, stereotypic behavior, and changes in language, such as aphasia. FTD illnesses can be divided into two broad categories based on predominance of changes in behavior or changes in language. Sleep disturbances are prominent in all FTDs. Studies of sleep changes in FTD are limited. Nocturnal agitation and wandering may occur in FTDs and should be treated the same as AD. A recent small study by Anderson et al. (2009) used prolonged actigraphy and sleep diaries to study circadian rhythm disorders in FTD. The study, though limited by size and the lack of PSG correlation, showed sleep-wake disturbances in all 13 patients with clinically diagnosed FTD; the disturbances, specifically increased nocturnal activity and decreased morning activity levels, were more severe than seen in mild AD. The neuropathology of FTD may explain some of the sleep disturbances. Diffuse severe degeneration of the orbital, frontal, basal forebrain, hippocampus, and temporal areas of the brain may be the direct or indirect cause of sleep wake disturbances. Medications commonly used to treat negative behavior in FTDs may also affect sleep and wakefulness in patients with FTDs. As in AD, they may contribute to daytime somnolence, worsening of cognitive dysfunction, or insomnia. The frequency of PLMs and associated arousals in patients with FTD is unknown and has not been studied. RLS on the other hand has anecdotally been seen with frequency in FTD. Boeve (2008) reported clinical response and tolerability with treatment of RLS with carbidoba/levodopa, pramipexole, and gabapentin. Side effects of hallucinations and delusions
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
289
may prove problematic in patients with psychotic features treated with these medications. Pathologic studies of patients with REM behavior disorder have not shown a correlation between FTD and RBD. More specifically, there are no studies that show RBD in patients with pure FTD. Hypersomnia has been anecdotally described by Boeve (2008) in patients with FTD. This was seen as decreased mean-sleep latencies and sleeponset REM periods on multiple sleep-latency testing. Wake-promoting medications and stimulant medications have been effective with promoting negative neurobehavioral or cardiovascular side effects, in Boeve’s experience. FTDs also include frontotemporal lobar dementia with ubiquitin and TAR-DNA binding protein 43 positive inclusions, frontotemporal lobar dementia with motor neuron disease, and frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17 associated with mutations in the gene encoding progranulin. These dementias are rare and there is a lack of knowledge and experience to describe specific related sleep disorders. Since they all involve frontotemporal dementia, the sleep disturbances may be similar. Synucleinopathies The synucleinopathies include Parkinson’s disease with dementia (PDD), dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), multiple systems atrophy (MSA). This group of disorders is described pathologically by the abnormal accumulation of the protein alpha-synuclein. Parkinson’s disease with dementia is characterized by the onset of parkinsonism more than one year before the onset of dementia. Dementia with Lewy body patients will have the onset of dementia within the first year of the onset of parkinsonism. Multiple systems atrophy patients have severe autonomic dysfunction, parkinsonism, and dementia that develops within years of onset of symptoms. The synucleopathies have early brainstem dysfunction as well as cerebral dysfunction which may account for some of the sleep disturbances seen in this group. PDD and DLB patients have circadian-rhythm disorders. Experience has shown more commonly advanced phase disorders, but this has not been systematically studied. Hypersomnia may occur with more frequency in PDD than in PD without dementia. Compta et al. (2008) published a study looking at hypocretin levels in patients with PD without dementia and PDD and normal patients. There was no clinically significant difference in hypocretin levels, but Epworth Sleepiness Scales were significantly higher
290
Dementia
in PDD compared to PD without dementia. Boeve (2008) noted an anecdotal increased incidence of hypersomnia in patients with DLB. Insomnia may occur in patients with synucleopathies and is commonly related to medication effect. Medications may contribute to sleep-wake disturbances in PDD and DLB. Dopamine agonists used for treatment of parkinsonism in patients with PDD may cause daytime sleepiness, nighttime stimulation, and hallucinations. Anticholinergics may also have nighttime stimulatory effects and can increase confusion. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors used for dementia in PDD and DLB may cause insomnia. Antidepressants may contribute to, or exacerbate, PLMs and RBD. PLMs and RLS occur in this population with unknown frequency, but increased rates have been reported. It is treated with increased doses of dopamine agonist medications during symptomatic times of the day. RBD is a not uncommon feature of the synucleinopathies. It may precede the onset of PDD or DLB by several years. Treatment of RBD is necessary when it leads to self-injurious behavior or injury to the bed partner, or if it otherwise causes sleep fragmentation. Creating a safe sleep environment is paramount. Melatonin or clonazepam are used for pharmacologic treatment. Melatonin may be more desirable due to less effects on gait and cognition than clonazepam. Patients with parkinsonism as seen in PDD, DLB, and MSA also have sleep disturbances related to motor control. Bradykinesia/akinesia, rigidity, tremors, dystonia, and muscle stiffness all contribute to sleep onset and maintenance difficulty. On the other hand, some authors have recognized a sleep benefit on the motor symptoms of PD. Presumably, the gradual increase in dopaminergic stores through the course of the sleep period may contribute to some apparent benefit after a night of sleep. Patients with MSA typically have similar sleep disturbances to patients with PDD and DLB. In addition, however, sleep-disordered breathing may be seen more commonly in patients with MSA. Upper airway obstruction at the glottis level may lead to obstructive sleep apnea, and degeneration of the pontomedullary respiratory centers may lead to central sleep apnea. Vocal cord abductor paralysis may lead to clinical stridor and, more important, life-threatening breathing problems. Other Neurodegenerative Dementias Huntington’s disease (HD) is a hereditary neurodegenerative disorder. It is caused by an unstable expanded CAG triplet in the HD gene. Chorea is the most common movement disorder associated with HD.
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
291
Neuropsychiatric disorders such as irritability and depression are common, as well as sleep disturbances. Circadian-rhythm disorders in HD have not been systematically studied and the data is contradictory. It has been suggested that HD patients may have reduced expression of circadian-clock genes. Uncorroborated studies have reported sleep disturbances in the form of insomnia, increased sleep latency, reduced sleep efficiency, advanced sleep phase, PLMs, and RBD. Medication used for behavioral and neuropsychiatric control affect daytime sleepiness as in other neurodegenerative disorders. RLS and RBD have been reported to occur in HD but it has not been directly studied. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) has not been shown to correlate well with insomnia, in limited studies, which may suggest a cause other than poor nocturnal sleep leading to EDS. In a subjective study by Videnovic et al. (2009), looking at sleep disturbances in HD, there was a high clinical correlation between depression and disturbed sleep. There was no association between poor nocturnal sleep and medication use or irritability. Sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) has not been studied. In general, lower BMIs are seen in patients with HD. More studies are needed to further evaluate the incidence and prevalence. Prion Disorders Causing Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathy These include fatal familial insomnia, Creutzfeldt-Jacob disease, and Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker disease. These disorders are rare and information on sleep disturbances is sparse. Fatal familial insomnia (FFI) is a rare prion disease characterized by insomnia, dysautonomia, and motor dysfunction. It has been postulated to be caused by an autosomal dominant GAC to AAC mutation of the PRNP gene causing selective degeneration of the anterior and dorsomedial thalamic nuclei. A case report by Gistau et al. (2006) of a patient with fatal familial insomnia showed hormonal dysregulation of the circadian rhythm and PSG findings were consistent with a global abnormality of the sleep wake cycle. In a study by Krasnianski et al. (2008), PSG findings of patients with FFI showed a reduction in rapid eye movements, decreased sleep efficiency, decreased slow-wave sleep, PLMs, and central apnea. Clinically, these patients manifested with a wide range of psychiatric symptoms, vegetative symptoms, and autonomic dysfunction. Fatal familial insomnia may be confused with Creutzfeldt-Jacob disease (CJD) because of the rapid cognitive decline and psychiatric symptoms.
292
Dementia
However, FFI disease progression is usually more prolonged and signs typical of CJD generally occur late in the disease. CJD is a very rare transmissible prion disease that manifests as rapidly progressive dementia, myoclonus, other neurological dysfunction, and psychiatric symptoms. Sleep disturbances seem prominent in CJD studied cases, but studies are limited. A study by Wall et al. (2005) showed sleep disturbances that ranged from profound hypersomnia to insomnia. Insomnia was more common than hypersomnia. Sleep aids such as benzodiazepines and hypnotic medications were helpful in treating insomnia in these patients. Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker disease (GSS) is another very rare transmissible prion disease characterized by progressive ataxia and other neurological signs and symptoms, and dementia. There are no studies describing sleep abnormalities in these patients. A study performed by Guentchev et al. (1999) characterized the areas affected by neuronal loss, spongiform change, and astrogliosis—frontal cortex, temporal cortex, entorhinal cortex, pre- and parasubiculum, and all regions of the hippocampus. Based on this pattern of neuronal loss, which was the same pattern seen in CJD in Guentchev’s study, sleep abnormalities and psychiatric issues are likely common. Vascular Disorders Vascular dementia (VaD) is a controversial clinical and radiographic diagnosis. Currently, it is diagnosed when there is clinical dementia with MRI or CT brain showing significant subcortical and/or cortical ischemic events. Fuh, Wang, and Cummings (2005) performed a study looking at the neuropsychiatric profiles of patients diagnosed with AD and VaD. The most prominent differences seen were in the domain of sleep disturbances. In Fuh’s study, patients with cortical VaD had more severe sleep disturbances than those with AD. This was consistent with a study conducted by AharonPeretz et al. (1991), which showed that patients with VaD have a more sleepwake cycle disruption and decreased sleep quality than patients with AD. In summary, sleep disturbances are common in patients with dementia. It is imperative that clinicians are cognizant of the various manifestations of these disturbances in order to improve a patient’s quality of life and maximize daytime functioning. REFERENCES Aharon-Peretz. J., A. Masiah, T. Pillar, et al. 1991. Sleep-wake cycles in multi-infarct dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology 41: 1616–1619.
Sleep Disorders in Dementia
293
American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2005. The international classification of sleep disorders: Diagnostic and coding manual. 2nd ed. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Anderson, K. N., C. Hatfield, et al. 2009. Disrupted sleep and circadian patterns in frontotemporal dementia. European Journal of Neurology 16: 317–323. Arriagada, P. V., J. H. Growdon, E. T. Hedley-Whyte, and B. T. Hyman. 1992. Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel duration and severity of Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 42: 631. Aurora, R. N., R. S. Zak, R. K. Maganti, S. H. Auerbach, K. R. Casey, S. Chowdhuri, A. Karippot, K. Ramar, D. A. Kristo, and T. I. Morgenthaler. 2010. Best practice guide for the treatment of REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD). Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine 6 (1): 85–95. Boeve, B. F. 2008. Update on the diagnosis and management of sleep disturbances in dementia. Sleep Medicine Clinics 3: 347–360. Compta, Y., J. Santamaria, et al. 2008. Cerebrospinal hypocretin, daytime sleepiness and sleep architecture in Parkinson’s disease dementia. Brain 132: 3308–3317. Datta, S. 2007. Neurobiological mechanisms for the regulation of mammalian sleep-wake behavior: Reinterpretation of historical evidence and inclusion of contemporary cellular and molecular evidence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 3: 775–824. Dauvilliers, Y. 2007. Insomnia in patients with neurodegenerative conditions. Sleep Medicine 4 (Suppl. 4): S27–S34. Deguchi, K., K. Ikeda, R. Goto, M. Tsukaguchi, Y. Urai, K. Kurokohchi, T. Touge, N. Mori, and T. Masaki. 2010. The close relationship between life-threatening breathing disorders and urine storage dysfunction in multiple system atrophy. Journal of Neurology 257: 1287–1292. Deschenes, C., and S. M. McCurry. 2009. Current treatments for sleep disturbances in individuals with dementia. Current Psychiatry Reports 11 (1): 20–26. Feenstra, M. G., M. H. Botterblom, et al. 2000. Dopamine and noradrenaline efflux in the prefrontal cortex in the light and dark period: Effects of novelty and handling and comparison to the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience 100: 741–748. Ferri, C. P., M. Prince, C. Brayne, H. Brodaty, L. Fratiglioni, M. Ganguli, K. Hall, et al. 2005. Global prevalence of dementia: A Delphi consensus study. Lancet 366 (9503): 2112–2117. Fuh, J. L., S. J. Wang, and J. L. Cummings. 2005. Neuropsychiatric profiles in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 76: 1337–1341. Gallassi, R., A. Morreale, et al. 1992. Fatal familial insomnia: Neuropsychological study of a disease with thalamic degeneration. Cortex 28: 175–187. Gistau, V. S., L. Pintor, et al. 2006. Fatal familial insomnia. Psychosomatics 47: 6. Guentchev, M., J. Wanschitz, et al. 1999. Neuronal vulnerability in human prion diseases. American Journal of Pathology 155: 1453–1457.
294
Dementia
Ibfar, C., S. Ancoli-Israel, A. Chesson, and S. Quan. 2007. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Kadotani, H., T. Kadotani, T. Young, P. E. Peppard, L. Finn, I. M. Colrain, G. M. Murphy Jr., and E. Mignot. 2001. Association between apolipoprotein E 4 and sleep-disordered breathing in adults. JAMA 285: 2888–2890. Krasnianski, A., M. Bartl, et al. 2008. Fatal familial insomnia: Clinical features and early identification. Annals of Neurology 63: 658–661. Morton, A. J., N. I. Wood, et al. 2005. Disintegration of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian timing in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience 25: 157–163. Querfurth, H., and F. M. LaFerda. 2010. Alzheimer ’s disease. New England Journal of Medicine 362: 329–344. Suzuki, M., H. Saigusa, et al. 2010. Multiple system atrophy manifesting as complex sleep-disordered breathing. Auris, Nasus, Larynx 37: 110–113. Videnovic, A., S. Leurgans, et al. 2009. Daytime somnolence and nocturnal sleep disturbances in Huntington disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 15: 471–474. Wall, C. A., T. A. Rummins, et al. 2005. Psychiatric manifestations of CreutzfeldtJakob Disease: A 25-year analysis. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 17: 489–495. Wu, Y. H., D. Swaab, et al. 2007. Disturbance and strategies for reactivation of the circadian rhythm system in aging and Alzheimer ’s disease. Sleep Medicine 8: 623–636.
Chapter 12
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease Brooke K. Walter and James B. Leverenz
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative movement disorder that is classically characterized by the motor symptoms of bradykinesia, muscle rigidity, resting tremor, and, in later stages, postural instability. It affects between 4.1 and 4.6 million people worldwide and is poised to double in prevalence in the next 20 years (Dorsey et al. 2007). This anticipated expansion is due to greater worldwide life expectancy, increased survival of affected individuals, and increasing diagnosis of previously unrecognized cases. Untreated, most patients become severely disabled or die 10 to 14 years after disease onset (Poewe and Wenning 1996). While medical and surgical therapies have substantially improved motor disability, there is an increasing recognition that nonmotor symptoms contribute greatly to disability and quality of life (Hely et al. 2005). These nonmotor manifestations include sensory, psychiatric, autonomic, and cognitive disturbances. This chapter will focus on cognitive impairment (CI) in PD. Although James Parkinson originally reported that intellectual function is preserved in the “shaking palsy” (Parkinson 1817), CI is now recognized as a common and very problematic complication of PD (Emre et al. 2007). CI encompasses a spectrum of cognitive disturbance ranging from CI with no dementia (CIND) to frank PD-related dementia (PDD). CIND has been reported in 19% of newly diagnosed, untreated patients with PD, a twofold increase over similarly aged adults (Aarsland, Bronnick, et al. 2009). In a prospective study of newly diagnosed PD patients, 57% developed CIND by 3–5 years and a further 10% were diagnosed with dementia (Williams-Gray et al. 2007). PD patients with CIND have
296
Dementia
increased risk of developing dementia (Janvin et al. 2006), analogous to the increased risk of Alzheimer ’s dementia in non-PD patients with mild cognitive impairment (Petersen et al. 2001). Dementia is more common in patients with PD compared to age-matched controls, with studies reporting a five- to sixfold increased risk (Aarsland et al. 2001; Hobson and Meara 2004). The point prevalence of PDD among PD patients in community based studies has been estimated to be between 25% and 30% (Aarsland et al. 2003; Buter et al. 2008; Riedel et al. 2008). Prospective longitudinal studies of CI in PD have consistently demonstrated that PD patients are at high risk for dementia. Large, often community-based, studies in Norway, Germany, Australia, and the United Kingdom have reported high cumulative prevalence and incidence of PDD (see Figure 12.1 and Table 12.1). Thus, development of dementia is to be expected in the vast majority of patients with PD, even more frequent than some motor complications, such as dyskinesias (Muller et al. 2007). Of particular concern, the presence of dementia in PD is associated with nursing home placement (Aarsland et al. 2000), greater disability (Weintraub et al. 2004), greater caregiver distress (Aarsland et al. 1999), and mortality (Buter et al. 2008). Thus, it is important for the clinician to recognize CI as an important complication of PD, to treat it, and for the research community to assist in the development of better treatments. Figure 12.1
Cumulative Prevalence of Dementia in Parkinson’s Disease
per 1000 patient year
of 30 20-year survivors
a
b
2009
2008
2008
2008
2007
2004
2003
N
Patient sample
Community based 171 No dementia longitudinal cohort at enrollment Aarsland Norway Community based 224 Same cohort longitudinal cohort as Buter study Foltynie UK Community based 201 Newly cross-section diagnosed Williams- UK Community based 180 Gray longitudinal cohort Buter Norway Community based 233 longitudinal cohort Hely Australia Multicenter longitu- 136 dinal cohort Riedel Germany Nationwide cross873 section Aarsland Norway Community based 196 Newly diagcross-section nosed and drug naive
Location Study design
2001 Aarsland Norway
Year Author
Table 12.1 Epidemiological Studies in Parkinson’s Disease Dementia Dementia
All CI
n/a
n/a
20
12
5
n/a
19%
57%
75%
83%b 29%
60%
27%
10%
78%
8
26%
25%
4.2
82
30
95
18–44%
36%
Follow-up Point Cumm. Point Cumm. Annual Point Cumm. (years) prev. prev. prev. prev. incidencea prev. prev.
CIND
298
Dementia
This review will examine CI in PD and associated motor and nonmotor clinical characteristics. We will review diagnostic criteria, ancillary diagnostic evaluation, pathophysiology, and treatment. We conclude with a discussion of future directions in research and clinical practice. DEFINITIONS The DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for dementia require the presence of impairment in multiple cognitive domains and that these cognitive impairments are associated with significant impairment in social or occupational functioning (American Psychiatric Association 2000). These impairments must represent a decline from previous levels of performance or functioning. We will use these criteria for our discussion of dementia in PD. In addition, per current criteria (discussed further below [Emre et al. 2007]), PDD will refer specifically to patients who fulfilled criteria for motor PD one year prior to the development of dementia. In many PD patients with CI, the CI it is not sufficiently severe to compromise daily function. These patients will be classified as “cognitive impairment with no dementia” (CIND). CIND is also referred to in the PD literature as mild CI (MCI), but this term may cause confusion with the MCI observed in prodromal Alzheimer ’s disease patients and thus for the sake of this review we will use the term CIND. PD patients with normal cognition are referred to as PD, nondemented (PDND). CLINICAL CHARACTERISTICS Time Course PDD has an insidious onset and progression but the time to onset and rate of progression vary significantly between patients (Aarsland et al. 2004; Williams-Gray et al. 2007). CIND is present in some patients at time of PD diagnosis (Aarsland, Bronnick, et al. 2009). CIND may be a prodrome to frank dementia in some patients, although it is not currently clear whether all patients with CIND will progress to PDD (Janvin et al. 2006). In a longitudinal study, the annual decline of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was 2.3 points in PDD patients compared to less than 1 point in PDND patients and healthy control subjects (Aarsland et al. 2004). Cognitive Features PDD is associated with a variety of cognitive deficits including the domains of executive function, attention, visuospatial function, and memory.
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
299
Generally the profile for cognitive domain impairment is different in PDD than observed in other dementias such as AD, although these differences are less obvious in severe dementia due to the eventual impairments in almost all cognitive domains. In PDD and PD-CIND, executive dysfunction and attention are perhaps the most consistently reported deficits, while memory and language dysfunction are much less prominent in PDD than in AD (Watson and Leverenz 2010). For example, in a large study of 488 PDD patients and 488 AD patients, the neuropsychological profile accurately predicted 75% of the diagnoses. While both groups demonstrated memory impairment, the AD group performed significantly worse on memory measures. The tests with the greatest discrimination between the two groups were a measure of orientation (dependent on memory), in which the AD group performed most poorly, and a measure of attention, in which the PDD group showed the greatest deficits (Bronnick et al. 2007). Executive function, high-level cognitive skills involved in regulating behavior and monitoring other cognitive processes, is also reported to be frequently impaired cognitive domain among PD patients with CI (Caviness et al. 2007). Patients with PD also perform more poorly on visuospatial tasks. Memory impairment, although less severe than in AD, is a frequent feature of PDD. However, in PDD and CIND, memory recall is reportedly more impaired than memory recognition, unlike AD in which both are impaired (Emre et al. 2007; Watson and Leverenz 2010). There is some heterogeneity of the neuropsychological profile in PDD and subtypes of dysexecutive, amnestic, and mixed CI have been proposed (Lewis et al. 2005). Subtypes have also been reported in PD-CIND including amnestic, single-domain nonamnestic, and mild multidomain impairment (Janvin et al. 2006). The pathophysiologic significance of this variability in neuropsychological profile is not clear, but certainly raises the possibility of differing underlying pathologies (Leverenz et al. 2009). Behavioral Features PDD is associated with several behavioral changes including depression, apathy, hallucinations, and delusions. These neurobehavioral symptoms predominate in later disease, are difficult to treat, and are associated with reduced quality of life (Aarsland et al. 1999, 2007; Hely et al. 2005; Schrag, Jahanshahi, and Quinn 2000). Often overlooked in PD, depressive symptoms can be confused with motor findings (psychomotor retardation mistaken for bradykinesia and masked facies) and somatic complaints (fatigue and sleep disturbance are common to both disorders). In addition, demented patients may have increased difficulty articulating their
300
Dementia
mood-related symptoms. Depressed PD patients demonstrate diminished global cognitive performance and specifically have shown impairment in the cognitive domains of naming and verbal memory (Fernandez et al. 2009). Executive functions have also been shown to be impaired in depressed nondemented PD patients (Santangelo et al. 2009). It is unclear whether the primary cause of poor performance was depression, intrinsic CIND, or a third factor (such as common neuropathology) causing both CI and depression. It is unknown whether these mood changes are the result of coping with a chronic, debilitating illness or whether they may be inherent changes due to underlying PD neuropathology. PDD and PDCIND patients should be screened for depression and, when appropriate, treated both pharmacologically and behaviorally. Apathy is also very common in PD and, while frequently associated with depression, it can occur independently (Oguru et al. 2009). In a fouryear population based, longitudinal study, greater than 60% of PD patients reported apathy (Pedersen et al. 2009), as measured by the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Cummings et al. 1994). Dementia and depression were both found to be risk factors for apathy (Pedersen et al. 2009). Apathy has also been linked to frontal and visuo-constructional deficits (Santangelo et al. 2009), suggesting a cognitive underpinning of this symptom. It is clinically important to differentiate apathy from depression as the pharmacologic and behavioral management can differ. For example, antidepressants have been shown to improve depression in PD, while acetylcholinesterase inhibitors may improve apathy (Devos et al. 2008; Figiel and Sadowsky 2008). Psychotic symptoms are common in PD; in particular, visual hallucinations are reported in up to 74% of 20-year PD survivors (Hely et al. 2008). In contrast, less than 10% of autopsy-confirmed AD patients, without coexistent Lewy bodies, suffer from hallucinations, indicating an association between hallucinations and Lewy body pathology (Tsuang et al. 2009). Visual hallucinations are much more common than auditory, olfactory, or tactile hallucinations, although all forms may occur (Diederich et al. 2009). Visual hallucinations in PD patients are often well formed, taking the shape of people, animals, or objects. They can be adverse effects of dopaminergic treatment, particularly dopamine agonists (Goetz et al. 2001), but also result from the underlying disease (Fenelon, Goetz, and Karenberg 2006). Delusions appear to be less common than hallucinations in PD and, when present, often co-occur with hallucinations and CI (Marsh et al. 2004; Kulisevsky et al. 2008). Delusions are often of a paranoid or jealous nature and are associated with increased caregiver burden (Marsh et al. 2004).
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
301
PD patients can suffer from a wide variety of sleep disturbances; among these, rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder (RBD) may be particularly associated PD and other disorders associated with Lewy body pathology (Boeve et al. 2003). RBD is characterized by loss of normal muscle atonia during REM sleep resulting in complex, sometimes violent, movements. Several studies have suggested an association between RBD and PD-CIND with the majority of PD-CIND patients also suffering from RBD (Gagnon et al. 2009; Vendette et al. 2007); notably, another group has not confirmed this association (Yoritaka et al. 2009). Risk Factors In addition to CIND and RBD (discussed above), several other risk factors have been identified for development of PDD. Older age has been consistently associated with increased risk for dementia in PD (Aarsland et al. 2001; Riedel et al. 2008; Hely et al. 2008; Williams-Gray et al. 2007). This association with age may result from an increased susceptibility to dementia, an increased risk of co-morbid dementia from other causes such as AD or cerebrovascular disease, or likely both. Greater disease severity and longer duration of disease, factors correlated with each other and with age, are also linked to development of dementia (Aarsland et al. 2001; Riedel et al. 2008). Lower education attainment has been reported among PDD patients (Riedel et al. 2008). Other clinical features associated with the development of PDD are a nontremor-predominant motor phenotype, poor pentagon copying, and impaired semantic fluency (Williams-Gray et al. 2007). DIAGNOSIS Diagnostic Criteria In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), PDD is categorized as dementia due to a general medical condition and defines dementia in terms of functional impairment due to CI (American Psychiatric Association 2000). More specific criteria were proposed by the Movement Disorders Society (MDS) Task Force in 2007 (Emre et al. 2007) (see Table 12.2). The core features include the development of dementia in the context of already established motor PD, as determined by the UK Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank criteria (Gibb and Lees 1988). The CI must be a decline from premorbid performance and affect two or more of the following four cognitive domains: attention, executive function, visuo-constructive ability, and memory. CI must be sufficiently severe
302
Dementia
Table 12.2 Features of Parkinson’s Disease Dementia I.
Core diagnostic features A. Diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease B. Dementia of slow onset and progression developing greater than one year after onset of Parkinson’s disease and characterized by 1. Impairment in at least one cognitive domain 2. A decline from pre-morbid level of function 3. Cognitive deficits sufficiently severe to impair daily functioning II. Associated clinical features A. Cognitive Features 1. Attention impaired 2. Executive functions impaired 3. Visuo-spatial processing impaired 4. Memory impaired 5. Language function largely intact B. Behavioral features 1. Apathy 2. Personality change 3. Mood change 4. Hallucinations 5. Delusions 6. Somnolence III. Features that make PDD diagnosis uncertain A. Co-morbid abnormalities that can contribute to cognitive dysfunction but are not sufficiently severe to cause dementia B. Uncertain time-course between onset of motor symptoms and dementia IV. Features suggesting alternative cause of cognitive impairment A. Cognitive symptoms arising in context of systemic disease, intoxication, or mood disorder. B. Clinical and imaging features suggestive of vascular dementia
Source: Table adapted from Emre et al. 2007
to impair daily function (in social, occupational, or self-care domains) independent of functional impairments from motor or autonomic symptoms. Supportive criteria include the presence of typical behavioral features (depression, apathy, anxiety, delusions, hallucinations, or excessive daytime somnolence) and absence of features suggesting alternative diagnoses. Criteria for probable and possible PDD were delineated. This assessment is based on history and clinical and cognitive examinations. The MDS criteria specify that dementia must arise in the context of an established PD diagnosis (Hughes et al. 1993). This condition serves
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
303
to distinguish PDD from dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and other dementias (including Alzheimer ’s dementia, AD). DLB is diagnosed when dementia either precedes or occurs within one year of onset of PD motor symptoms and when other core symptoms such as fluctuations and visual hallucinations are present (Emre et al. 2007; McKeith et al. 2005). The MDS has subsequently proposed a two-tiered system for PDD diagnosis (Dubois et al. 2007). Level 1 is designed for clinicians without expertise in neuropsychological assessment who require simple, practical diagnostic criteria and brief neuropsychological measures. A proposed algorithm for this level includes (1) diagnosis of PD by Queen’s Square Brain Bank criteria (Hughes et al. 1993); (2) PD onset at least one year prior to onset of dementia; (3) decline of global cognitive efficiency as evidenced by MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975) score < 26; (4) CI severe enough to impair daily life (e.g., based on caregiver questionnaire or questioning patient about medication regimen); (5) impairment in two of the four cognitive domains most impacted in PDD: attention as assessed by months reversed (Shum et al. 1990) or serial sevens (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975), executive function as measured by phonological verbal fluency (Benton and Hamsher 1989), visuo-constructive function such as clock drawing (Sunderland et al. 1989) or MMSE pentagons (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975), or memory as demonstrated in three-word recall (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975) (see Table 12.2). There should be an absence of major depression, delirium, or other confounding cause of dementia. Level 2 involves extensive neuropsychological and behavioral testing and is appropriate when more detailed neuropsychological information is required because of diagnostic uncertainty in Level 1 or for research purposes. Ancillary Diagnostic Evaluations PDD must be distinguished from other forms of dementia because treatment and prognosis differ according to etiology. The American Academy of Neurology (AAN) recommends structural brain imaging, with either noncontrast computerized tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), to exclude structural lesions in demented patients (Knopman et al. 2001). Neuroimaging studies have found structural lesions in 5% of demented patients without clinical history or signs suggesting those lesions (Chui and Zhang 1997). In particular, subdural hematoma, normal pressure hydrocephalus, and brain tumor should be detected and treated appropriately. Other findings, such as vascular lesions or lesions suggestive of a Parkinson-plus disorder, may alter the diagnosis and course of
304
Dementia
medical treatment. MRI studies in PDD have shown whole brain and regional cortical atrophy, but these changes overlap with those seen in other dementias and, thus, offer only limited contribution in PDD diagnosis (see Emre et al. 2007 for review) The AAN also recommends screening demented patients for depression, vitamin B12 deficiency, hypothyroidism, and, if clinically suspected, syphilis. While these disorders can cause or contribute to a dementia-like syndrome (Knopman et al. 2001), more often they are co-morbidities of an underlying dementing disease such as AD and PD. Appropriate treatment of these can improve cognitive function even in the context of another primary etiology of dementia such as PD. PATHOPHYSIOLOGY The pathological processes that underlie CI in PD are likely primarily related to the pathology linked to PD: Lewy bodies and Lewy-related neurites (Lewy Related Pathology, LRP). LRP is best detected utilizing immunohistochemistry for alpha-synuclein, the predominant protein component of this pathologic change of PD (Spillantini et al. 1997; Schneider et al. 2002). Pathologic studies using this technique to detect LRP in well-characterized PD patients who fulfilled criteria for PDD, that is, motor parkinsonism preceding dementia by one year or more, have found a high frequency of LRP in brainstem, limbic, and neocortical regions of the brain (Aarsland et al. 2005; Apaydin et al. 2002; Braak et al. 2005; Galvin, Pollack, and Morris 2006; Hurtig et al. 2000). All of these studies have found that the presence or severity of LRP in limbic and neocortical regions is associated with a clinical history of dementia in PD. The contribution of other pathological processes such as Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular disease to dementia in PD are unclear. Some studies have suggested that co-existent Alzheimer ’s disease occurs in less than 10% of PDD patients at autopsy (Aarsland et al. 2005; Apaydin et al. 2002; Braak et al. 2005), while others have observed co-existent pathologic AD in 30 to 40% of PDD patients (Galvin, Pollack, and Morris 2006; Hurtig et al. 2000). Given the limited number of autopsy cases in each of these studies, fewer than 200 in total, it is not surprising that there is disagreement between these studies. One alternate method to examine this issue is to evaluate biomarkers linked to Alzheimer ’s disease pathology, specifically cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) levels of Aβ (senile plaques) and tau (neurofibrillary tangles). In AD multiple studies have consistently found that CSF levels of Aβ are lower than normal and tau levels are elevated (Blennow et al. 2010). At this point, it appears that CI in PD is associated with
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
305
lowered CSF Aβ levels, similar to AD, but not elevated tau (Montine et al. 2010). These findings would appear to be most consistent with the neuropathological studies that have observed elevated Aβ deposition in the brain of PDD patients, without significant coexistent tau deposition (i.e., neurofibrillary tangle) (Apaydin et al. 2002). Thus, the preponderance of clinical biomarker and neuropathologic data suggest that the majority of PD cases with CI do not have all the pathologic features of AD, but some AD-associated pathologic changes may play a role in the pathophysiology of CI in PD. Clinically, one can hypothesize that the subset of PD with predominant declarative short-term memory impairments might also have coexistent LRP and Alzheimer ’s disease. The contribution of vascular disease to CI in PD is even less well studied. To the best of our knowledge there have been no systematic autopsy studies examining this issue. It is clear from autopsy studies in communitybased autopsy samples that vascular disease is an important contributor to dementia in the general population (Sonnen et al. 2007; White et al. 2002). Given these findings it is likely that there are a subset of PD patients with CI who have both LRP and vascular pathology contributing to their deficits. While the consensus appears to be that LRP drives the CI observed in PD, further research is necessary to further elucidate other potential contributors to CI in PD. This becomes particularly relevant as diseasespecific treatments for Alzheimer ’s disease and other neurodegenerative disorders become available. TREATMENT While PDD and AD appear to have distinct neuropathology, both share a common cholinergic deficit. In AD, the deficit presumably arises from neurofibrillary tangle pathologic change in the cholinergic basal forebrain, while in PDD the deficit is likely due to LRP in those same neural structures (Tiraboschi et al. 2000). Interestingly, Frederick Lewy’s original description of Lewy bodies was in the same cholinergic basal forebrain neurons, not in the dopaminergic substantia nigra. Given this, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AChEi), originally designed to address the cholinergic deficiency in AD, can be rationally used in PDD. To date only one large-scale placebo-controlled study has examined the use of AChEi in PDD (see Table 12.3). A 24-week study randomly assigned 541 patients with mild or moderate PDD to placebo or 3mg to 12mg daily oral doses of Rivastigmine (Emre et al. 2004). The primary outcome measures were the cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment
2002 2004
2005 2009
2009 2009 2004
Donepezil Donepezil
Donepezil Galantamine
Memantine Memantine Rivastigmine
Aarsland Leroi Emre
Ravina Grace
Aarsland Leroi
Author
+ CGIC n/a + CGIC − CIBIC + CIBIC
72a 25 541
+ CIBIC n/a
Global
22 69
14 16
N + MMSE − MMSE, DRS attention, memory, or executive measures − ADAScog +MMSE − Multiple attention, memory, visuospatial measures − MMSE, +AQT +DRS, −MMSE +ADAScog
Cognitive
Included 32 dementia with Lewy body patients
− BPRS − NPI FSBS PDQ39 − NPI, − NPI +NPI
− NPI n/a
Behavioral
−DAD, n/a +ADSCADL
n/a n/a
n/a n/a
ADL
Abbreviations: ADAScog (Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment Scale Cognitive Subscale), ADL (activities of daily living), ADSCADL (Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study Activities of Daily Living), AQT(a quick test of cognition speed), BPRS (Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale), CIBIC (clinician’s interview based impression of change), CGIC (Clinical Global Impression of Change), DAD (Disability Assessment for Dementia), DRS (Dementia Rating Scale), FSBS (Frontal Systems Behavioral Scale), MMSE (Mini-Mental Status Examination), NPI (Neuropsychiatric Inventory), PDQ39 (Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire -39),
a
Note: + indicates improved score, - indicates no change or worsened score
Year
Drug
Table 12.3 Placebo-Controlled Treatment Trials in Parkinson’s Disease Dementia
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
307
Scale (ADAS-cog) (Rosen, Mohs, and Davis 1984) and Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study–Clinician’s Global Impression of Change (ADCSCGIC) (Schneider et al. 1997). Secondary outcomes included other cognitive measures (MMSE, clock drawing, verbal fluency test, and a measure of attention), a behavior measure (neuropsychiatric inventory, NPI [Cummings et al. 1994]), and a metric for activities of daily living (ADL). In the 410 patients who completed the study, there was statistically significant improvement in all of the primary and secondary measures in patients treated with rivastigmine. There was clinically meaningful ADCS-CGIC improvement in 5% of patients on rivastigmine and worsening in 10% of patients on placebo, both favoring use of rivastigmine in PDD and similar to the benefit of this class of medications in AD. In an extension study, 273 patients (from both placebo and active drug groups) completed an additional 24 weeks of open-label rivastigmine (Poewe et al. 2006). The group as a whole showed improvement in the ADAS-cog and subjects initially treated with placebo showed gains similar to the drug treatment group in the original study. Rivastigmine has also been shown to improve cognitive and behavior symptoms in DLB (McKeith et al. 2000). Smaller studies using the other available AChEi medications donepezil and galantamine have not consistently found positive cognitive or behavioral effects (Aarsland et al. 2002; Leroi et al. 2004; Ravina et al. 2005; Grace, Amick, and Friedman 2009). It is not clear if this indicates an actual difference in effectiveness between the different AChEi agents, or whether these studies were just too small to adequately detect the modest effects observed in the larger rivastigmine trial. AChEi are associated with a number of adverse effects. In the study by Emre and colleagues (2004), patients treated with rivastigmine suffered nausea, vomiting, and tremor with greater frequency than those treated with placebo. Not surprisingly, there was also greater attrition in the rivastigmine treated group. Another study using the same database reported that tremor exacerbation occurred only transiently during dose titration (Oertel et al. 2008). Transdermal rivastigmine has fewer gastrointestinal side effects (Winblad et al. 2007) and would appear to be a reasonable alternative to oral treatment of PDD, but the clinical effectiveness and side effect profile in PDD has not yet been specifically studied. Memantine is an N-methyl D-aspartate receptor antagonist that is used to limit the potential negative effects of glutamate overactivity in neurodegenerative disease. It has shown symptomatic benefit as mono-therapy in moderate to severe AD (McShane, Areosa Sastre, and Minakaran 2006) and also can be combined with an AChEi for a positive clinical effect in AD (Tariot et al. 2004). In a small (40 PDD cases) placebo-controlled
308
Dementia
study combining PDD and DLB patients, memantine was associated with improved global clinical ratings, but other measures of cognition, behavior, and activities of daily living failed to demonstrate a benefit of treatment (Aarsland, Ballard et al. 2009). In another small study (N=25), memantine showed no benefit compared to placebo except that the memantine-treated group had greater deterioration after withdrawal of the drug (Leroi et al. 2009). In these studies, memantine was generally well tolerated with mild adverse events that were similar between treatment and placebo groups (Aarsland, Ballard et al. 2009; Leroi et al. 2009). These small studies are encouraging, but larger-scale studies are needed to better determine efficacy and tolerability. CONCLUSIONS It appears that, with sufficient survival time, virtually all PD patients will develop CI and that this nonmotor complication is associated with substantial morbidity and mortality. Unfortunately, treatments for CI in PD only have modest effects on CI and no treatment is available for the underlying pathological processes. Important future directions for research include the need to develop a better understanding of how the primary pathology, LRP, and processes such as Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular disease lead to cognitive dysfunction in PD. Biomarkers from neuroimaging, genetics, and body fluids (e.g., CSF) will likely play a very important role in helping us understand the underlying processes leading to CI in a particular PD patient. As better disease-specific treatments are developed, it will be very important to identify the processes underlying the dementing syndrome PD so that the clinician can tailor the interventions to each patient. REFERENCES Aarsland, D., K. Andersen, J. P. Larsen, A. Lolk, and P. Kragh-Sorensen. 2003. Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease: An 8-year prospective study. Arch Neurol 60 (3): 387–392. Aarsland, D., K. Andersen, J. P. Larsen, A. Lolk, H. Nielsen, and P. Kragh-Sorensen. 2001. Risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease: A community-based, prospective study. Neurology 56 (6): 730–736. Aarsland, D., K. Andersen, J. P. Larsen, R. Perry, T. Wentzel-Larsen, A. Lolk, and P. Kragh-Sorensen. 2004. The rate of cognitive decline in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 61 (12): 1906–1911. Aarsland, D., C. Ballard, Z. Walker, F. Bostrom, G. Alves, K. Kossakowski, I. Leroi, F. Pozo-Rodriguez, L. Minthon, and E. Londos. 2009. Memantine in patients
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
309
with Parkinson’s disease dementia or dementia with Lewy bodies: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, multicentre trial. Lancet Neurol 8 (7): 613–618. Aarsland, D., K. Bronnick, U. Ehrt, P. P. De Deyn, S. Tekin, M. Emre, and J. L. Cummings. 2007. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with Parkinson’s disease and dementia: Frequency, profile and associated care giver stress. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 78 (1): 36–42. Aarsland, D., K. Bronnick, J. P. Larsen, O. B. Tysnes, and G. Alves. 2009. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: The Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology 72 (13): 1121–1126. Aarsland, D., K. Laake, J. P. Larsen, and C. Janvin. 2002. Donepezil for cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: A randomised controlled study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72 (6): 708–712. Aarsland, D., J. P. Larsen, K. Karlsen, N. G. Lim, and E. Tandberg. 1999. Mental symptoms in Parkinson’s disease are important contributors to caregiver distress. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14 (10): 866–874. Aarsland, D., J. P. Larsen, E. Tandberg, and K. Laake. 2000. Predictors of nursing home placement in Parkinson’s disease: A population-based, prospective study. J Am Geriatr Soc 48 (8): 938–942. Aarsland, D., R. Perry, A. Brown, J. P. Larsen, and C. Ballard. 2005. Neuropathology of dementia in Parkinson’s disease: A prospective, community-based study. Ann Neurol 58 (5): 773–776. American Psychiatric Association. 2000. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Apaydin, H., J. E. Ahlskog, J. E. Parisi, B. F. Boeve, and D. W. Dickson. 2002. Parkinson disease neuropathology: Later-developing dementia and loss of the levodopa response. Arch Neurol 59 (1): 102–112. Benton, A. L., and K. D. Hamsher. 1989. Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates. Blennow, K., H. Hampel, M. Weiner, and H. Zetterberg. 2010. Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma biomarkers in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol 6 (3): 131–144. Boeve, B. F., M. H. Silber, J. E. Parisi, D. W. Dickson, T. J. Ferman, E. E. Benarroch, A. M. Schmeichel, et al. 2003. Synucleinopathy pathology and REM sleep behavior disorder plus dementia or parkinsonism. Neurology 61 (1): 40–45. Braak, H., U. Rub, E. N. Jansen Steur, K. Del Tredici, and R. A. de Vos. 2005. Cognitive status correlates with neuropathologic stage in Parkinson disease. Neurology 64 (8): 1404–1410. Bronnick, K., M. Emre, R. Lane, S. Tekin, and D. Aarsland. 2007. Profile of cognitive impairment in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease compared with Alzheimer ’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 78 (10): 1064–1068. Buter, T. C., A. van den Hout, F. E. Matthews, J. P. Larsen, C. Brayne, and D. Aarsland. 2008. Dementia and survival in Parkinson disease: A 12-year population study. Neurology 70 (13): 1017–1022.
310
Dementia
Caviness, J. N., E. Driver-Dunckley, D. J. Connor, M. N. Sabbagh, J. G. Hentz, B. Noble, V. G. Evidente, H. A. Shill, and C. H. Adler. 2007. Defining mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 22 (9): 1272–1277. Chui, H., and Q. Zhang. 1997. Evaluation of dementia: A systematic study of the usefulness of the American Academy of Neurology’s practice parameters. Neurology 49 (4): 925–935. Cummings, J. L., M. Mega, K. Gray, S. Rosenberg-Thompson, D. A. Carusi, and J. Gornbein. 1994. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44 (12): 2308–2314. Devos, D., K. Dujardin, I. Poirot, C. Moreau, O. Cottencin, P. Thomas, A. Destee, R. Bordet, and L. Defebvre. 2008. Comparison of desipramine and citalopram treatments for depression in Parkinson’s disease: A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. Mov Disord 23 (6): 850–857. Diederich, N. J., G. Fenelon, G. Stebbins, and C. G. Goetz. 2009. Hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol 5 (6): 331–342. Dorsey, E. R., R. Constantinescu, J. P. Thompson, K. M. Biglan, R. G. Holloway, K. Kieburtz, F. J. Marshall, et al. 2007. Projected number of people with Parkinson disease in the most populous nations, 2005 through 2030. Neurology 68 (5): 384–386. Dubois, B., D. Burn, C. Goetz, D. Aarsland, R. G. Brown, G. A. Broe, D. Dickson, et al. 2007. Diagnostic procedures for Parkinson’s disease dementia: Recommendations from the movement disorder society task force. Mov Disord 22 (16): 2314–2324. Emre, M., D. Aarsland, A. Albanese, E. J. Byrne, G. Deuschl, P. P. De Deyn, F. Durif, et al. 2004. Rivastigmine for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med 351 (24): 2509–2518. Emre, M., D. Aarsland, R. Brown, D. J. Burn, C. Duyckaerts, Y. Mizuno, G. A. Broe, et al. 2007. Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 22 (12): 1689–1707. Fenelon, G., C. G. Goetz, and A. Karenberg. 2006. Hallucinations in Parkinson disease in the prelevodopa era. Neurology 66 (1): 93–98. Fernandez, H. H., R. H. See, M. F. Gary, D. Bowers, R. L. Rodriguez, C. Jacobson, and M. S. Okun. 2009. Depressive symptoms in Parkinson disease correlate with impaired global and specific cognitive performance. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 22 (4): 223–227. Figiel, G., and C. Sadowsky. 2008. A systematic review of the effectiveness of rivastigmine for the treatment of behavioral disturbances in dementia and other neurological disorders. Curr Med Res Opin 24 (1): 157–166. Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh. 1975. “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12 (3): 189–198. Gagnon, J. F., M. Vendette, R. B. Postuma, C. Desjardins, J. Massicotte-Marquez, M. Panisset, and J. Montplaisir. 2009. Mild cognitive impairment in rapid
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
311
eye movement sleep behavior disorder and Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 66 (1): 39–47. Galvin, J. E., J. Pollack, and J. C. Morris. 2006. Clinical phenotype of Parkinson disease dementia. Neurology 67 (9): 1605–1611. Gibb, W. R., and A. J. Lees. 1988. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51 (6): 745–752. Goetz, C. G., S. Leurgans, E. J. Pappert, R. Raman, and A. B. Stemer. 2001. Prospective longitudinal assessment of hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 57 (11): 2078–2082. Grace, J., M. M. Amick, and J. H. Friedman. 2009. A double-blind comparison of galantamine hydrobromide ER and placebo in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 80 (1): 18–23. Hely, M. A., J. G. Morris, W. G. Reid, and R. Trafficante. 2005. Sydney Multicenter Study of Parkinson’s disease: Non-L-dopa-responsive problems dominate at 15 years. Mov Disord 20 (2): 190–199. Hely, M. A., W. G. Reid, M. A. Adena, G. M. Halliday, and J. G. Morris. 2008. The Sydney multicenter study of Parkinson’s disease: The inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Mov Disord 23 (6): 837–844. Hobson, P., and J. Meara. 2004. Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of older subjects with Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. Mov Disord 19 (9): 1043–1049. Hughes, A. J., S. E. Daniel, S. Blankson, and A. J. Lees. 1993. A clinicopathologic study of 100 cases of Parkinson’s disease. Arch Neurol 50 (2): 140–148. Hurtig, H. I., J. Q. Trojanowski, J. Galvin, D. Ewbank, M. L. Schmidt, V. M. Lee, C. M. Clark, et al. 2000. Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate with dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 54 (10): 1916–1921. Janvin, C. C., J. P. Larsen, D. Aarsland, and K. Hugdahl. 2006. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: progression to dementia. Mov Disord 21 (9): 1343–1349. Knopman, D. S., S. T. DeKosky, J. L. Cummings, H. Chui, J. Corey-Bloom, N. Relkin, G. W. Small, B. Miller, and J. C. Stevens. 2001. Practice parameter: Diagnosis of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56 (9): 1143–1153. Kulisevsky, J., J. Pagonabarraga, B. Pascual-Sedano, C. Garcia-Sanchez, and A. Gironell. 2008. Prevalence and correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Parkinson’s disease without dementia. Mov Disord 23 (13): 1889–1896. Leroi, I., J. Brandt, S. G. Reich, C. G. Lyketsos, S. Grill, R. Thompson, and L. Marsh. 2004. Randomized placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 19 (1): 1–8. Leroi, I., R. Overshott, E. J. Byrne, E. Daniel, and A. Burns. 2009. Randomized controlled trial of memantine in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 24 (8): 1217–1221.
312
Dementia
Leverenz, J. B., J. F. Quinn, C. Zabetian, J. Zhang, K. S. Montine, and T. J. Montine. 2009. Cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson disease. Curr Top Med Chem 9 (10): 903–912. Lewis, S. J., T. Foltynie, A. D. Blackwell, T. W. Robbins, A. M. Owen, and R. A. Barker. 2005. Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease in the early clinical stages using a data driven approach. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 76 (3): 343–348. Marsh, L., J. R. Williams, M. Rocco, S. Grill, C. Munro, and T. M. Dawson. 2004. Psychiatric comorbidities in patients with Parkinson disease and psychosis. Neurology 63 (2): 293–300. McKeith, I., T. Del Ser, P. Spano, M. Emre, K. Wesnes, R. Anand, A. Cicin-Sain, R. Ferrara, and R. Spiegel. 2000. Efficacy of rivastigmine in dementia with Lewy bodies: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled international study. Lancet 356 (9247): 2031–2036. McKeith, I. G., D. W. Dickson, J. Lowe, M. Emre, J. T. O’Brien, H. Feldman, J. Cummings, et al. 2005. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: Third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 65 (12): 1863–1872. McShane, R., A. Areosa Sastre, and N. Minakaran. 2006. Memantine for dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2): CD003154. Montine, T. J., M. Shi, J. F. Quinn, E. R. Peskind, S. Craft, K. A. Chung, J. Jankovic, et al. 2010. CSF Abeta(42) and tau in Parkinson’s disease with cognitive impairment. Mov Disord 25 (15): 2682–2685. Muller, T., D. Woitalla, H. Russ, K. Hock, and D. A. Haeger. 2007. Prevalence and treatment strategies of dyskinesia in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm 114 (8): 1023–1026. Oertel, W., W. Poewe, E. Wolters, P. P. De Deyn, M. Emre, C. Kirsch, C. Hsu, S. Tekin, and R. Lane. 2008. Effects of rivastigmine on tremor and other motor symptoms in patients with Parkinson’s disease dementia: A retrospective analysis of a double-blind trial and an open-label extension. Drug Saf 31 (1): 79–94. Oguru, M., H. Tachibana, K. Toda, B. Okuda, and N. Oka. 2009. Apathy and depression in Parkinson disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 23 (1): 35–41. Parkinson, J. 1817. An essay on the shaking palsy. Reproduced in James Parkinson (Ed. M. Critchley, 1955). London: Macmillan. Pedersen, K. F., G. Alves, D. Aarsland, and J. P. Larsen. 2009. Occurrence and risk factors for apathy in Parkinson disease: A 4-year prospective longitudinal study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 80 (11): 1279–1282. Petersen, R. C., J. C. Stevens, M. Ganguli, E. G. Tangalos, J. L. Cummings, and S. T. DeKosky. 2001. Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56 (9): 1133–1142. Poewe, W. H., and G. K. Wenning. 1996. The natural history of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 47 (6 Suppl. 3): S146–152.
Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease
313
Poewe, W., E. Wolters, M. Emre, M. Onofrj, C. Hsu, S. Tekin, and R. Lane. 2006. Long-term benefits of rivastigmine in dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease: An active treatment extension study. Mov Disord 21 (4): 456–461. Ravina, B., M. Putt, A. Siderowf, J. T. Farrar, M. Gillespie, A. Crawley, H. H. Fernandez, M. M. Trieschmann, S. Reichwein, and T. Simuni. 2005. Donepezil for dementia in Parkinson’s disease: A randomised, double blind, placebo controlled, crossover study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 76 (7): 934–939. Riedel, O., J. Klotsche, A. Spottke, G. Deuschl, H. Forstl, F. Henn, I. Heuser, et al. 2008. Cognitive impairment in 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Results from the German Study on Epidemiology of Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (GEPAD). J Neurol 255 (2): 255–264. Rosen, W. G., R. C. Mohs, and K. L. Davis. 1984. A new rating scale for Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 141 (11): 1356–1364. Santangelo, G., C. Vitale, L. Trojano, K. Longo, A. Cozzolino, D. Grossi, and P. Barone. 2009. Relationship between depression and cognitive dysfunctions in Parkinson’s disease without dementia. J Neurol 256 (4): 632–638. Schneider, J. A., J. L. Bienias, D. W. Gilley, D. E. Kvarnberg, E. J. Mufson, and D. A. Bennett. 2002. Improved detection of substantia nigra pathology in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Histochem Cytochem 50 (1): 99–106. Schneider, L. S., J. T. Olin, R. S. Doody, C. M. Clark, J. C. Morris, B. Reisberg, F. A. Schmitt, M. Grundman, R. G. Thomas, and S. H. Ferris. 1997. Validity and reliability of the Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study-Clinical Global Impression of Change. The Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 11 (Suppl. 2): S22–32. Schrag, A., M. Jahanshahi, and N. Quinn. 2000. What contributes to quality of life in patients with Parkinson’s disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 69 (3): 308–312. Shum, D. H., K. McFarland, J. D. Bain, and M. S. Humphreys. 1990. Effects of closed-head injury on attentional processes: an information-processing stage analysis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 12 (2): 247–264. Sonnen, J. A., E. B. Larson, P. K. Crane, S. Haneuse, G. Li, G. D. Schellenberg, S. Craft, J. B. Leverenz, and T. J. Montine. 2007. Pathological correlates of dementia in a longitudinal, population-based sample of aging. Ann Neurol 62 (4): 406–413. Spillantini, M. G., M. L. Schmidt, V. M. Lee, J. Q. Trojanowski, R. Jakes, and M. Goedert. 1997. Alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies. Nature 388 (6645): 839–840. Sunderland, T., J. L. Hill, A. M. Mellow, B. A. Lawlor, J. Gundersheimer, P. A. Newhouse, and J. H. Grafman. 1989. Clock drawing in Alzheimer ’s disease: A novel measure of dementia severity. J Am Geriatr Soc 37 (8): 725–729. Tariot, P. N., M. R. Farlow, G. T. Grossberg, S. M. Graham, S. McDonald, and I. Gergel. 2004. Memantine treatment in patients with moderate to severe Alzheimer disease already receiving donepezil: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 291 (3): 317–324.
314
Dementia
Tiraboschi, P., L. A. Hansen, M. Alford, M. N. Sabbagh, B. Schoos, E. Masliah, L. J. Thal, and J. Corey-Bloom. 2000. Cholinergic dysfunction in diseases with Lewy bodies. Neurology 54 (2): 407–411. Tsuang, D., E. B. Larson, E. Bolen, M. L. Thompson, E. Peskind, J. Bowen, W. McCormick, et al. 2009. Visual hallucinations in dementia: A prospective community-based study with autopsy. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17 (4): 317–323. Vendette, M., J. F. Gagnon, A. Decary, J. Massicotte-Marquez, R. B. Postuma, J. Doyon, M. Panisset, and J. Montplaisir. 2007. REM sleep behavior disorder predicts cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease without dementia. Neurology 69 (19): 1843–1849. Watson, G. S., and J. B. Leverenz. 2010. Profile of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Pathology 20 (3): 640–645. Weintraub, D., P. J. Moberg, J. E. Duda, I. R. Katz, and M. B. Stern. 2004. Effect of psychiatric and other nonmotor symptoms on disability in Parkinson’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 52 (5): 784–788. White, L., H. Petrovitch, J. Hardman, J. Nelson, D. G. Davis, G. W. Ross, K. Masaki, L. Launer, and W. R. Markesbery. 2002. Cerebrovascular pathology and dementia in autopsied Honolulu-Asia Aging Study participants. Ann NY Acad Sci 977: 9–23. Williams-Gray, C. H., T. Foltynie, C. E. Brayne, T. W. Robbins, and R. A. Barker. 2007. Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s disease cohort. Brain 130 (Pt. 7): 1787–1798. Winblad, B., G. Grossberg, L. Frolich, M. Farlow, S. Zechner, J. Nagel, and R. Lane. 2007. IDEAL: A 6-month, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the first skin patch for Alzheimer disease. Neurology 69 (4 Suppl. 1): S14–22. Yoritaka, A., H. Ohizumi, S. Tanaka, and N. Hattori. 2009. Parkinson’s disease with and without REM sleep behaviour disorder: Are there any clinical differences? Eur Neurol 61 (3): 164–170.
About the Contributors
SANFORD AUERBACH, MD, is a behavioral neurologist at Boston Medical Center, where he has been the Director of the Sleep Disorders Center and the Director of Behavioral Neurology. He is an Associate Professor of Neurology, Psychiatry, and Behavioral Neurosciences at the Boston University School of Medicine. Dr. Auerbach is a board-certified sleep specialist who has been actively involved in the care of patients with insomnia and other sleep disorders since he first became Director of the Sleep Disorders Center in 1988. He is also responsible for the development of the ACGME-approved fellowship program in sleep medicine at Boston Medical Center. Dr. Auerbach has been actively involved in research efforts at the Framingham Study and the Boston University Alzheimer ’s Center. His research interests have covered a variety of topics in sleep medicine and behavioral neurology. LARS BERTRAM graduated from medical school at Ruhr University in Bochum, Germany, in 1997 and began his clinical training at the Alzheimer Centre of the Klinikum Rechts der Isar in Munich. In 1999, he joined the Genetics and Aging Research Unit at Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH)/Harvard Medical School (led by Professor Rudolph Tanzi) to work as a postdoctoral fellow on the identification and characterization of novel late-onset AD genes. He was appointed Assistant in Genetics at MGH (2002) and to the faculty of Harvard Medical School as Assistant Professor of Neurology (2004). Since October 2008, Dr. Bertram has headed the Neuropsychiatric Genetics Group in the Department of Vertebrate Genomics at the Max-Planck Institute for Molecular Genetics in Berlin. During his career, Dr. Bertram has received numerous awards and honors, including stipends from the Collège Franco-Allemand, the Erasmus Foundation,
316
About the Contributors
the Harvard Center for Neurodegeneration and Repair, and the National Alliance on Research on Schizophrenia and Depression. Scientifically, Dr. Bertram’s expertise lies in the mapping and characterization of complex disease loci, predominantly in the field of neuropsychiatric disorders. In addition to his laboratory work, Dr. Bertram has pioneered the development of bioinformatic approaches that systematically synthesize genetic data for complex disorders. ANNA BURKE, MD, completed her medical school training at the Medical University of Gdansk, Poland. She subsequently went on to complete an internal medicine internship at Mount Sinai School of Medicine in New York, residency training in adult psychiatry at the Institute of Living in Hartford, Connecticut, and fellowship training at McLean Hospital in Belmont, Massachusetts. Dr. Burke currently practices in both a clinical and a research setting as a geriatric psychiatrist and a dementia specialist at Banner Alzheimer ’s Institute in Phoenix, Arizona. She has worked extensively with patients and families suffering with Alzheimer ’s disease and related dementias. Her area of expertise includes diagnosis and treatment of dementia as well as treatment of associated behavioral and psychiatric disturbances. PAUL M. BUTLER, MTS, is an MD-PhD candidate at Boston University School of Medicine. His research interests and publications include topics at the interstices of evolution, medicine, and the humanities. SUSANA CARDOSO finished her degree in biology in 2008. Presently she is working on her PhD in the Faculty of Sciences and Technology at the University of Coimbra, Portugal. In her PhD project, she started by evaluating the impact of type 1 diabetes and insulin-induced hypoglycemia in brain mitochondria, namely cortical and hippocampal mitochondria. She is also interested in exploring the role of mitochondrial uncoupling proteins in hyperglycemic and hypoglycemic situations. She has a PhD fellowship (SFRH/BD/43968/2008) from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology. She received a travel grant from the Federation of European Neuroscience Society/International Brain Research Organization–Western Europe Regional Committee (FENS/IBRO-WERC). CRISTINA CARVALHO is a PhD student of cellular biology in the Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology at University of Coimbra, Portugal. She has a degree in biology and a master ’s degree in molecular and cellular biology. Her research is mainly focused on the role of brain endothelium
About the Contributors
317
dysfunction in brain aging, type 2 diabetes, and Alzheimer ’s disease. Cristina is particularly interested in studying the mitochondrial-associated pathways in the above-mentioned conditions. Her PhD work is supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (SFRH/ BD/43965/2008). She received a travel grant from the Federation of European Neuroscience Society/International Brain Research Organization– Western Europe Regional Committee (FENS/IBRO-WERC). SÓNIA C. CORREIA obtained her biology degree in 2007 from the Faculty of Sciences and Technology at the University of Coimbra, Portugal. She performed her scientific training focused on the effect of metformin, an antidiabetic drug, on brain oxidative stress status in an animal model of type 2 diabetes, under the supervision of Dr. Paula I. Moreira. Currently, she is a cell biology PhD student with the Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology at the University of Coimbra under the supervision of Dr. Moreira. Her current project is testing the hypothesis that mitochondria are crucial organelles involved in the neuroprotection mediated by preconditioning phenomena against diabetes-induced neurodegenerative events. She has a PhD fellowship (SFRH/BD/40702/2007) from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology and was the recipient of fellowships from the Federation of the Societies of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, the Luso-American Foundation, and the Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation. RAYMON DURSO, MD, is an Associate Professor of Neurology at Boston University School of Medicine, a faculty member of the Graduate School of Behavioral Neurosciences at Boston University, and lecturer at Harvard Medical School. He is Director of the Parkinson/Movement Disorders clinics within the Veterans Administration Boston Healthcare System. He has done research on Parkinson disease for more than 25 years. His areas of research interest have focused on levodopa pharmacokinetics and cognitive/neurobehavioral dysfunction in Parkinson disease. ILAN HALPERIN obtained his BA in psychology and social sciences in 2000. He joined the memory disorder clinic and the psychogeriatric clinic at the Sourasky Medical Center in Tel Aviv. In 2001, he started his studies toward a combined MA in research psychology and PhD in brain sciences at the Bar Ilan University. Dr. Halperin became a licensed psychologist in 2006 and earned his PhD in 2009 after completing his research study on the association between cognitive impairment in old age and iron balance disorders. He routinely practices cognitive evaluations in diverse populations that suffer from a variety of cognitive disorders. As part of his duties,
318
About the Contributors
he also participates in clinical trials that concentrate mainly on mild cognitive impairment and dementia. His research interests include the fields of neuropsychology, psychiatry, and aging. Currently, he focuses on the association between dementia and depression. ERICA HARRIS, MPH, is a PhD candidate in behavioral neuroscience at Boston University School of Medicine (BUSM). She graduated from the University of Virginia in 2001 with a BA in psychology and then worked at Duke University Medical Center on studies involving at-risk youths and how they make successful transitions in school. She then obtained her MPH from Boston University in 2005 with dual concentrations in epidemiology and social and behavioral sciences. She is currently a grants administrator in the Department of Neurology at BUSM. Her research interests and publications are related to sleep, Parkinson’s disease, the concept of the self, and the relationship between religion and the brain. HOWARD S. KIRSHNER, MD, is Professor and Vice Chairman of the Department of Neurology and an Adjunct Professor of Psychiatry and Hearing and Speech Sciences at Vanderbilt University Medical Center. He also serves as the Director of the Vanderbilt Stroke Center and the Program Director of Stroke Rehabilitation at Vanderbilt Stallworth Rehabilitation Hospital. He is a graduate of Williams College and Harvard Medical School. He completed his residency in medicine and neurology at the Massachusetts General Hospital as well as a Staff Associate position at the National Institutes of Health. Dr. Kirshner joined the Vanderbilt faculty in the Department of Neurology in 1978. His clinical and research interests have included acute stroke treatment, stroke prevention, and rehabilitation of stroke deficits, especially aphasia and cognitive disorders. He also has interest in the dementias, including Alzheimer ’s disease, vascular dementia, and frontotemporal dementia. He is currently the book review editor of the Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology and has served as an editor of Neurology, the online neurology text EMedicine, and the behavioral neurology issue of Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. AMOS D. KORCZYN is the Sieratzki Professor of Neurology at Tel Aviv University. He graduated from the Hebrew University–Hadassah Medical School in Jerusalem in 1966 with an MD and an MSc in pharmacology (cum laude). He trained in neurology at Beilinson Hospital and at the National Hospital for Nervous Diseases in London. He was the chairman of the Department of Neurology at the Tel Aviv Medical Center from 1981 to 2002. Professor Korczyn has a particular interest in dementia. He has
About the Contributors
319
authored or coauthored more than 600 articles in peer-reviewed journals, as well as chapters in books. He is or has been an editorial board member of 15 international journals and organized several neurological conferences, mainly in the field of dementia, Parkinson’s disease, and other degenerative brain disorders as well as CONy–the International Congress on Controversies in Neurology. JAMES B. LEVERENZ received his undergraduate and medical education at the University of Washington in Seattle. He obtained his neurology training at the New York Hospital–Cornell Medical Center and neuropathology fellowship training at the University of Chicago. Currently he is a Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at the University of Washington and a neurologist in the VA–Puget Sound Health Care System (Mental Illness and Parkinson’s Disease Research Education and Clinical Centers). He directs the clinical core of the Pacific Northwest Udall Center and the education core for the University of Washington Alzheimer ’s Disease Research Center. His primary clinical and research interests are the Lewy body–associated neurodegenerative disorders including Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. KIYOSHI MAEDA, MD, PhD, is a Professor in the Division of Psychiatry at the Kobe University Graduate School of Medicine. PAULA I. MOREIRA, PhD, is a Researcher at the Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology and a Teaching Assistant of Physiology in the Faculty of Medicine at the University of Coimbra, Portugal. Her research is focused primarily on the impact of neurodegenerative conditions on brain function, with special focus on bioenergetics. She has also studied the physiologic process of aging and the pathologic process of diabetes as important risk factors for neurodegeneration. She is editor for several journals including the Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease, Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, Frontiers in Dementia, Open Neurology Journal, and Mini-Reviews in Medicinal Chemistry. She won the Stimulus to Research prize in 2003, supported by the Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation, and the L’Oreal for Women in Science award in 2008, supported by L’Oreal Portugal/UNESCO/Foundation for Science and Technology. HARUHIKO ODA, MD, PhD, is head physician of the Department of the Aging Brain and Cognitive Disorders at the Hyogo Brain and Heart Center in Himeji, Japan. Upon graduating from the Kobe University School of Medicine in 2001, he trained at the Kobe University Hospital as a geriatric
320
About the Contributors
psychiatrist. He began neuropsychological research on patients with dementia during his doctoral course in medical sciences at the Kobe University Graduate School of Medicine in 2005. He has published articles on the neuropsychological profile of dementia with Lewy bodies. RENATO X. SANTOS obtained her biology degree in 2008 from the Faculty of Sciences and Technology at the University of Coimbra, Portugal. He spent a year of scientific training under the supervision of Dr. Paula I. Moreira, devoting his attention to the effects of diet on brain oxidative status and to the study of brain mitochondrial bioenergetics. He is currently a PhD student with the Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology at the University of Coimbra under the supervision of Dr. Moreira. As a graduate student in the cell biology field, his interests are focused on the impact of diabetes and Alzheimer ’s disease on brain mitochondrial dynamics. His work is supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (SFRH/BD/43972/2008), and he was the recipient of a fellowship from the Luso-American Foundation. PHILIPPE TAUPIN earned his PhD in neurosciences from the Université Pierre et Marie Curie in France. He did his postdoctoral studies in the laboratory of Professor F. H. Gage at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies in San Diego. After completing his postdoctoral studies, he went on to take the position of Head of Laboratory and Associate Professor and to set up his own research program and laboratory in Singapore. Recently, he was appointed as scientific director of a newly created research institute and as a Professor at Dublin City University, Ireland. He is currently editor-in-chief of the Journal of Neurodegeneration and Regeneration. He has been engaged as an entrepreneur in setting up a biotechnology company to bring his expertise and experience to the pharmaceutical and biotechnology sector. BROOKE K. WALTER graduated from the University of Virginia School of Medicine and completed both her neurology residency and movement disorders fellowship at University of Washington in Seattle. She is a movement disorders neurologist in Portland, Oregon. KESHA WILFORD, MD, is a board-certified neurologist and movement disorder specialist who is currently completing a fellowship in sleep medicine at Boston Medical Center. YASUJI YAMAMOTO, MD, PhD, is an Assistant Professor in the Division of Psychiatry at the Kobe University Graduate School of Medicine.
About the Series Editor
PATRICK MCNAMARA, PhD, is Associate Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry at Boston University School of Medicine (BUSM) and is Director of the Evolutionary Neurobehavior Laboratory in the Department of Neurology at the BUSM and the VA New England Healthcare System. Upon graduating from the Behavioral Neuroscience Program at Boston University in 1991, he trained at the Aphasia Research Center at the Boston VA Medical Center in neurolinguistics and brain-cognitive correlation techniques. He then began developing an evolutionary approach to problems of brain and behavior and currently is studying the evolution of the frontal lobes, the evolution of the two mammalian sleep states (REM and NREM) and the evolution of religion in human cultures.
Index
acetylcholine, 136–140, 170–171 acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AchEi), 249, 250, 305, 307 ACh, 145, 260 advanced glycation endproducts (AGEs), 93 ageism, 186 aggression, 139, 141, 142–144 aging, 67, 183–184, 193, 279; dementia and, 184–185; depression and, 190–192 (see also depression); gender and, 66; gene conflict and, 61–62; kinship theory and, 59, 61; neurotransmitter changes and, 200–203; psychology of, 186, 286; quality-of-life (QoL) and, 187–190 agitation, 139, 140, 141, 142–143 akinesia, 261 ␣-ketoglutarate dehydrogenase, 95 alpha-synuclein aggregation, 28, 29, 30 alpha-synucleinopathies, 172 alpha-synuclein protein (SNCA), 27–31, 41, 46, 49–50, 259, 262 alpha 2-receptors, 141 AlzGene, 12, 13, 16, 17 Alzheimer ’s Assessment Scale (ADAS), 274 Alzheimer ’s disease, 89–90, 115–116, 185; behavior and
psychological disturbances and, 136–151 (see also specific neurotransmitters); earlyonset (EOAD), 2, 3 (table), 3–4, 115–116, 118, 119, 120; familial, 1–2, 115–116, 123; genes and, 1–2, 5–6, 14, 15–17; insulin and, 99–102; late-onset (LOAD), 2, 6–7, 8–9 (table), 115, 118–119, 120, 128; mitochondrial abnormalities and, 91, 92, 94, 95–99, 102; mixed, 250; neurogenesis and, 116–117, 120–125, 126 (figure), 127–128; oxidative stress and, 92–99, 102–103, 128; Parkinson’s disease and, 174–175, 304; pathophysiology of, 5, 92–102, 117–122; screening, 306, 307 (see also screening, dementia and); sleep disorders and, 285–288, 292. See also familial Alzheimer ’s disease (FAD); genetic association studies; genome-wide Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS), 305, 307 Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study-Clinician’s Global Impression of Change (ADCS-CGIC), 307
324
Index
amantadine, 26 American Academy of Neurology (AAN), 303, 304 amnestic syndrome, 245, 246 amyloid angiopathy, 3, 244–245. See also angiopathy amyloid  (A) peptide, 1, 4, 5, 90, 93, 96, 97–98, 101; in vitro, 16; production, 10–12, 15, 90, 245. See also oxidative stress amyloid  precursor protein (A PP), 89, 90, 94, 96–97, 101, 117–118, 120–121. See also amyloid deposits; oxidative stress amyloid deposits, 123–124. See also amyloid  (A) peptide; amyloid precursor protein (APP) amyloid plaques. See amyloid  (A) peptide; amyloid precursor protein (APP) amyloid precursor protein (APP), 10, 12, 18; early-onset familial Alzheimer ’s disease (EOFAD) and, 2–3, 3 (table), 4, 6 amyloidopathies, 285–288 aneuploidy, 119–120, 122–123, 128–129; chromosomes and, 123–124; neurogenesis and, 124–127 Angelman’s syndrome, 32, 59 angeopathy, 244–245 angiotensin converting enzyme 1 (ACE), 16 anticholinergic agents, 137, 264, 265 anticonvulsants, 146 antidepressants, 203, 287, 290, 300 antipsychotics. See atypical antipsychotics; neuroleptics anxiety, 140, 141, 143, 287 apathy, 142, 221; Parkinson’s disease and, 221–228, 300; subtypes of, 228–230; treatment, 230–232 Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES), 224, 226
Apathy Inventory (AI), 225 Apathy Scale (AS), 224, 225 apolipoprotein E (APOE), 6–7, 10–12, 18, 89 apolipoprotein epsilon 4 allele (APOE 4), 7, 8 (table), 13, 118–119; risk factors and, 10, 13 apoptosis, 27, 28, 30 AP-2 ␣, 38 aripiprazole, 266 aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase (AAAD), 40–41 arteriovenous malformations (AVM), 248–249 asperger syndrome, 266 ATP (adenosine-5-triphosphate), 92, 102 attentional function, 269–270 atypical antipsychotics, 144–145. See also neuroleptics auto-activation deficit, 230 Back Depression Inventory, 191 basal ganglia, 222, 223, 224, 228, 229, 230 Beckwith-Widemann syndrome, 59 behavioral and psychological disturbances in dementia (BPSD), 135–151, 267, 299–301 benzodiazepines, 146 beta-adrenergic receptor bindings, 141  blocking agents, 142  noradrenergic receptor agonists, 202 Binswanger ’s disease, 247 bioinformatics models, 33, 36–37 (table) bipolar disorder, 196–197, 266 blood-brain barrier (BBB), 99 Braak hypothesis, 173, 174 bradykinesia, 261 brain changes, 197–198. See also cognitive impairment; neurotransmitter changes
Index brain metabolism. See glucose metabolism bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU), 121 CADASIL (Cerebral Autosomal Dominant Arteriopathy with Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy), 247–249, 250 calcium, intracellular (Ca2+), 92, 102, 148 California Criteria for Ischemic Vascular Dementia (IVD), 239, 240 (table) Cambridge Mental Disorders in Elderly Examination (CAMCOG), 271 cardiovascular disease, 199 catecholamine metabolism, 38, 39 (figure), 39–42 cathepsin D (CTSD), 55 caudate activity, 164, 165, 166 cell cycle re-entry, 12, 122–123, 125, 128 cell proliferation, 12, 125 central nervous system (CNS), 116, 128; acetylcholine deficiency in, 136–137; degeneration and, 121. See also sleep, disorders central sleep apnea (CSA), 284–285 cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), 100, 136, 141, 149, 201, 243, 304 cerebrovascular disease, 183, 186 (figure), 199, 207–208 cholesterol, 10, 242 cholinergic deficits, 136, 137–140, 171 cholinesterase inhibitor, 26, 137, 138, 139–140, 249–250, 270 chromosome 1 (1p36), 56 chromosome 1 (1q31-q42), 124 chromosome 11 (11p15.5), 53–55 chromosome 14 (14q24), 4, 124 chromosome 17 (17q21.1), 124, 125 chromosome 19 (19q13.2), 124 chromosome 21 (21q21), 2, 123–124, 125
325
circadian rhythm disorders (CRDs), 282–285, 286, 288, 289 citalopram, 144 Clock Drawing Test (CDT), 193–194 clusterin (Clu), 15 cognitive impairment (CI): fluctuation and, 262–263, 266; Parkinson’s disease and, 163–167, 295, 296–299, 301, 303, 305, 308; postdepression, 205–206; reaction to, 195; reduction, 241–242; screening, 193–194, 264, 305–307 (see also cognitive rating scales); stroke and, 242–243; transient ischemic attacks and, 242. See also executive cognitive function; mild cognitive impairment (MCI) cognitive impairment with no dementia (CIND), 295, 299, 300, 301 cognitive rating scales, 168–169, 193– 194, 264. See also specific tests collegen vascular disease, 248 computerized tomography (CT), 303 cordisol, 202, 203 cortical dementia, 160 corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRF), 149–150 Creutzfeldt-Jacob disease (CJD), 291, 292 Cumulative Illness Rating Scale, 205 cyclin B, 120 cyclophilin D, 98 cystatin 3 (CST3), 16 cytochrome c oxidase (COX) activity, 96 DAG (diacylglycerol), 47 deficit syndrome, 231 dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), 203 dementia: depression and, 195–197, 203–207, 208 (figure), 208–209; neurotransmitter systems and, 200–203 (see also
326
Index
neurotransmitters); reversible, 194; risk factors, 196–199, 203–205; screening, 192–194, 303. See also Alzheimer ’s disease; behavioral and psychological disturbances in dementia (BPSD); dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB); frontotemporal dementia; Parkinson’s disease (PD); Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD); vascular dementia (VaD); vascular disease Dementia Rating Scale, 205 dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), 171–174, 258–259, 275–276; neuropsychological profile, 268–275, 289; pathology of, 259–260; symptoms, 261–268 dentate gyrus (DG), 116, 120, 122, 125, 127 depression, 139, 141, 142, 143, 148, 206–209; brain changes and, 197–198; dementia and, 195–198, 203–205, 208 (figure); early onset, 190; isolation and, 186; late-life, 190–192, 206, 207–209; neurotransmitter system and, 200–203; Parkinson’s disease and, 223, 227; post-, 205–206, 207–208; reversible dementia and, 194; screening, 191–192; vascular brain disease and, 199–200 depression-executive dysfunction (DED), 203–204, 205 developmental stages, 60 (table) DGK- (diacylglycerol kinase, theta), 45–50 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), 190, 298, 301 dividing cells, 122, 125 DJ-1, 30, 41
DNA duplication, 121, 125, 128 donepezil, 138, 249, 250, 306 (table) DOPA decarboxylase (DDC), 40–41 dopamine (DA), 145, 150–151, 222, 223; aggregates, 39, 40; dysfunction, 27; midbrain, 35, 37; Parkinson’s disease and, 164–167; receptor, 52–55; transport, 267. See also pharmaceuticals, dopamine and dopamine beta-hydroxylase (DBH), 40 dopamine-four-receptor (DRD4), 53–55 dopamine transporter imaging, 267 dorsolateral frontal lobes, 164, 165, 166 Down’s syndrome, 4, 123 dynamin-related protein 1 (Drp1), 98–99 early-onset Alzheimer ’s disease (EOAD), 2, 3 (table), 3–4, 115–116, 118, 119, 120; genes and, 2, 3 (table), 3–4, 5–6 early onset depression (EOD), 190 [18F]fluorodopa, 164, 166, 170 emotional-affective apathy, 229 entorhinal cortex, 197 excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS), 284, 291 executive cognitive function, 160–161, 163, 164; Parkinson’s disease and, 229–230; vascular dementia and, 245 executive dysfunctions, 203 extracellular plaques, 185. See also amyloid deposits familial Alzheimer ’s disease (FAD), 1–2, 115–116, 123; early-onset, 2, 3 (table), 3–4 familial Parkinson’s disease, 173 fatal familial insomnia (FFI), 291 fibroblasts, 95, 96, 98–99 5-HTR2A (serotonin receptor, 2A), 42
Index fluctuation, 262–263, 266 Foxa2 (forkhead box protein A2), 34–35, 37 free radical theory of aging, 65 frontal cortex, 164, 166, 228. See also ventrolateral frontal cortex frontal lobes, 230 frontotemporal dementia (FTD), 5, 149, 288, 289 GAK (cyclin G associated kinase), 46 galantamine, 138, 249, 306 (table) Gamma-aminobutyric Acid (GABA), 145–147, 150, 281 GATA2 (GATA-binding protein 2), 44–45 gender, 66, 114, 189 gene conflict, 59, 61–62. See also imprinted genes gene inheritance, 1–2, 5–6, 14, 15–18. See also genetic association studies; genome-wide association analysis (GWAS) gene mutation: Alzheimer ’s disease and, 2, 3–4, 5–6, 89, 94, 115, 118–119; apolipoprotein E (APOE) and, 6, 10, 118 General Health Questionnaire, 191 genetic association studies, 12–13 genetic risk factor. See apolipoprotein E (APOE); gene inheritance genome-wide association analysis (GWAS), 2, 7, 11–12 (table), 13, 14, 15, 17 Geriatric Depression Scale, 191, 204 Gerstamm-Straussler-Scheinker (GSS) disease, 291, 292 Global Severity Score, 224 glucocorticoid hormones, 202, 203 glucose metabolism, 99–100, 101, 102–103 glucose transporters (GLUTs), 99 glutamate (Glu), 147–149 glutamate receptor, 250
327
glutathione (GSH), 94 GRB10 (Growth factor receptor bound protein 10), 50–52 GSK-3, 102 Hackinski Ischemic Score, 239, 239 (table) hallucinations, 142, 148, 288–289; Lewy bodies and, 172, 257, 260, 263–264, 271–273; pharmaceuticals and, 137, 270; as a symptom, 168, 260, 263–264, 267, 287, 299, 300 haloperidol, 137 Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD), 191 haploidy, 31, 32 hippocampus, 100, 101, 116–120, 125, 127, 129 hormonal system, 202–203 Human Genome Epidemiology Network (HuGENet), 13 Huntington’s disease (HD), 290–291 hypercortisolism, 202, 203 hypersomnia, 284, 287, 288, 289, 292 hypertension, 241, 247, 249 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, 202 IGF-1, 102 IL8 (interleukin 8), 16 imaging studies, 163, 169, 170–171; dopamine transport, 267; vascular dementia (VaD) and, 239; white-matter disease and, 247. See also neuroimaging studies impaired antioxidant defenses, 94 imprinted genes, 46 (figure), 53–54 (table); bioinformatics models and, 36–37 (table); catecholamine metabolism and, 38, 39 (figure), 39–42; centers for, 52, 55–56; dysfunction of, 32; kinship theory and, 58–59;
328
Index
Lewy body formation and, 44–45; maternally, 37, 38–40, 41–42, 44–50, 51(figure), 52; midbrain and, 34–35, 37–38; mitochondrial function and, 42–44; paternally, 40–41, 52, 66; X-linked, 56, 57 (table), 58, 66 impulsivity, 143 inbreeding, 59, 61, 62 insomnia, 282, 284, 292. See also specific sleep disorders insulin, 90, 99–102 insulin-degrading enzyme (IDE), 101 insulin-like growth factor-1 receptor (IGF-1R), 99 insulin receptor (IR), 99–100, 101 insulin-resistant brain state, 101 intergenerational transfer, 62–64, 67 intracerebroventricular (icv) injection of diabetogenic streptozotocin (STZ), 101–102 irritability, 144 ischemic stroke, 238, 246 isocitrate dehydrogenase, 95 kinship theory, 32–33, 58–59, 61 lacunar state, 246–247 late-life depression (LLD), 190–192, 206, 207–209 late-onset Alzheimer ’s disease (LOAD), 2, 6–7, 8–9 (table), 115, 118–120, 128 late-onset depression (LOD), 190–191 L-DOPA, 40, 55 left-versus right-onset Parkinson’s disease, 232 Leigh syndrome, 43 lesions, 224; apathy and, 229, 230; Lewy bodies and, 28; vascular, 238, 243, 244, 245, 248–249, 250 (see also vascular dementia [VaD]); white-matter, 243–244
leucine-rich repeat kinase 2 gene (LRRK2), 29 leukoaraiosis. See white-matter disease levodopa, 166–167, 264, 265 Lewy bodies, 27, 28, 29, 30, 258–259; formation of, 44–45. See also dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) LIM1A (LIM homeobox transcription factor 1 alpha), 35 LIM1B (LIM homeobox transcription factor 1 beta), 35, 37 lipid peroxidation, 93 Little Apathy Rating Scale (LARS), 224–225, 226–227 locus coeruleus (LC), 140, 141, 142, 201–202 longevity, 64, 65, 67 low-density lipoprotein receptor (LDLR), 16 lymphocytes, 119, 122 madumnal alleles, 59, 60 (table), 62, 63–64, 66 magnetic resonance imaging (MRI): depression and, 198; Parkinson’s disease and, 165, 167, 303; silent stroke and, 243–244 mania, 139, 141 maternal alleles, 52, 63 (figure). See also imprinted genes, maternally medication. See pharmaceuticals melatonin, 280, 283, 284, 286, 290 memantine, 250, 306 (table), 308 memory, 136, 139, 150, 273–274. See also working memory memory testing, 161–163, 165, 166–167, 171 mesocortical tract, 166, 167 metabolic syndrome, 242 MHPG, 140, 141 midbrain, 166 mild cognitive impairment (MCI), 94, 195–196, 207 (figure), 228, 246,
Index 298; apathy and, 229; depression and, 205, 206, 207 (figure) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), 168, 193–194, 225, 227, 261, 270–271, 275 mitochondrial cascade hypothesis, 90 mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) mutation, 92, 94–95, 96 mitochondrial durability, 90, 92 mitochondrial dysfunction, 90, 91 (figure), 92, 94, 95–99, 102 mitochondrial function, 42–44, 95 mitochondrial fusion, 29 mood disorders, 199. See also psychiatric conditions movement disorders, 261. See also parkinsonism; Parkinson’s disease Movement Disorder Society Task Force, 301, 302–303 Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS), 222–223 mRNA, 100, 101 multi-infarct dementia, 246 multiple-system atrophy, 173, 189 muscarinic receptors, 137 NDUFA4 (NADH ubiquinone oxidoreductase Protein A 4), 42–43 NDUFS4 (NADH ubiquinone oxidoreductase Fe-S Protein 4), 42–43 nervous system. See central nervous system (CNS) neurodegenerative disease, 32. See specific diseases neurofibrillary tangles (NFT), 5, 90, 117, 118, 124; formation, 93, 102; studies, 202 neurogenesis, 116–117, 120–124, 128; aneuploidy and, 124–125, 126 (figure), 127 neuroimaging studies, 146, 197, 199, 267–268. See also imaging
329
studies; magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); positron emission tomography (PET); single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) neuroleptics, 265, 266–267. See also atypical antipsychotics neuron, 92; damage, 93; degeneration, 120; genesis of, 116–117; glucose and, 99, 102; Lewy bodies and, 172 (see also impaired antioxidant defense; oxidative stress). See also dopamine; neurotransmitter; specific neurons neuropathology, 1, 244, 259–260. See also specific diseases neuropeptides, 149–150 neuropeptide Y, 149 Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), 225, 227, 264 neurotoxicity, 148 neurotransmitter changes, 136–151, 200–203 neurotransmitters, 135–151. See also Gamma-aminobutyric Acid (GAMA) nigra, 163–164 nigrostriatal-thalamic-cortex, 163, 167 NINDS-AIREN criteria, 239, 240 (table), 240–241 N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), 147–148, 307 non-rapid eye movement (NREM), 279, 280 noradrenergic changes, 201–202 noradrenergic neurons, 140–143 norepinephrine (NA), 140–143, 145 neural stem cells (NSCs), 116–117, 129 nucleic acid oxidation, 93 nucleus basalis of Meynert (NBM), 139, 170 obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), 282, 284–285, 286, 287–288
330
Index
olfactory impairment, 226 ontogenetic development, 62 oxidative stress, 92–99, 102, 128 padumnal alleles, 59, 60 (table), 62, 66 paralysis, 265–266 parasomnias, 282 PARK genes, 173 Parkin (PARK2), 30, 31 Parkinson, James, 295 parkinsonism, 265, 267, 273, 289, 290; characteristics of, 257; dementia and, 261–262, 266, 288, 289; pharmaceuticals and, 40; research and, 44, 258. See also Parkinson’s disease (PD); Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD) Parkinson’s disease (PD), 295, 298; Alzheimer ’s disease and, 174–175, 304; apathy and, 221–222, 224–230; clinical characteristics of, 26–27, 298–301, 302 (figure); cognitive impairment and, 164–169; dementia and, 295–296, 296 (figure), 302 (figure), 306 (table) (see also dementia with Lewy bodies [DLB]; Parkinson’s disease dementia [PDD]); diagnosis, 301–304; dopamine and, 164–167; evolutionary perspectives, 58–59, 61–67, 159–163; genetics of, 27–31, 60 (table); genomic imprinting and, 31–34; imaging studies and, 163, 170–171; imprinted genes and, 34–35, 36–37 (table), 37–38, 39 (figure), 39–52, 53–54 (table), 55–56, 58–59; kinship theory and, 32–33, 58–59, 61, 67–68; Lewy bodies and, 171–172, 258–259, 261–262; neurobiology of, 223–224, 304–305; research, 297 (table); treatment, 230–232,
305, 306 (table), 307 (see also gene imprinting); X-linked genes and, 56, 57 (table), 58. See also parkinsonism Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD), 295, 296, 296 (figure), 297 (table); clinical characteristics of, 298–301, 302 (figure); diagnosis, 301–304; pathophysiology of, 304–305; treatment, 305, 306 (table), 307–308. See also dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB); Parkinson’s disease (PD), dementia and PAX transcription activation domain interacting protein (PAXIP1), 5, 6 periodic limb movements (PLMs), 283, 288 peroxisome proliferators-activated receptor gamma (PPARG), 44 pharmaceuticals: acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, 249, 250, 305, 307; Alzheimer ’s disease and, 138, 139–140, 142–143, 144; anticholinergics, 264, 265, 270; antidepressants, 203, 287, 290, 300; apathy and, 230–231; atypical antipsychotics, 144–145, 266, 267; cholinesterase inhibitors, 26, 137, 138, 139–140, 270, 300; dopamine and, 40, 230, 264, 265; GABA receptors and, 146; Lewy bodies and, 264, 265, 270; neuroleptics, 265, 266–267; norepinephrine and, 142–143; Parkinson’s disease and, 166–167, 300, 305, 306 (table), 307–308; serotonergic agents and, 144; sleep and, 287, 288, 290, 291; vascular dementia and, 249–250. See also specific pharmaceuticals phencyclidine (PCP), 148 pheochromocytoma cells (PC12), 95
Index phylogentic comparison, 62 physostigmine, 137 PICALM (phosphatidylinositol binding clathrin assembly protein), 14, 15 PINK1, 30 PI-3K, 102 polymerase ␥ (POLG), 44 polyunsaturated fats (PUFAs), 49, 50 positron emission tomography (PET), 99, 267–268, 272; Parkinson’s disease and, 164, 165, 170–171 postmortem studies: alpha 2-receptors and, 141; depression and, 197–198; Lewy bodies and, 174; mtDNA mutation and, 96; neurotransmitters and, 137, 140, 143–144, 146, 149; TCA enzyme complexes and, 95; vascular disease and, 244 Prader-Willi syndrome, 34, 59 prazosin, 142–143 prefrontal cortex (PFC), 229 presenilin 1 (PSEN1), 3 (table), 4, 10, 18, 89, 94, 119, 120, 124; mutation and, 127 presenilin 2 (PSEN2), 3 (table), 4, 18, 89, 94, 119, 124 prion disorders, 291–292 PROGRESS trial, 241 propranolol, 142 protein oxidation, 93 psychiatric conditions, 150, 199, 200, 223, 266–267. See also bipolar disorder; depression; psychosis; schizophrenia; unipolar disorder psychological resource, 187–188 psychosis, 137, 138, 139, 142, 148, 150. See also hallucinations; psychiatric conditions pyruvate dehydrogenase (PDH), 95 quality-of-life (QoL), 187–190 quetiapine, 266
331
R406W, 5 rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, 279–280, 281, 286, 301. See also REM Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD) Ravens Progressive Matrices, 162 reactive oxygen species (ROS): Alzheimer ’s disease and, 92, 94–95; Parkinson’s disease and, 27, 28, 30 REM Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD), 265–266, 282, 287, 289, 301 reproduction, 61, 62, 63 (figure), 64 respiratory chain complexes, 95 restless leg syndrome (RLS), 282, 283, 286, 288 reversible dementia, 194 reward-related behaviors, 150 risk factors, 241–242; dementia and, 196–199, 203–205; genetic, 10, 13 risperidone, 266 rivastigmine, 26, 138, 306 (table), 307 schizophrenia, 148, 231, 266 scopolamine, 137, 138 screening tests: apathy and, 224–228; cognitive impairment and, 193–194, 264, 305–307 (see also cognitive rating scales); dementia and, 192–194, 303; visuoperceptual, 270–272 selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), 204–205 serotonergic agents, 144 serotonin (5-HT), 143–145, 200, 201 severe neuroleptic sensitivity, 266–267 silent stroke, 243–244, 246 Silver-Russell syndrome, 59 single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), 268–269, 272 sleep, 139, 143, 279–282; in dementia, 285–292; disorders, 282–285
332
Index
(see also REM Sleep Behavior Disorder [RBD]) sleep-distorted breathing, 284, 286, 287–288, 291 Sneddon’s syndrome, 248 social activity, 186 social position, 188 socioeconomic factors (SEFs), 188–190 somatostatin, 149 SORL1 (sortilin-related receptor), 15 stem cells, 116 stroke, 199, 237, 238–239, 242–243: silent, 243–244, 246 Stroop Color-Word test, 205 subcortical dementia, 160 substantia nigra pars compacyta (SNc), 222 substantia nigral tissue, 26, 27 subventricular zone (SVZ), 116, 125 succinic dehydrogenase (SDH), 96 suicide, 201 suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), 139, 280, 286 Symbol Digit Modalities test, 227 synuclein, 172–173, 174. See also alphasynuclein protein (SNCA) synucleinopathies, 289–290 Syst-Eur hypertension study, 241 tau hyperphosphory, 5, 93, 102, 124, 125 tauopathies, 288–289 TCA enzyme complexes, 95 thiol, 92, 94 thiol redox circuits, 92 TNK1 (tyrosine kinase, non receptor, 1), 15–16 Tourette syndrome, 266 Tower of London (TOL) test, 162, 230 transient ischemic attack (TIAs), 242 transmissible spongiform encephalopathy, 291–292 tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-␣), 15–16 Two-Hit hypothesis, 93 tyrostine hydroxylase (TH), 38–40, 41
unipolar disorder, 196–197, 199 University of Illinois RMDAS (Risk Markers for Dementia after Stroke) project, 243 vascular cognitive impairment. See vascular dementia (VaD) vascular dementia (VaD), 185–186; CADASIL and, 247–249; neurobiology, 245–246; prevention, 249; risk factors, 241–246; treatment, 249 vascular depression hypothesis, 198 vascular disease, 199–200, 237–238, 241–245; diagnostic criteria and, 238–239, 239 (table), 240 (table), 240–241; Parkinson’s disease and, 304; sleep disturbances and, 292. See also vascular dementia (VaD) vasculitis, 248 ventrical tegmental, 166 ventrolateral frontal cortex, 163, 164, 165 vesicular glutamate transporters (VGluT), 147 visuoperceptual function, 270–273 wealth, 189 Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleRevised (WAIS-R), 269, 271, 272 Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R), 270, 274 white matter changes (WMC), 198, 199, 243–244 white matter disease, 247 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, 161–162, 168 Wolf-Hirschhorn syndrome, 47 working memory, 161–163, 164, 167, 171. See also memory xanomeline, 137, 138 X-linked gene, 43, 56, 57 (table), 58, 66 yohimbine, 141–142
DEMENTIA Volume 3: Treatments and Developments Patrick McNamara, Editor
Brain, Behavior, and Evolution Patrick McNamara, Series Editor
This page intentionally left blank.
Copyright 2011 ABC-CLIO, LLC All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, except for the inclusion of brief quotations in a review, without prior permission in writing from the publisher. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Dementia / Patrick McNamara, editor. p. cm.—(Brain, behavior, and evolution) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-0-313-38434-9 (hard copy : alk. paper)—ISBN 978-0-313-38435-6 (ebook) 1. Dementia. 2. Alzheimer ’s disease. I. McNamara, Patrick, 1956– II. Series: Brain, behavior, and evolution [DNLM: 1. Dementia. WM 220] RC521.D4524 2011 616.8’3—dc22 2010041082 ISBN 978-0-313-38434-9 EISBN 978-0-313-38435-6 15
14
13
12
11
1
2
3
4
5
This book is also available on the World Wide Web as an eBook. Visit www.abc-clio.com for details. Praeger An Imprint of ABC-CLIO, LLC ABC-CLIO, LLC 130 Cremona Drive, P.O. Box 1911 Santa Barbara, California 93116-1911 This book is printed on acid-free paper Manufactured in the United States of America
This page intentionally left blank.
Contents
Series Foreword Preface: Hopeful Trends in Meeting the Challenge of the Dementias Patrick McNamara
vii ix
Volume 3: Treatments and Developments Chapter 1. Neuropsychiatry of Dementia: Nonpharmacologic Interventions for Inappropriate Behaviors Jiska Cohen-Mansfield Chapter 2. Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia: Promising Results Dorothy Forbes and Debra Morgan Chapter 3. Nonpharmacological Approaches to Treating Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Advanced Dementia: Person-Centered, Stage-Related Care Karan Kverno
1
23
43
Chapter 4. Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia Pamela Lindsey
73
Chapter 5. Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia: Treatment with Antipsychotics Rosa Liperoti
93
vi
Chapter 6.
Chapter 7.
Contents
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly and in Those at Risk for Dementia Patrick McNamara
119
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis: Compensatory and Regenerative Processes Philippe Taupin
135
Chapter 8.
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care Emmelyne Vasse and Myrra Vernooij-Dassen
151
Chapter 9.
Depression and Dementia Jane S. Saczynski and Rosanna M. Bertrand
173
Chapter 10. Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer ’s Disease Frédéric Assal
205
About the Contributors
233
About the Series Editor
239
Index
241
Series Foreword
Beginning in the 1990s, behavioral scientists—that is, people who study mind, brain, and behavior—began to take the theory of evolution seriously. They began to borrow techniques developed by the evolutionary biologists and apply them to problems in mind, brain, and behavior. Now, of course, virtually all behavioral scientists up to that time had claimed to endorse evolutionary theory, but few used it to study the problems they were interested in. All that changed in the 1990s. Since that pivotal decade, breakthroughs in the behavioral and brain sciences have been constant, rapid, and unremitting. The purpose of the Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series of titles published by ABC-CLIO is to bring these new breakthroughs in the behavioral sciences to the attention of the general public. In the past decade, some of these scientific breakthroughs have come to inform the clinical and biomedical disciplines. That means that people suffering from all kinds of diseases and disorders, particularly brain and behavioral disorders, will benefit from these new therapies. That is exciting news indeed, and the general public needs to learn about these breakthrough findings and treatments. A whole new field called evolutionary medicine has begun to transform the way medicine is practiced and has led to new treatments and new approaches to diseases, like the dementias, sleep disorders, psychiatric diseases, and developmental disorders that seemed intractable to previous efforts. The series of books in the Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series seeks both to contribute to this new evolutionary approach to brain and behavior and to bring the insights emerging from the new evolutionary approaches to psychology, medicine, and anthropology to the general public. The Brain, Behavior, and Evolution series was inspired by and brought to fruition with the help of Debora Carvalko at ABC-CLIO. The series editor,
viii
Series Foreword
Dr. Patrick McNamara, is the director of the Evolutionary Neurobehavior Laboratory in the Department of Neurology at Boston University School of Medicine. He has devoted most of his scientific work to development of an evolutionary approach to problems of sleep medicine and to neurodegenerative diseases. Titles in the series will focus on applied and clinical implications of evolutionary approaches to the whole range of brain and behavioral disorders. Contributions are solicited from leading figures in the fields of interest to the series. Each volume will cover the basics, define the terms, and analyze the full range of issues and findings relevant to the clinical disorder or topic that is the focus of the volume. Each volume will demonstrate how the application of evolutionary modes of analysis leads to new insights on causes of disorder and functional breakdowns in brain and behavior relationships. Each volume, furthermore, will be aimed at both popular and professional audiences and will be written in a style appropriate for the general reader, the local and university libraries, and graduate and undergraduate students. The publications that become part of this series will therefore bring the gold discovered by scientists using evolutionary methods to understand brain and behavior to the attention of the general public, and ultimately, it is hoped, to those families and individuals currently suffering from those most intractable of disorders— the brain and behavioral disorders.
Preface: Hopeful Trends in Meeting the Challenge of the Dementias Patrick McNamara
It is estimated that 24.3 million people around the world have dementia and that, with an estimated 4.6 million new cases every year, we can expect about 43 million people and their families to face the challenge of dementia by 2020. There are several forms of dementia, with the most common being Alzheimer ’s disease (40% of cases), vascular dementia with or without Alzheimer features (25%), and dementia with Lewy bodies (25%), the latter being related to the increasingly important form of dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. The annual healthcare costs for Alzheimer ’s disease alone is estimated at about $155 billion in the United States. A substantial portion of these costs is due to behavioral and neuropsychiatric disturbances associated with the dementing process— yet these neuropsychiatric and behavioral problems have only recently become the focus of study and treatment in the biomedical communities. The successes of neuropsychiatric approaches to the dementias is measured in reduced suffering for patients and their families and reduced healthcare costs for the system as a whole. The authors of the chapters in these three volumes, devoted to emerging trends in dementia studies, have virtually all emphasized identification, study, and treatment of behavioral and neuropsychiatric problems of patients and their families. The reason they have done so is the dawning realization in both the biomedical and caregiving communities that targeting behavioral and neuropsychiatric problems of dementia leads to some pretty effective scientific studies of mechanisms and very effective and low-cost treatment programs that act to alleviate both patients’ suffering and caregivers’ burdens.
x
Preface
Although the standard, it has long been established that dementia most commonly occurs in older people, and that primary symptoms are memory impairment (both short- and long-term), deficits in executive functions, and impairments of abstract thinking and judgment. It has now become crystal clear that some of the best and earliest predictors of dementia risk are mood and personality changes, which all too often are misdiagnosed as depression or some other common mood disorder. Family members may express concern to a primary care physician, but these concerns too often get ignored or shunted aside as a standard mood disorder. It is vitally important to take reports of significant behavioral changes seriously as identification of cognitive components of a dementing process—may be a later-occurring symptom than the behavioral changes. Although the three-step diagnostic process (single question about memory, MMSE, neuropsychological testing) has high positive predictive value, it only detects 18% of future dementia cases. It is the behavioral and neuropsychiatric disturbances, along with incipient cognitive changes, that may yield better detection rates for dementia. Tremendous progress has been made in identification of biomarkers for dementia. The use of functional imaging, proteomic, genetic, biochemical and electrophysiological markers, including sleep polysomnographic techniques, has meant that our ability to detect dementia early on has vastly improved. In addition, the new appreciation of the importance of behavioral and psychiatric problems in dementia as well as validated assessment tools to measure these behavioral problems suggests that it is time to deploy all these new techniques to identify those at risk for dementia so as to prevent or to slow onset of the disorder in these individuals. What is needed are large-scale, multisite, comparative studies that can evaluate optimal use and validity of these various techniques for detecting and selecting asymptomatic people at risk for dementia. The recent Leon Thal Symposium 2009 in Las Vegas, Nevada, explored algorithms, biomarkers, and assessment tools for identifying asymptomatic individuals at elevated risk for dementia. The consensus recommendations of symposium participants included: 1. Establishment of a National Database for Longitudinal Studies as a shared research core resource; 2. Launch of a large collaborative study that will compare multiple screening approaches and biomarkers to determine the best method for identifying asymptomatic people at risk;
Preface
xi
3. Initiation of a Global Database that extends the concept of the National Database for Longitudinal Studies for longitudinal studies beyond the United States; and 4. Development of an educational campaign that will promote healthy brain aging. (Khachaturian et al. 2010) These are all laudable recommendations. But behavioral and neuropsychiatric assessment tools must be included in these large multisite studies of at-risk individuals. A perusal of the essays in these volumes (volume 1 focuses on epidemiologic, descriptive, historical, and diagnostic innovations in dementia; volume 2 focuses on biobehavioral mechanisms of dementia; and volume 3 focuses on emerging treatment strategies including treatments for behavioral problems of dementia) leaves one with a sense of hope and confidence that the daunting challenges of the dementias, both for patients and for families, are finally being effectively addressed. REFERENCE Khachaturian, Z. S., D. Barnes, R. Einstein, et al. 2010. Developing a national strategy to prevent dementia: Leon Thal Symposium 2009. Alzheimer ’s and Dementia 6 (2): 89–97.
Chapter 1
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia: Nonpharmacologic Interventions for Inappropriate Behaviors Jiska Cohen-Mansfield
Persons with dementia often exhibit inappropriate behaviors, which may be distressing and incomprehensible to caregivers as well as emotionally and financially taxing. These behaviors often reflect distress of persons with dementia and affect their care. Managing the inappropriate behaviors associated with dementia is one of the most difficult challenges a clinician must face when treating patients with this disease (Finkel and Burns 2000), and the inability to successfully meet this challenge often results in the patient’s institutionalization (Cohen, Gold, Shulman, et al. 1993; Burke and Morgenlander 1999; Hebert et al. 2001), increased burden on the family and professional caregivers, and utilization of pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic treatments. For the purpose of this chapter, inappropriate behaviors are defined as “inappropriate verbal, vocal, or motor activity that is not judged by an outside observer to be an obvious outcome of the needs or confusion of the individual” (Cohen-Mansfield and Billig 1986). These have been labeled problem behaviors, disruptive behaviors, disturbing behaviors, behavioral problems, and agitation, all terms that can be used interchangeably. Inappropriate behaviors may result from depressed or anxious affect, but the term refers specifically to observable behavior rather than internal states. Inappropriate behaviors have been divided into four main subtypes (Cohen-Mansfield 2008; Cohen-Mansfield et al. 1995): physically
2
Dementia
aggressive behaviors, such as hitting, kicking, or biting; physically nonaggressive behaviors, such as pacing or inappropriately handling objects; verbally nonaggressive agitation, such as constant repetition of sentences or requests; and verbal aggression, such as cursing or screaming. In the past, inappropriate behaviors have been treated with psychotropic drugs or physical restraints, or were simply ignored. Due to ethical and utility factors, such as the ethics and lack of efficacy of physical restraint and the potential for adverse drug side effects, researchers questioned these practices, leading to the Omnibus Budget Reconciliation Act (OBRA) mandate of 1987 to reduce physical and chemical restraints (Siegler et al. 1997). Additionally, concern regarding physical restraints and pharmacologic treatment, as well as research documenting the role of unmet needs in the presentation of behavior problems, has prompted the development of nonpharmacologic interventions for dementia-related inappropriate behaviors and their adoption as the first line of treatment in multiple clinical guidelines (e.g., the American Geriatrics Society (2009); the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry as described in Lyketsos, Colenda, and Beck 2006; American Medical Directors Association 2009; Salzman et al. 2008). Indeed, organizations such as the American Geriatrics Society state that, when treating dementia-related agitation, the context and environmental triggers of inappropriate behaviors should be examined, it should be determined whether delusions or hallucinations are interfering with function, underlying physical discomfort should be explored as a cause, and if underlying physical discomfort can be excluded, nonpharmacologic strategies should be explored. Only when nonpharmacologic strategies are ineffective in treating the agitation should a pharmacologic agent be selected on the basis of symptoms. This chapter addresses the underlying assumptions and the importance of nonpharmacologic interventions, reviews trials of nonpharmacologic interventions, and discusses the impact of and barriers to knowledge and implementation of nonpharmacologic interventions. UNMET NEEDS AND THE IMPORTANCE OF NONPHARMACOLOGIC INTERVENTIONS There are several theories of the etiology of inappropriate behaviors, including biological and genetic origins of these behaviors (e.g., Craig et al. 2004; Holmes 2000; McIlroy and Craig 2004), a behavioral model of the behavior as triggered and reinforced by the environment, a theory of reduced stress threshold in dementia, and an unmet needs model (CohenMansfield 2000b). In this chapter we will describe the latter model in
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
3
greater detail. The choice of this model is based on research results as well as on the finding of a strong placebo response in clinical trials of persons with dementia (Katz et al. 1999; Teri et al. 2000), which reflects the “added attention” patients receive during their participation in a clinical trial. This strong response observed among patients who apparently lack the cognitive abilities to “know” they are receiving a medication highlights the importance of nonbiological factors in the etiology of these behavioral symptoms. Consequently, this chapter focuses on an etiology based on environmental and social causes of the behavior, and the interaction between the physical and social environment with the individual’s needs and capabilities as the explanation for the onset of behavioral problems in dementia (Cohen-Mansfield 2000b). In the past, it was not believed that persons with dementia retained higher-level needs or functions. We have since learned, however, that although persons with dementia differ from cognitively intact persons in their ability to articulate and independently meet higher-level needs, these needs are present nonetheless (Cohen-Mansfield and Werner 1995). Evidence shows that a large proportion of dementia-related behavior problems stem from an incongruence between the needs of the person who suffers from dementia and the degree to which his or her environment fulfills these needs (Barton, Findlay, and Blake 2005; Cohen-Mansfield and Werner 1995; Palmer et al. 1999). In fact, it is our experience as well as that of other researchers that many behavior problems constitute a response to physical pain or discomfort (Cohen-Mansfield et al. 1990; Douzjian, Wilson, and Shultz 1998), feelings of loneliness or isolation (Cohen-Mansfield and Werner 1997), boredom (Buettner and Kolanowski 2003; Ice 2002), or sensory deprivation (Cohen-Mansfield 2000b). Thus, many “problematic behaviors” may represent a cry for help, a result of unmet needs, or an inadequate attempt to fulfill those needs. For example, Hancock, Woods, Challis and Orrell (2006) found that sensory or physical disability (including mobility problems and incontinence) needs, mental health needs, and social needs of persons with dementia in residential care were often unmet and were associated with psychological problems such as anxiety and depression. Due to such findings, it is critical that the evaluation and care of unmet needs become the guiding principles of good, domain-specific patient care. Nonpharmacologic interventions aim to address what we have learned to be the most important etiologic basis of behavioral problems in dementia. Similar to the notion of “person-centered care” (Touhy 2004), this approach can be better described as “informed care,” a treatment approach that is based on knowledge of the needs of persons with dementia in general and the individual in particular. Care can be enhanced by an approach
4
Dementia
of rehabilitation and restorative care, yet the main focus is that of improving overall well-being and addressing the needs of the older individual with dementia, even when those needs are not obvious or articulated. Admittedly, the implementation of this type of care is more complex than prescribing a psychoactive medication, and the identification of the needs to be fulfilled is more difficult than articulating the specific psychiatric syndrome from which the patient may be suffering. However, these interventions avoid the potentially harmful side effects that result from pharmacologic treatments (Ballard et al. 2009; Folks 2003; Gill et al. 2009). Also, reducing inappropriate behaviors via sedation with psychoactive medication can potentially rob the person with dementia of the very limited resources he or she has in either expressing or attending to his or her needs (Cohen-Mansfield 2000a; Fisher and Swingen 1997), thereby diminishing the ability of caretakers to detect and address the true underlying need. Most important, nonpharmacologic interventions aim to improve the quality of life of the person with dementia. In summary, the importance of utilizing a nonpharmacologic approach for inappropriate behaviors associated with dementia is threefold: (a) it aims to address the psychosocial/environmental underlying reasons for the behavior, which have been documented in prior research, thus increasing quality of life; (b) it avoids the limitations of pharmacologic interventions, namely adverse side effects, drug-drug interactions, and limited efficacy (Cohen-Mansfield et al. 1999); and (c) when medication is efficacious, it may mask the actual need and reduce the already compromised communication by the older person, thereby limiting the caregiver ’s ability to properly care for that person. TYPES OF NONPHARMACOLOGIC INTERVENTIONS There are many reviews of nonpharmacologic interventions in dementia (e.g., Ayalon et al. 2006; Cohen-Mansfield 2001, 2005; Barton, Findlay, and Blake 2005; Hermans, Htay, and McShane 2007; Kasl-Godley and Gatz 2000; Kong, Evans, and Guevara 2009; Kverno et al. 2009; Landreville et al. 2006; Livingston et al. 2005; Logsdon, McCurry, and Teri 2007; Robinson et al. 2007; O’Connor et al. 2009; Spira and Edelstein 2006). All of the aforementioned reviews report that there is insufficient high-quality research and that additional studies are needed, although each review found evidence for the efficacy of various interventions. For example, Ayalon and colleagues (2006) conclude that “interventions that address behavioral issues and unmet needs and that include caregivers or bright light therapy may be efficacious.” Kong, Evans, and Guevara (2009) found
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
5
that “only sensory interventions had efficacy in reducing agitation.” Landreville et al. (2006) state that “staff training programs and environmental modifications appear to be the most effective strategies” to treat aggressive behaviors, whereas Livingston et al. (2005) assert that “only behavior management therapies, specific types of caregiver and residential care staff education, and possibly cognitive stimulation appear to have lasting effectiveness for the management of dementia-associated neuropsychiatric symptoms.” Logsdson, McCurry, and Teri (2007) found that only “behavioral problem-solving therapies that identify and modify antecedents and consequences of problem behaviors and increase pleasant events and individualized interventions based on progressively lowered stress threshold models that include problem solving and environmental modifications meet Evidence Based Treatment criteria.” O’Connor et al. (2009) report that “treatments with moderate or large effect sizes included aromatherapy, ability-focused carer education, bed baths, preferred music and muscle relaxation training.” These past reviews came to divergent conclusions regarding the efficacy of various interventions, although this may partially be explained by the implementation of differing inclusion criteria. Given the range of conclusions of past reviews, the review in this chapter takes a different approach. Our goal is to demonstrate the evidence for the types of interventions that have shown promise in this field rather than use strict evidence-based criteria that may or may not be applicable for the population, settings, and interventions that fit persons with dementia and behavior problems. Based on an understanding of nonpharmacologic interventions providing for unmet needs, we do not require interventions to last beyond the time in which they are provided as a criterion for their utility. We discuss nonpharmacologic interventions that have been tested in the following categories: sensory enhancement/relaxation methods, cognitive interventions, social contact (real or simulated), environmental modifications, structured activities, behavioral therapy, staff training, medical/ nursing interventions, and individualized interventions, which normally use interventions from the other categories as they apply to the specific individual. Sensory Interventions Sensory interventions have shown significant efficacy in reducing inappropriate behaviors (Kong, Evans, and Guevara 2009). These interventions include massage/touch therapy (Kim and Buschmann 1999; Hicks-Moore and Robinson 2008; Remington 2002; Rowe and Alfred 1999), music
6
Dementia
(Gerdner and Swanson 1993; Hicks-Moore and Robinson 2008; Remington 2002), aromatherapy (Ballard et al. 2002), and snoezelen (Holtkamp et al. 1997; van Weert et al. 2005), though a Cochrane review by Chung and Lai (2002) found no effects for the latter intervention. Cognitive Interventions Documented cognitive interventions include cognitive stimulation (Breuil et al. 1994; Spector, Woods, and Orrell 2008) and reality orientation (Spector et al. 2000, 2001). Reality orientation was developed as a strategy to ameliorate cognitive decline by stimulating the confused individual with repetitive orienting information being presented on an individual or group level (Scanland and Emershaw 1993). While a meta-analysis of reality orientation studies found this intervention to be successful in improving environmental orientation, cognition, and behavioral problems (Spector et al. 2000, 2007), several studies revealed no significant improvements in mental status, social behavior, or activities of daily living (Hogstel 1979; Holden and Sinebruchow 1978; Scanland and Emershaw 1993). Cognitive stimulation therapy (CST) is a more advanced form of reality orientation that consists of small-group activity sessions based on person-centered principles. One of the goals of CST is the promotion of well-being and confidence in participants by increasing their awareness and involvement in the sessions (Knowles 2010). Although CST requires financial resources, studies show that the benefits gained may be worth the cost, as both Breuil et al. (1994) and Spector et al. (2003) found that CST participants had significantly improved cognitive function as compared to the control group. Social Interventions Several studies have found real or simulated social interaction and stimuli with social features to be effective in reducing agitation and more engaging than other types of stimuli (Cohen-Mansfield, Marx, Dakheel-Ali, et al. 2010). Although one-on-one social interaction is consistently the most beneficial intervention (Cohen-Mansfield and Werner 1997; Cohen-Mansfield, Thein, et al. 2010), nursing home staff and family are not always available to provide individual attention. Consequently, researchers and practitioners have examined alternative forms of social contact (Cohen-Mansfield 2005). Documented social interventions that have been shown to benefit persons with dementia and behavior problems include one-on-one social interaction, live pet and robotic pet therapy (Banks, Willoughby, and Banks 2008;
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
7
Libin and Cohen-Mansfield 2004), baby dolls (Cohen-Mansfield, Thein, et al. 2010), and respite videos or simulated presence therapy (Camberg et al. 1999; Hall and Hare 1997; Lund et al. 1995; Peak and Cheston 2002). Environmental Modifications Research has shown that persons with dementia may benefit from specific environmental modifications, which generally included either ambiance-changing interventions, provision of space that accommodates behaviors, such as indoor paths or outdoor gardens, or interventions affecting specific environmental attributes, such as signage, for enhancing orientation or camouflaging doors to decrease exit seeking. Ambiancechanging interventions that have been linked with decreased agitation include enhancing the nursing home environment (e.g., by simulating a home or outdoor environment) (Cohen-Mansfield and Werner 1998), administration of preferred music during bathing (Clark, Lipe, and Bilbrey 1998), and privacy and personalization in bedrooms (Zeisel et al. 2003). Similarly, changing specific attributes has been reported to be beneficial, including camouflaging exit doors (Kincaid and Peacock 2003; Zeisel et al. 2003), and increasing toilet visibility (Namazi and Johnson 1991). Although several studies have found that bright-light therapy can decrease agitation (Fetveit and Bjorvatn 2005; Lovell, Ancoli-Israel, and Gevirtz 1995), a recent study found that ambient bright light was not effective in reducing agitation in dementia and may exacerbate this behavioral symptom (Barrick et al. 2010). A successful accommodating intervention for wandering in persons with dementia is exposure to an outdoor garden (Cohen-Mansfield and Werner 1999). Cohen-Mansfield and Werner (1998) found that spending time in an outdoor garden significantly decreased trespassing and improved mood. Hernandez (2007) concluded that therapeutic gardens should be a standard feature of nursing facilities. Structured Activities and Stimulation There are a wide variety of structured activities that can be used as interventions for inappropriate behaviors in dementia. Some examples of structured activities that have been found to decrease agitation are: a threeweek supervised exercise program (Aman and Thomas 2009), recreational interventions, and manipulatives, sorting, sewing, music, nurturing, and tactile stimulation (Aronstein, Olsen, and Schulman 1996), Montessoribased-activities groups (Lin et al. 2009), and art activities (Sterritt and Pokorny 1994). One review found that there is insufficient evidence to be
8
Dementia
able to say whether or not physical activity programs are beneficial for people with dementia (Forbes et al. 2008), although Aman and Thomas (2009) saw a significant improvement in agitation scores and six-meter walk times of cognitively impaired participants after their engagement in a three-week exercise program, and a meta-analysis found significant improvements for cognitive performance and behavior (among other outcome variables) after exercise training for persons with dementia (Heyn, Abreu, and Ottenbacher 2004). Although there is a wide variety of stimuli to choose from, research has shown that any stimulus is preferable to none (Cohen-Mansfeild, Marx, et al. 2010). This is an important message for nursing facilities, as residents often spend long periods of time unoccupied (Lodgsdon 2000). Behavioral Interventions Behavioral interventions have been successful in decreasing inappropriate behaviors (Teri et al. 1997). A review of behavioral interventions for agitation in older adults with dementia found mixed results, but concluded that behavioral management of agitation shows promise (Spira and Edelstein 2006). Some of the behavior therapies examined include differential reinforcement (Heard and Watson 1999) and stimulus control (Bird, Alexopoulos, and Adamowicz 1995). Staff Training and Mentoring Interventions involving staff training are important for decreasing agitation, with research reporting some important successes and some failures. A pilot study found Dementia Care Mapping, a method designed to support caregivers in providing person-centered care, reduced verbal agitation (e.g., excessively asking for attention, complaining, and negativism) in participants with dementia (Kuiper et al. 2009). A large, randomized controlled trial comparing Dementia Care Mapping, person-centered care, and usual care with 289 persons with dementia in 15 residential facilities found that mapping and person-centered care significantly decreased agitation as measured by the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (Chenoweth et al. 2009). A two-day staff education program followed by continued monthly guidance was able both to improve quality of care by reducing the frequency of restraints and antipsychotic drugs and to reduce severity of agitation (Testad et al. 2010). A controlled study of an eight-week staff education and training program resulted in a significant decrease in agitation in the intervention group but not in the control group (Deudon et al. 2009).
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
9
Medical/Nursing Interventions Medical and nursing interventions include controlling pain (Douzjian, Wilson, and Schultz 1998), removing physical restraints (Evans, Wood, and Lambert 2002), and providing or fixing dysfunctional hearing aids (Gray 2004). Utilization of a combination of interventions has also been successful, such as in the AGE program, which included Activities, Guidelines for psychotropic medications, and Education rounds (Rovner et al. 1996), as well as in an interdisciplinary program involving consciousness-raising activities, educational sessions, and clinical follow-up (Monette et al. 2007). TAILORING NONPHARMACOLOGIC INTERVENTIONS TO THE INDIVIDUAL Dementia has a unique progression in each individual, and caregivers or nursing staff will therefore encounter various types and levels of symptom severity, against the background of different personal interests, and cognitive and physical functioning. Consequently, it is prudent to tailor nonpharmacologic interventions to each individual being treated. The concept of individualizing interventions based on past and present preferences has been supported by several studies. Gerdner (2000) found that music based on a person’s past preferences had a greater beneficial impact on behavior than nonindividualized music. Cohen-Mansfield, Marx, Thein, and Dakheel-Ali (2010) found that nursing home residents with dementia with current interests in music, art, and pets were more engaged by stimuli that reflected these interests than residents without these interests. From a theoretical point of view, interest in stimuli represents the interaction between person and stimulus beyond the impact of general person variables, such as gender or level of cognitive function. Personal characteristics and the specific nature of the behavior also impact the need to individualize the intervention. This is obvious in terms of cognitive level and sensory deficits, as those dictate which interventions can be processed. Different types of verbally disruptive behaviors (e.g., hallucinations vs. requests for attention) seemed to respond differentially to different interventions (e.g., family videotape vs. one-on-one social interaction; Cohen-Mansfield et al. 1997). Personal characteristics, such as demographics, cognitive functioning, medical status, and functional status (including hearing and vision), have been found to impact engagement in persons with dementia (Cohen-Mansfield et al. 2009). For example, female gender, higher cognitive functioning and better ADL functioning have been
10
Dementia
linked with longer engagement with stimuli (Cohen-Mansfield et al. 2009) and longer participation in activities (Dobbs et al. 2005; Resnick, Fries, and Verbrugge 1997; Voelkl, Fries, and Galecki 1995), while participants with hearing impairment were more likely to refuse stimuli (Cohen-Mansfield et al. 2009). Decreased social interactions have been linked with pain (Chibnall et al. 2005) and visual and hearing impairment (Resnick, Fries, and Verbrugge 1997), possibly because pain could cause social withdrawal and sensory impairment could make social interaction more challenging. One issue in terms of the feasibility of tailoring interventions is that the tools to do so are still being developed. However, an assessment that has proven useful is the Self-Identity Questionnaire (SIQ; Cohen-Mansfield, Golander, and Arnheim 2000). The SIQ explores the following four selfidentity role domains: professional, family/social, hobbies/leisure time activities, and personal attributes/traits/achievements. Cohen-Mansfield, Parpura-Gill, and Golander (2006) examined the impact of incorporating self-identity into treatment of persons with dementia. Participants assigned to the treatment group were engaged in activities corresponding to their most salient role identity and showed a significant increase in pleasure, interest, and involvement in activities, fewer agitated behaviors during treatment, and increased orientation to the treatment period. Consequently, administering the SIQ may be a useful component of the process of tailoring interventions to the individual. The delineation of the parameters to be addressed when tailoring stimuli may be assisted by a model of examining the heterogeneity in dementia (Cohen-Mansfield 2001). This model describes interpersonal differences as stemming from variation in the domains of biological/ medical, psychosocial, and environmental, each of which is examined across the time-points of initial predisposition, lifelong influences, and current conditions. This framework may be useful in classifying the issues that need to be taken into account when individualizing treatment. Cognitive and sensory deficits, mobility, social abilities, environmental factors, and the specific nature of the behavior need to be taken into account when tailoring nonpharmacologic interventions for inappropriate behaviors in dementia. NONPHARMACOLOGIC INTERVENTIONS AND AFFECT The impact of nonpharmacologic interventions on the affect of persons with dementia has also been reported (Cohen-Mansfield, Libin, and Marx 2007; Moore, Delaney, and Dixon 2007; Orsulic-Jeras, Schneider, and Camp 2007; Williams and Tappen 2007). Moore, Delaney, and Dixon (2007)
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
11
presented a variety of activities to promote and elicit engagement in three nursing home residents and found that the residents’ levels of happiness increased with each activity presented, when comparing pre- and postactivity affect. Some researchers have examined the impact of individualized interventions on affect. Kolanowski, Litaker, and Buettner (2005) reported more positive affect after implementing activities that were tailored to meet individual needs. In a study of 167 nursing home residents with dementia, Cohen-Mansfield and colleagues (2007) created individualized interventions that matched each person’s unmet need and physical and cognitive abilities, as well as lifelong interests, hobbies and past roles, and found that these interventions significantly increased pleasure and interest and decreased agitation. Other interventions aimed at improving depressed affect include providing opportunities for persons with dementia to exercise control, such as making decisions about meals or caring for a plant (Langer and Rodin 1976), individualizing activities based on knowledge of what experiences are reinforcing for persons with dementia (Teri and Logsdon 1991; Teri et al. 1997), and self-affirming interventions. BE-ACTIV, a behavioral activities-based intervention for depression that can be implemented in nursing homes collaboratively with nursing home activities staff (Meeks et al. 2006), was successful in improving depressed affect. Reminiscence therapy is a self-affirming intervention that encourages persons with dementia to talk about their past and may utilize audiovisual aids such as old family photos and objects. Reminiscence can enhance individuals’ sense of identity, sense of worth, or general well-being (Brooker and Duce 2000), and may also stimulate memory processes. Although a review found that this therapy significantly improved mood and cognition, there is a need for more rigorous and high-quality studies (Woods et al. 2005). In validation therapy, a therapist accepts the disorientation of a person with dementia and validates his or her feelings (Feil 1982). Loneliness is highly correlated with depressed affect, and social interaction (real or simulated) and contact interventions are therefore also appropriate for improving affect (Cohen-Mansfield et al. in press). BARRIERS TO THE IMPLEMENTATION OF NONPHARMACOLOGIC INTERVENTIONS IN DEMENTIA The actual utilization of nonpharmacologic interventions in dementia falls far short of its potential, and a number of systemic issues are responsible for this discrepancy. There is currently a dearth of funding for the practice of such interventions as well as for the attainment of knowledge
12
Dementia
about them through systematic research. The provision of pharmacologic interventions is reimbursed, and the underlying structure for the delivery of psychoactive medications is largely in place in terms of the roles of physicians, medicine aids, filling prescriptions at pharmacies, monitoring, and quality control systems. However, the purchase of nonpharmacologic interventions is generally not reimbursed, and the methodology for providing them is a work in progress. No one in the care system is currently responsible for assessing, observing, and analyzing inappropriate behavior or psychotic symptoms in order to determine their etiology and their impact on individuals’ lives. A study of depression by Meeks and colleagues (2006) found that barriers to the implementation of nonpharmacologic interventions include ongoing medical stressors and poor staff follow-through. Indeed, regardless of the success of a nonpharmacologic treatment plan, without staff or caregiver diligence in following that plan, individuals will likely lose any improvements over time. The same research team found that during BE-ACTIV, a promising activitiesbased intervention for depression that is implemented in nursing homes collaboratively with activities staff, the behavioral problem-solving and weekly communication between the resident’s mental health consultant and activities staff helped remove barriers to pleasant events. Some interventions are inherently more difficult to put in place than others, such as those of a technological nature. Freedman, Calkins, and Van Haitsma (2005) found that technology can potentially be effective in increasing efficiencies and enhancing the quality of care and quality of life for older people living in nursing homes, assisted living, and continuing care retirement communities. However, this study identified the following significant barriers to such interventions: lack of information about cost-effectiveness of technologies; lack of information about other aspects of technologies; limited resources for providers to purchase technologies; concerns about liability and associated costs; lack of reimbursement for technologies in these settings; limited resources for manufacturers to develop useful technologies; lack of standards to facilitate integration; discouragement of innovation by the regulatory environment; staffing-related challenges, and the challenges of managing the process of change. If family and caregivers are willing and motivated to maintain a nonpharmacologic treatment plan, it is possible to surmount many barriers to treatment. There are several prerequisites to effective nonpharmacologic care of persons with dementia. In order to provide nonpharmacologic interventions, the system of care must promote an atmosphere and practice of caring that goes beyond what is currently found in most care settings. A practice style that includes good communication skills, compassion,
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
13
and empathy by caregivers, as well as a high level of flexibility of direct care staff and the larger organization are needed and are often lacking (Cohen-Mansfield and Parpura-Gill 2007). In order to allow for alternative interventions, the system of care must promote autonomy and respect for the person with dementia and maximize flexibility in all procedures. Greater monetary resources must also be allocated for research to develop the knowledge necessary for optimizing such care. As such knowledge is gained, concomitant changes in reimbursement and the structure of system of care need to take place in order to impact the practice of dementia care. There is an urgent need to improve our ability to answer basic questions: Which interventions are efficacious for which individuals? Which aspects of an intervention are necessary for it to be efficacious? What are the active ingredients, or principles at work, in different interventions? Which personal characteristics (gender, culture, prior stress) should be considered in matching an intervention to an individual? What is the impact of the person delivering the intervention and the manner in which it is delivered? Only once these basic questions are answered can the issues of effectiveness and costs be properly addressed. Although the process of developing a nonpharmacologic treatment plan is initially costly in terms of time and money, these costs will be decreased when approached as a system change. Similarly, enhancing flexibility and communication training for any one resident is expensive, but becomes much cheaper when implemented as a new philosophy of care throughout the institution. If such nonpharmacologic interventions were reimbursed in the manner of pharmacologic ones, it is likely that most costs would be offset by the decreased use of psychotropic drugs and related adverse events. Such changes in care philosophy and practice are likely to lead to more humane care and improved quality of life for persons with dementia and their caregivers. In order to increase the use of nonpharmacologic interventions in dementia care, there is a need for public education and advocacy concerning the importance of such interventions and their support. Nonpharmacologic interventions generally provide more personalized care for persons with dementia, addressing their needs and thereby preventing or treating inappropriate behaviors or declines in function and maximizing quality of life.
REFERENCES Aman, E., and D. R. Thomas. 2009. Supervised exercise to reduce agitation in severely cognitively impaired persons. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 10 (4): 271–276.
14
Dementia
American Geriatrics Society. 2009. A guide to dementia diagnosis and treatment. http://www.americangeriatrics.org/dementia/Dementia_Diagnosis2 .asp#treat (accessed April 14, 2010). American Medical Directors Association. 2009. Clinical practice guideline: Dementia. http://www.amda.com/tools/cpg/dementia.cfm (accessed April 14, 2010). Aronstein, Z., R. Olsen, and E. Schulman. 1996. The nursing assistant’s use of recreational interventions for behavioral management of residents with Alzheimer ’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias 11: 26–31. Ayalon, L., A. M. Gum, L. Feliciano, and P. A. Arean. 2006. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological interventions for the management of neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with dementia. Archives of Internal Medicine 166: 2182–2188. Ballard, C., M. L. Hanney, M. Theodoulou, S. Douglas, R. McShane, K. Kossakowski, et al. 2009. The dementia antipsychotic withdrawal trial (DART-AD): Longterm follow-up of a randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurology 8 (2): 151–157. Ballard, C. G., J. T. O’Brien, K. Reichelt, and E. K. Perry. 2002. Aromatherapy as a safe and effective treatment for the management of agitation in severe dementia: The results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial with Melissa. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63 (7): 553–558. Banks, M. R., L. M. Willoughby, and W. A. Banks. 2008. Animal-assisted therapy and loneliness in nursing homes: Use of robotic versus living dogs. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 9(3): 173–177. Barrick, A. L., P. D. Sloane, C. S. Williams, C. M. Mitchell, B. R. Connell, W. Wood, et al. 2010. Impact of ambient bright light on agitation in dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 25 (10): 1013–1021. Barton, S., D. Findlay, and R. A. Blake. 2005. The management of inappropriate vocalization in dementia: A hierarchical approach. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20 (12): 1180–1186. Bird, M., P. Alexopoulos, and J. Adamowicz. 1995. Success and failure in five case studies: Use of cued recall to ameliorate behavior problems in senile dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 6: 607–609. Breuil, V., J. De Rotrou, F. Forette, D. Tortrat, A. Ganansia-Ganem, A. Frambourt, et al. 1994. Cognitive stimulation of patients with dementia: Preliminary results. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 9 (3): 211–217. Brooker, D., and L. Duce. 2000. Wellbeing and activity in dementia: A comparison of group reminiscence therapy, structured goal-directed group activity and unstructured time. Aging and Mental Health 4 (4): 354–358. Buettner, L., and A. Kolanowski. 2003. Practice guidelines for recreation therapy in the care of people with dementia. Geriatric Nursing 24 (1): 18–25. Burke, J. R, and J. C. Morgenlander. 1999. Managing common behavioral problems in dementia. How to improve quality of life for patients and families. Postgraduate Medicine 106: 131–134.
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
15
Camberg, L., P. Woods, W. L. Ooi, A. Hurley, L. Volicer, J. Ashley, et al. 1999. Evaluation of simulated presence: A personalized approach to enhance wellbeing in persons with Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 47 (4): 446–452. Chenoweth, L., M. T. King, Y. Jeon, H. Brodaty, J. Stein-Parbury, R. Norman, et al. 2009. Caring for Aged Dementia Care Resident Study (CADRES) of person-centered care, dementia-care mapping, and usual care in dementia: A cluster-randomised trial. Lancet Neurology 8 (4): 317–325. Chibnall, J. T., R. C. Tait, B. Harman, and R. A. Luebbert. 2005. Effect of acetaminophen on behavior, well-being, and psychotropic medication use in nursing home residents with moderate-to-severe dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53 (11): 1921–1929. Chung, J. C. C., and C. K. Y. Lai. 2002. No evidence of the efficacy of snoezelen or multi-sensory stimulation programmes for people with dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 4. Art. No.: CD003152. DOI: 10.1002/14651858. CD003152. Clark, M. E., A. W. Lipe, and M. Bilbrey. 1998. Use of music to decrease aggressive behaviors in people with dementia. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 24 (7): 10–17. Cohen, C. A., D. P. Gold, K. I. Shulman, J. T. Wortley, G. McDonald, and M. Wargon.1993. Factors determining the decision to institutionalize dementing individuals: A prospective study. The Gerontologist 33 (6): 714–720. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2000a. Nonpharmacological management of behavioral problems in persons with dementia: The TREA model. Alzheimer ’s Care Quarterly 1 (4): 22–34. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2000b. Theoretical frameworks for behavioral problems in dementia. Alzheimer ’s Care Quarterly 1 (4): 8–21. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2001. Nonpharmacological interventions for inappropriate behaviors in dementia: A review, summary, and critique. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 9 (4): 361–381. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2005. Nonpharmacological interventions for persons with dementia. Alzheimer ’s Care Quarterly 6 (2): 129–145. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2008. Agitated behavior in persons with dementia: The relationship between type of behavior, its frequency, and its disruptiveness. Journal of Psychiatric Research 43 (1): 64–69. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and N. Billig. 1986. Agitated behaviors in the elderly: A conceptual review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 34 (10): 711–721. Cohen-Mansfield, J., N. Billig, S. Lipson, A. S. Rosenthal, and L. G. Pawlson. 1990. Medical correlates of agitation in nursing home residents. Gerontology 36 (3): 150–158. Cohen-Mansfield, J., H. Golander, and G. Arnheim. 2000. Self-identity in older persons suffering from dementia: Preliminary results. Social Science and Medicine 51: 381–394. Cohen-Mansfield, J., A. Libin, and M. S. Marx. 2007. Nonpharmacological treatment of agitation: A controlled trial of systematic individualized intervention.
16
Dementia
Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 62A (8): 908–916. Cohen-Mansfield, J., S. Lipson, P. Werner, N. Billig, L. Taylor, and R. Woosley. 1999. Withdrawal of haloperidol, thioridazine and lorazepam in the nursing home. Archives of Internal Medicine 159: 1733–1740. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, M. Dakheel-Ali, N. G. Regier, K. Thein, and L. Freedman. 2010. Can agitated behavior of nursing home residents be prevented with the use of standardized stimuli? Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 58 (8): 1459–1464. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, N. G. Regier, and M. Dakheel-Ali. 2009. The impact of personal characteristics on engagement in nursing home residents with dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24 (7): 755–763. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, K. Thein, and M. Dakheel-Ali. 2010. The impact of past and present preferences on stimulus engagement in nursing home residents with dementia. Aging and Mental Health 14 (1): 67–73. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, K. Thein, and M. Dakheel-Ali. In press. The impact of stimuli on affect in persons with dementia. Journal of Psychiatric Research. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and A. Parpura-Gill. 2007. Practice style in the nursing home: Dimensions for assessment and quality improvement. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23 (4): 376–386. Cohen-Mansfield, J., A. Parpura-Gill, and H. Golander. 2006. Utilization of selfidentity roles for designing interventions for persons with dementia. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 61B (4): 202–212. Cohen-Mansfield, J., K. Thein, M. Dakheel-Ali, N. G. Regier, and M. S. Marx. 2010. The value of social attributes of stimuli for promoting engagement in persons with dementia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 198 (8): 586–592. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and P. Werner. 1995. Environmental influences on agitation: An integrative summary of an observational study. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Care and Related Disorders and Research 10 (1): 32–37. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and P. Werner. 1997. Management of verbally disruptive behaviors in the nursing home. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 52A (6): M369–M377. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and P. Werner. 1998. Visits to an outdoor garden: Impact on behavior and mood of nursing home residents who pace. In Research and practice in Alzheimer ’s disease intervention in gerontology, ed. B. J. Vellas, J. Fitten, and G. Frisconi, 419–436. Paris: Serdi Publishing. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and P. Werner. 1999. Outdoor wandering parks for persons suffering from dementia. A survey of characterization and use. Alzheimer ’s Disease and Associated Disorders: An International Journal 13 (2): 109–117. Cohen-Mansfield, J., P. Werner, W. J. Culpepper, and D. Barkley. 1997. Evaluation of an in-service training program on dementia and wandering. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 23: 40–47.
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
17
Cohen-Mansfield, J., P. Werner, V. Watson, and S. Pasis. 1995. Agitation among elderly persons at adult day-care centers: The experiences of relatives and staff members. International Psychogeriatrics 7 (3): 447–458. Craig, D., D. J. Hart, R. Carson, S. P. McIlroy, and A. P. Passmore. 2004. Allelic variation at the A218C tryptophan hydroxylase polymorphism influences agitation and aggression in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neuroscience Letters 363 (3): 199–202. Deudon, A., N. Maubourguet, X. Gervais, E. Leone, P. Brocker, L. Carcaillon, S. Riff, B. Lavallart, and P. H. Robert. 2009. Non-pharmacological management of behavioural symptoms in nursing homes. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24 (12): 1386–1395. Dobbs, D., J. Munn, S. Zimmerman, M. Boustani, C. S. Williams, P. D. Sloane, et al. 2005. Characteristics associated with lower activity involvement in longterm care residents with dementia. The Gerontologist 45 (1): 81–86. Douzjian, M., C. Wilson, and M. Shultz. 1998. A program to use pain control medication to reduce psychotropic drug use in residents with difficult behavior. Annals of Long-Term Care 6: 174–179. Evans, D., J. Wood, and L. Lambert. 2002. A review of physical restraint minimization in the acute and residential care settings. Journal of Advanced Nursing 40 (6): 616–625. Feil, N. 1982. Validation: The Feil method. Cleveland, Ohio: Feil Productions. Fetveit, A., and B. Bjorvatn. 2005. Bright light treatment reduces actigraphicmeasured daytime sleep in nursing home patients with dementia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 13: 420–423. Finkel, S. I., and A. Burns. 2000. Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD): A clinical and research update. International Psychogeriatrics 12 (Suppl. 1): 9–12. Fisher, J., and D. Swingen. 1997. Contextual factors in the assessment and management of aggression in dementia patients. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 4: 171–190. Folks, D. G. 2003. Antipsychotic agents. In Agitation in patients with dementia: A practical guide to diagnosis and management, ed. D. P. Hay, D. T. Klein, L. K. Hay, G. T. Grossberg, and J. S. Kennedy, pp. 167–185. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. Forbes, D., S. Forbes, D. G. Morgan, M. Markle-Reid, J. Wood, and I. Culum. 2008. Physical activity programs for persons with dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 3. Art. No.: CD006489. DOI: 10.1002/14651858. CD006489.pub2. Freedman, V. A., M. Calkins, and K. Van Haitsma. 2005. An exploratory study of barriers to implementing technology in residential long-term care settings. Gerotechnology 4 (2): 86–100. Gerdner, L. A. 2000. Effects of individualized vs, classical “relaxation” music on the frequency of agitation in elderly persons with Alzheimer ’s disease and related disorders. International Psychogeriatrics 12: 49–65.
18
Dementia
Gerdner, L. A., and E. A. Swanson. 1993. Effects of individualized music on confused and agitated elderly patients. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 7: 284–291. Gill, S. S, G. M. Anderson, H. D. Fischer, C. M. Bell, P. Li, S. T. Normand, and P. A. Rochon. 2009. Syncope and its consequences in patients with dementia receiving cholinesterase inhibitors: A population-based cohort study. Archives of Internal Medicine 169 (9): 867–873. Gray, K. F. 2004. Managing agitation and difficult behavior in dementia. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine 20 (1): 69–82. Hall, L., and J. Hare. 1997. Video respite™ for cognitively impaired persons in nursing homes. American Journal of Alzheimers Disease and Other Dementias 12 (3): 117–121. Hancock, G. A., B. Woods, D. Challis, and M. Orrell. 2006. The needs of older people with dementia in residential care. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21 (1): 43–49. Heard, K., and T. S. Watson. 1999. Reducing wandering by persons with dementia using differential reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 32: 381–384. Hebert, R., M. F. Dubois, C. Wolfson, L. Chambers, and C. Cohen. 2001. Factors associated with long-term institutionalization of older people with dementia: Data from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 56A: M693–M699. Hermans, D. G., U. H. Htay, and R. McShane. 2007. Non-pharmacological interventions for wandering of people with dementia in the domestic setting. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (Online) 1, CD005994. Hernandez, R. O. 2007. Effects of therapeutic gardens in Special Care Units for people with dementia. Journal of Housing for the Elderly 21 (1–2): 117–152. Heyn, P., B. Abreu, and K. Ottenbacher. 2004. The effects of exercise training on elderly persons with cognitive impairment and dementia. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 85 (10): 1694–1704. Hicks-Moore, S. L., and B. A. Robinson. 2008. Favorite music and hand massage. Dementia 7 (1): 95–108. Hogstel, M. O. 1979. Use of reality orientation with aging confused patients. Nursing Research 28 (3): 161–165. Holden, U. P., and A. Sinebruchow. 1978. Reality orientation therapy: A study investigating the value of this therapy in the rehabilitation of elderly people. Age and Ageing 7: 83–90. Holmes, C. 2000. Contribution of genetics to the understanding of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. International Psychogeriatrics 12: 83–88. Holtkamp, C. C., K. Kragt, M. C. van Dongen, E. van Rossum, and C. Salentijn. 1997. Effect of snoezelen on the behavior of the demented elderly. Tijdschrift voor Gerontologie en Geriatrie 28 (3): 124–128.
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
19
Ice, G. H. 2002. Daily life in a nursing home: Has it changed in 25 years? Journal of Aging Studies 16 (4): 345–359. Kasl-Godley, J., and M. Gatz. 2000. Psychosocial interventions for individuals with dementia: An integration of theory, therapy, and a clinical understanding of dementia. Clinical Psychology Review 20 (6): 755–782. Katz, I. R., D. V. Jeste, J. E. Mintzer, C. Clyde, J. Napolitano, and M. Brecher. 1999. Comparison of risperidone and placebo for psychosis and behavioral disturbances associated with dementia: A randomized, double blind trial. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 60: 107–115. Kim, E. J., and M. T. Buschmann. 1999. The effect of expressive physical touch on patients with dementia. International Journal of Nursing Studies 36 (3): 235–243. Kincaid, C., and J. R. Peacock. 2003. The effect of a wall mural of decreasing four types of door-testing behaviors. Journal of Applied Gerontology 22 (1): 76–88. Knowles, J. 2010. Cognitive stimulation therapy: Why it deserves better awareness and availability. Journal of Care Services Management 4 (2): 188–194. Kolanowski, A. M., M. Litaker, and L. Buettner. 2005. Efficacy of theory-based activities for behavioral symptoms of dementia. Nursing Research 54 (4): 219–228. Kong, E. H., L. K. Evans, and J. P. Guevara. 2009. Nonpharmacological intervention for agitation in dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aging and Mental Health 13 (4): 512–520. Kuiper, D., G. J. Dijkstra, J. Tuinstra, and J. W. Groothoff. 2009. The influence of Dementia Care Mapping (DCM) on behavioral problems of persons with dementia and the job satisfaction of caregivers: A pilot study. Tijdschrift voor Gerontologie en Geriatrie 40 (3): 102–111. Kverno, K. S., B. S. Black, M. T. Nolan, and P. V. Rabins. 2009. Research on treating neuropsychiatric symptoms of advanced dementia with non-pharmacological strategies, 1998–2008: A systematic literature review. International Psychogeriatrics/IPA 21 (5): 825–843. Landreville, P., A. Bedard, R. Verreault, J. Desrosiers, N. Champoux, J. Monette, et al. 2006. Non-pharmacological interventions for aggressive behavior in older adults living in long-term care facilities. International Psychogeriatrics 18 (1): 47–73. Langer, E. J., and Rodin, J. 1976. Effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an institutional setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 (2): 191–198. Libin, A., and J. Cohen-Mansfield. 2004. Therapeutic robocat for nursing home residents with dementia: A comparative study. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias 19 (2): 111–116. Lin, L. C., M. H. Yang, C. C. Kao, S. C. Wu, S. H. Tang, and J. G. Lin. 2009. Using acupressure and Montessori-based activities to decrease agitation for residents with dementia: A cross-over trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 57 (6): 1022–1029.
20
Dementia
Livingston, G., K. Johnston, C. Katona, and J. Paton. 2005. Systematic review of psychological approaches to the management of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry 162 (11): 1996–2021. Logsdon, R. G. 2000. Behavioral outcomes of dementia special care units: Results from four of the NIA collaborative studies. The Gerontologist 40 (Special Issue 1): 133. Logsdon, R. G., S. M. McCurry, and L. Teri. 2007. Evidence-based psychological treatments for disruptive behaviors in individuals with dementia. Psychology and Aging 22 (1): 28–36. Lovell, B. B., S. Ancoli-Israel, and R. Gevirtz. 1995. Effect of bright light treatment on agitated behavior in institutionalized elderly subjects. Psychiatry Research 57: 7–12. Lund, D. A., R. D. Hill, M. S. Caserta, and S. D. Wright. 1995. Video respiteTM: An innovative resource for family, professional caregivers, and persons with dementia. The Gerontologist 35 (5): 683–687. Lyketsos, C., C. C. Colenda, and C. Beck. 2006. Position statement of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry regarding principles of care for patients with dementia resulting from Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14 (7): 561–573. McIlroy, S., and D. Craig. 2004. Neurobiology and genetics of behavioral syndromes of Alzheimer ’s disease. Current Alzheimer Research 1 (2): 135–142. Meeks, S., L. Teri, K. Van Haitsma, and S. Looney. 2006. Increasing pleasant events in the nursing home: Collaborative behavioral treatment for depression. Clinical Case Studies 5 (4): 287–304. Monette, J., N. Champoux, M. Monette, L. Fournier, C. Wolfson, G. Galbaud du Fort, et al. 2007. Effects of an interdisciplinary educational program on antipsychotic prescribing among nursing home residents with dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23: 574–579. Moore, K., J. A. Delaney, and M. R. Dixon. 2007. Using indices of happiness to examine the influence of environmental enhancements for nursing home residents with Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 40 (3): 541–544. Namazi, K. H., and B. D. Johnson. 1991. Environmental effects on incontinence problems in Alzheimer ’s disease patients. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Care and Related Disorders and Research 6 (6): 16–21. O’Connor, D. W., D. Ames, B. Gardner, and M. King. 2009. Psychosocial treatments of behavior symptoms in dementia: A systematic review of reports meeting quality standards. International Psychogeriatrics 21 (2): 225–240. Orsulic-Jeras, S., N. M. Schneider, and C. J. Camp. 2007. Special feature: Montessori-based activities for long-term care residents with dementia. Topics in Geriatric Rehabilitation 16 (1): 78–91. Palmer, C. V., M. Adams, J. Bourgeois, J. Durrant, and M. Rossi. 1999. Reduction in caregiver-identified problem behavior in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease post hearing aid fitting. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 42 (2): 312–328.
Neuropsychiatry of Dementia
21
Peak, J. S., and R. I. L. Cheston. 2002. Using simulated presence therapy with people with dementia. Aging and Mental Health 6 (1): 77–81. Remington, R. 2002. Calming music and hand massage with agitated elderly. Nursing Research 51: 317–323. Resnick, H. E., B. E. Fries, and L. M. Verbrugge. 1997. Windows to their world: The effect of sensory impairments on social engagement and activity time in nursing home residents. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 52B (3): S135–S144. Robinson, L., D. Hutchings, H. O. Dickinson, L. Corner, F. Beyer, T. Finch, et al. 2007. Effectiveness and acceptability of non-pharmacological interventions to reduce wandering in dementia: A systematic review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22: 9–22. Rovner, B. W., C. D. Steele, Y. Shmuely, and M. F. Folstein. 1996. A randomized trial of dementia care in nursing homes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 44 (1): 7–13. Rowe, M., and D. Alfred. 1999. The effectiveness of slow-stroke massage in diffusing agitated behaviors in individuals with Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 25 (6): 22–34. Salzman, C., D. V. Jeste, R. E. Meyer, J. Cohen-Mansfield, J. Cummings, G. T. Grossberg, et al. 2008. Elderly patients with dementia-related symptoms of severe agitation and aggression: Consensus statement on treatment options, clinical trials methodology, and policy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69 (6): 889–898. Scanland, S. G., and L. E. Emershaw. 1993. Reality orientation and validation therapy: Dementia, depression, and functional status. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 19 (6): 7–11. Siegler, E. L., E. Capezuti, G. Maislin, M. Baumgarten, L. Evans, and N. Strumpf. 1997. Effects of a restraint reduction intervention and OBRA ‘87 regulations on psychoactive drug use in nursing homes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 45 (7): 791–796. Spector, A., M. Orrell, S. Davies, and B. Woods. 2000. Reality orientation for dementia: A systematic review of the evidence of effectiveness from randomised controlled trials. The Gerontologist 40: 206–212. Spector, A., M. Orrell, S. Davies, and B. Woods. 2001. Can reality orientation be rehabilitated? Development and piloting of an evidence-based programme of cognition-based therapies for people with dementia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 11 (3–4): 377–397. Spector, A., M. Orrell, S. Davies, and B. Woods. 2007. WITHDRAWN: Reality orientation for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 18 (3): CD001119. Spector, A., L. Thorgrimsen, B. Woods, L. Royan, S. Davies, M. Butterworth, et al. 2003. Efficacy of an evidence-based cognitive stimulation therapy programme for people with dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry 183: 248–254. Spector, A., B. Woods, and M. Orrell. 2008. Cognitive stimulation for the treatment of Alzheimer ’s disease. Expert Reviewof Neurotherapeutics 8 (5): 751–757.
22
Dementia
Spira, A. P., and B. A. Edelstein. 2006. Behavioral interventions for agitation in older adults with dementia: An evaluative review. International Psychogeriatrics 18 (2): 195–225. Sterritt, P. F., and M. E. Pokorny. 1994. Art activities for patients with Alzheimer ’s and related disorders: Working with art may help a client get back in touch with the outside world. Geriatric Nursing 15: 155–159. Teri, L., and R. G. Logsdon. 1991. Identifying pleasant activities for Alzheimer ’s disease patients: The pleasant events schedule-AD. The Gerontologist 31 (1): 124–127. Teri, L., R. G. Logsdon, E. Peskind, M. Raskind, M. F. Weiner, R. E. Tractenberg et al. 2000. Treatment of agitation in AD: A randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Neurology 55 (9): 1271–1278. Teri, L., R. G. Logsdon, J. Uomoto, and S. M. McCurry. 1997. Behavioral treatment of depression in dementia patients: A controlled clinical trial. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 52B: P159–P166. Testad, I., C. Ballard, K. Bronnick, and D. Aarsland. 2010. The effect of staff training on agitation and use of restraint in nursing home residents with dementia: A single-blind, randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 71 (1): 80–86. Touhy, T. A. 2004. Dementia, personhood, and nursing: Learning from a nursing situation. Nursing Science Quarterly 17: 43–49. van Weert, J. C. M., A. M. van Dulmen, P. M. M. Spreeuwenberg, M. W. Ribbe, and J. M. Bensing. 2005. Behavioral and mood effects of snoezelen integrated into 24-hour dementia care. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 24–33. Voelkl, J. E., B. E. Fries, and A. T. Galecki. 1995. Predictors of nursing home residents’ participation in activity programs. The Gerontologist 35 (1): 44–51. Williams, C. L., and R. M. Tappen. 2007. Effect of exercise on mood in nursing home residents with Alzheimer ’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimers Disease and Other Dementias 22 (5): 389–397. Woods, B., A. E. Spector, C. A. Jones, M. Orrell, and S. P. Davies. 2005. Reminiscence therapy for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2. Art. No.: CD001120. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD001120.pub2. Zeisel, J., N. M. Silverstein, J. Hyde, S. Levkoff, M. P. Lawton, and W. Holmes. 2003. Environmental correlates to behavioral health outcomes in Alzheimer ’s special care units. The Gerontologist 43 (5): 697–771.
Chapter 2
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia: Promising Results Dorothy Forbes and Debra Morgan
In persons with dementia, cognitive decline is usually accompanied by challenging symptoms such as sleep-wake disturbances, inability to manage daily activities, communication difficulties, depression, mood swings, agitation, aggression, and wandering (Department of Health 2009, 7–8). The stress of these symptoms on family caregivers is a major risk factor for institutionalization of the person with dementia (Hogan et al. 2007, 366). Managing these symptoms can not only enhance the well-being of those with dementia and their family caregivers but can also have cost benefits for them and the health care system (Alzheimer Society of Canada 2010, 8–9; Hux et al. 1998, 457). Because of the increased risk of falls and fractures, increased confusion, decrements in self-care (McCurry et al. 2000, 611), and risk of death among older adults with some medications (e.g., conventional and atypical antipsychotic drugs; Wang et al. 2005, 2335), nonpharmaceutical interventions should be the first choice of treatment. Drug treatment should be considered only after nonpharmaceutical approaches have failed and reversible medical and environmental causes have been ruled out (Hogan et al. 2007, 369; McCurry et al. 2000, 611). Although the evidence is insufficient regarding the effectiveness of several nonpharmacological interventions, such as music, snoezelen (multisensory stimulation), reminiscence therapy, validation therapy, aroma therapy, massage therapy, and light therapy, the relative risk of using these approaches is low (McCurry et al.
24
Dementia
2000, 611) and persons with dementia may benefit from them (Hogan et al. 2007, 369). This chapter examines the evidence specifically related to light therapy in managing symptoms of dementia. PATHOLOGY OF AGING AND DEMENTIA With normal aging, people aged 65 years and over may experience changes in core body temperature, melatonin rhythm, and the circadian rest–activity cycle, which may present as fragmented nocturnal sleep, multiple and prolonged awakenings in the second half of the night, and increased daytime napping (Campbell et al. 1995, 151, 154; McCurry et al. 2000, 613). These abnormalities and other related disturbances such as rest-activity cycle disruptions and sundowning are more frequent and pronounced in older adults with Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) (McCurry et al. 2000, 604). The neurobiological basis of these behavioral disorders is related to degenerative changes in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the hypothalamus that result in the loss of the expression of vasopressin (AVP) mRNA. Indeed, Liu et al. (2000, 314, 318) revealed that the total amount of AVP-mRNA expressed in the SCN was three times lower in persons with AD than in age- and time-of-death matched controls. In addition, the amount of AVP-mRNA was three times higher during the daytime than at night in control adults aged 60 to 80 years whereas no clear diurnal rhythm was observed in persons with AD. These findings suggest that the neurological basis of the circadian-rhythm disturbances that are responsible for behavioral rhythm disorders is located in the SCN. Liu and colleagues (2000, 320) emphasize that the loss of neurons expressing AVPmRNA in the SCN does not necessarily mean that the neurons have died; they may still be present but inactive and no longer able to express AVPmRNA. Reactivation of SCN neurons expressing AVP-mRNA was shown to be possible in studies of aged rats. Lucassen, Hofman, and Swabb (1995, 263) revealed that exposure to bright light appeared to reverse age-associated decrease in AVP-mRNA in old rats. As in the studies of aged rats, stimulation with light may positively affect the SCN neurons in aging humans and specifically in persons with dementia. The circadian pacemaker in the SCN is synchronized with the 24-hour day by “zeitgebers” or triggers of which light is the most important. Light impinging on the retina is transduced into neural activity that reaches the SCN through the retinohypothalamic and possibly the geniculo-hypothalamic tracts. Light leads to changes in the firing rates of specialized neurons in the SCN that in turn affect circadian rhythms (van Someren et al. 1996, 260). However, in older adults with dementia most zeitgebers
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
25
are reduced due to diminished social contacts, age-related deficiencies in the eye (e.g., macular degeneration, cataracts, blindness), day-length (e.g., winter months have fewer external light cues), and less exposure to sufficient outdoor or bright light (Burns et al. 2009, 718; Gasio et al. 2003, 3; McCurry et al. 2000, 607). Reduced sensory input is likely to lower the “general level of excitement” that is thought to play an important role in the entrainment of circadian rhythms (Burns et al. 2009, 711–712; van Someren et al. 1996, 260). Thus, an environment weak in phase prompts coupled with neuropathological damage causing poor sensitivity to such prompts can result in rhythm disorders. A decreased ability to maintain a stable circadian pattern of daytime arousal and nocturnal quiescence may contribute to sleep disruptions (Ancoli-Israel et al. 2002, 282; Burns et al. 2009, 711–712; McCurry et al. 2000, 604), cognitive dysfunction (Liu et al. 2000, 314; McCurry et al. 2000, 604), behavioral disturbances (e.g., agitation and sundowning; Burns et al. 2009, 711–712; Haffmans et al. 2001, 106; McCurry et al. 2000, 605), functional impairment (McCurry et al. 2000, 604), and depression (Liu et al. 2000, 314; McCurry et al. 2000, 608) in persons with dementia.
EVIDENCE OF THE EFFECTIVENESS OF LIGHT THERAPY IN MANAGING SYMPTOMS OF DEMENTIA Trials that Examined the Effectiveness of Light Therapy The following description of the evidence builds on a recent Cochrane Review (Forbes et al. 2009) with the addition of one study (Burns et al. 2009) which was retrieved following the Cochrane Review publication. In total, nine randomized controlled trials (RCTs; eleven articles) that examined the effectiveness of light therapy in managing the symptoms of dementia are included (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003; Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003; Burns et al. 2009; Dowling et al. 2005, 2007, 2008; Gasio et al. 2003; Graf et al. 2001; Lyketsos et al. 1999; Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa 1998; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008), with a total of 421 participants, of whom 324 completed the studies. Participants in the included trials were diagnosed with dementia (n = 13, 3%), probable AD (n = 318, 76%), vascular dementia (n = 55, 13%), mixed dementia (n = 5, 1%) or another type of dementia (n = 30, 7%). It is interesting to note that few participants were diagnosed with mixed dementia, defined as the coexistence of AD and vascular dementia. Mixed dementia is one of the most common forms of dementia with a prevalence range of 20–40% of persons with dementia (Zekry, Hauw, and Gold 2002, 1431, 1432).
26
Dementia
Four of the trials were conducted in the United States (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman et al. 2003, 22; Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 194; Dowling et al. 2005, 738; Dowling et al. 2007, 961; Dowling et al. 2008, 239; Lyketsos et al. 1999, 520), one was conducted in Austria (Graf et al. 2001, 726), one in Japan (Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa 1998, 647), one in the Netherlands (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2642), one in Switzerland (Gasio et al. 2003, 207), and one in the United Kingdom (Burns et al. 2009, 711). All participants were residents in a long-term care/seniors facility. Sources of Bright Light Seven trials used a Brite-Lite box (e.g., Apollo Light Systems, Orem, Utah) which was approximately 24 inches wide by 12 inches high by 3 inches deep and placed one meter from the participant’s head. The BriteLite utilized cool-white florescent, nonultraviolet, full-spectrum light bulbs with special ballast to augment the brightness. The treatment groups received light therapy ranging from 2,500 to 10,000 lux and the control groups received dim red light or dim, low-frequency blinking light, less than 300 lux, either in the morning or evening, for 1–2 hours, for 10 days to 10 weeks. There were two exceptions: the use of dawn-dusk simulation (maximum 400 lux) or placebo dim red light (< 5 lux) (Gasio et al. 2003, 211) and the use of ceiling mounted light fixtures (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2643). The Dawn-Dusk Simulator included a computer algorithm that drove an electronic controller connected to an overhead halogen lamp placed behind a diffusing membrane behind each participant’s bed. The ceiling-mounted fixtures were Plexiglas diffusers containing an equal amount of Philips TLD840 and TLD940 florescent tubes, which were installed in the common living area. The lights were kept on between approximately 0900 and 1800 hours with the aim of an exposure of ±1000 lux (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2643–2644). Effects of the Light Therapy Several outcomes were measured following exposure to light therapy: cognition, function, sleep, behavioral disturbances, and psychiatric disturbances. These are each discussed below. Cognition Four studies (Burns et al. 2009, 712; Gasio et al. 2003, 208; Graf et al. 2001, 726; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2646) used the Mini-Mental
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
27
State Examination (MMSE), a commonly used screening tool that concentrates on the cognitive aspects of mental function: orientation, immediate recall, attention and calculation, delayed recall, and language (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975). Morning bright light (10,000 lux) was compared with standard fluorescent-tube light (100 lux) in Burns et al. (2009, 713), evening bright light (3,000 lux) was compared with dim light (100 lux) in Graf et al. (2001, 726), all-day bright light (1,000 lux) was compared with dim light (300 lux) in Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2643–2644), and dawn-dusk simulation with light up to 400 lux was compared with dawn-dusk simulation with dim red light (<5 lux) in Gasio et al. (2003, 209–210). The data in the Burns et al. (2009, 715) and Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2647) studies were combined because the light therapy was administered in the morning or all day and their light intensities were considered bright light. The pooled data revealed no effect following 14 to 42 days of treatment (MD = 1.34, 95% CI −0.89 to 3.57, p = 0.24). Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2647) data revealed similar results after one year of treatment (MD = 1.70, 95% CI −1.03 to 4.43, p = 0.22), and after two years of treatment (MD = 3.60, 95% CI −1.05 to 8.25, p = 0.13). Graf et al. (2001, 726) administered evening bright light for 10 days that had no effect on cognition (MD = 0.70, 95% CI −4.90 to 6.3, p = 0.81). Similarly, the Gasio et al. (2003, unpublished data provided by authors) study employing the dawn-dusk simulation revealed no effect at endpoint (MD = 0.46, 95% CI −14.14 to 15.06, p = 0.95) and at follow up (three weeks after treatment) (MD = −0.50, 95% CI −10.68 to 9.67, p = 0.92). Thus, none of the trials demonstrated a significant change in cognition as a result of the light therapy. Function One study (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2646) measured functional limitations using Nurse-Informant Activities of Daily Living (NIADL). This scale was an adaptation of the Katz ADL scale (Katz et al. 1963) and includes six items that measure competence in feeding, continence, transferring, going to toilet, dressing, and bathing (Holmes et al. 1990). After six weeks of treatment, light therapy had a positive effect in attenuating the increase in functional limitations (MD = −5.00, 95% CI −9.87 to −0.13, p = 0.04; see Figure 2.1). After one year of treatment, there was no significant effect (MD −5.00, 95% CI −11.16 to 1.16, p = 0.11); however, a significantly less steep increase in functional decline was observed after two years of light therapy (MD = −16.00, 95% CI −26.21 to −5.79, p = 0.002; see Figure 2.2). These significant findings show support for the benefit of light therapy in lessening functional decline in persons with dementia.
28
Dementia
Figure 2.1
Forest plot: Change in function after 42 days: daytime bright light vs. control. Figure 2.2
Forest plot: Change in function after two years: daytime bright light vs. control.
Sleep Sleep latency, defined as the amount of time between reclining in bed and the onset of sleep (Venes 2001, 1200), was measured using wristwatchsize actigraphs in trials conducted by Gasio et al. (2003, 215) and Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2646). However the data from these two studies could not be pooled due to differences in light intensity. Findings from Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2651) revealed that there were no significant improvements in sleep onset latency after six weeks of treatment (MD = 6.00, 95% CI −12.34 to 24.34, p = 0.52), one year of treatment (MD = 5.00, 95% CI −24.79 to 34.79, p = 0.74), and after two years of treatment (MD = 10.00, 95% CI −11.33 to 31.33, p = 0.36). Similarly, data from Gasio et al. (2003, 214) revealed that dawn-dusk simulation did not significantly reduce sleep latency after three weeks of treatment (MD = −79.00, 95% CI −327.17, 169.17, p = 0.53) and after three weeks of follow-up (MD = −62.00, 95% CI −216.55 to 92.55, p = 0.43). Seven studies measured total night sleep duration following ten days (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003, 26), two weeks (Burns et al. 2009, 712), three weeks (Gasio et al. 2003, 207), four weeks (Lyketsos et al. 1999, 521), ten weeks (Dowling et al. 2005, 740; Dowling et al. 2008, 240), and one and two years of treatment (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2646) that consisted of bright-light therapy (>2500 to 10,000 lux) for one to two hours in the morning (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003, 26; Burns et al. 2009,
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
29
713; Dowling et al. 2008, 240; Lyketsos et al. 1999, 521), afternoon/evening (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003, 26; Dowling et al. 2005, 740), all day bright light (1000 lux) (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2643), or dawndusk simulation (400 lux) morning and evening (Gasio et al. 2003, 209–210). The treatment groups were compared with control groups who received dim light. Unfortunately, Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. (2003, 30) reported only the combined findings from the treatment and control groups data, and Lyketsos et al.’s (1999, 521) study was a crossover design and did not appear to have utilized analyses appropriate to a paired design. Thus, the data from these studies were excluded from the analyses. Combined data from Burns et al. (2009, unpublished data provided by authors), Dowling et al. (2005, 741), Dowling et al. (2008, 243), and Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2648) combined data revealed no effect of morning to all-day bright light on total night sleep duration (MD = 18.16, 95% CI −5.63 to 41.95, p = .13). Evening bright light (Dowling et al. 2005, 741) revealed similar findings (MD = 10.00, 95% CI −59.22 to 79.22, p = .78). Data from Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2648) also revealed that bright light had no effect on night-sleep duration after one year (MD = −36.00, 95% CI −84.21 to 12.21, p = .14) and two years of treatment (MD = −36.00, 95% CI −121.69 to 49.69, p = .41). Data from Gasio et al. (2003, 214) were analyzed separately due to the lower intensity of treatment light. No effect was found after three weeks of treatment (MD = 143.00, 95% CI −637.66 to 923.66, p = .72), or at follow-up (MD = 110.00, 95% CI −77.22 to 297.22, p = .25). Four studies (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003, 25; Dowling et al. 2005, 740; Gasio et al. 2003, 210; Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa 1998, 649) measured night-time activity counts. Unfortunately, reported data from Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. (2003, 30) and Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa (1998, 650) were not appropriate for inclusion in the metaanalyses. The findings from Dowling et al. (2005, 740) and Gasio et al. (2003, 214) could not be combined due to the differences in intensity of the light therapy. Dowling et al. (2005, 740) measured activity scores per night for both morning and afternoon treatment groups compared with control groups after 10 weeks of treatment. No effect on nighttime activity scores was found when bright light was administered in the morning (MD = 855.78, 95% CI −867.84 to 2579.40, p = .33), or afternoon (MD = −78.60, 95% CI −627.17 to 469.97, p = .78). In Gasio et al. (2003, 214) activity for each participant was averaged in one-hour bins and then over seven consecutive days of baseline, treatment, and follow-up. No effect on night activity was found after three weeks of treatment (MD = −20.60, 95% CI −46.52 to 5.32, p = .12) and after three weeks of follow-up (MD = −24.70,
30
Dementia
95% CI −52.70 to 3.30, p = .08). Dowling et al. (2005, 740, and 2008, 243) also measured the number of nighttime awakenings. Again, there was no effect on the number of nighttime awakenings after 10 weeks of treatment in either the morning bright-light exposure (MD = −2.37, 95% CI −8.75 to 4.01, p = .47) or evening exposure (MD = −4.38, 95% CI −11.61, 2.86, p = .24). In summary, a significant change in sleep latency, total nighttime duration, nighttime activity counts, and number of nighttime awakenings was not observed following light therapy. Behavioral Disturbances Behavioral disturbances (e.g., agitation) were measured in six studies using several instruments: (1) Agitated Behavior Rating scale (ABRS; Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 196), which measured agitation, manual manipulation, searching and wandering, escape behaviors, tapping and banging, and verbal agitation (Bliwise and Lee 1993); (2) Behavioral Pathology in AD scale (Behave-AD; Lyketsos et al. 1999, 522), which measured paranoid and delusion ideation, hallucinations, activity disturbances, aggressiveness, and anxiety and phobias (Reisberg et al. 1987); (3) Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI; Gasio et al. 2003, 208, Dowling et al. 2007, 964–965), which measured the severity and frequency of delusions, hallucinations, dysphoria, anxiety, agitation/aggression, euphoria, disinhibition, irritability/lability, apathy, and aberrant motor activity (Cummings et al. 1994); and (4) Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI; Burns et al. 2009, 712; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2646), which measured aggressive behavior, physically nonaggressive behavior, verbal agitation, and a global rating of agitation (Cohen-Mansfield, Marx, and Rosenthal 1989). In two studies (Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 199; Dowling et al. 2007, 968) behavioral disturbances were compared between morning light therapy exposure and afternoon/evening light therapy and assessed in the morning and evening shifts (Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 199). The findings from Lyketsos et al. (1999, 522–523) could not be included in the analyses for reasons cited above. With light therapy administered during the morning or day time, behavioral disturbances measured by ABRS scores (Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 196), NPI scores (Dowling et al. 2007, 964–965), and CMAI scores (Burns et al. 2009, 712; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2646) were pooled. The results revealed that light therapy administered during the morning or daytime had no effect on behavioral disturbances (SMD = −0.04, 95% CI −0.33 to 0.26, p = .80) following 10 to 50 days of light therapy. Similarly, no effect on behavioral disturbances was observed in the evening
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
31
assessment following 10 days of treatment (MD = 0.11, 95% CI −0.23 to 0.45, p = .52; Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 199), after five days of follow-up measured in the morning (MD 0.02, 95% CI −0.23 to 0.27, p = .87; AncoliIsrael, Martin, et al. 2003, 199), in the evening (MD 0.07, 95% CL −0.26, 0.40, p = .67; Ancoli-Israel, Martin et al. 2003, 199), following one year of treatment (MD = −2.00, 95% CI −11.71to 7.71, p = .69; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2647), and after two years of light therapy (MD = −9.00, 95% CI −21.34 to 3.34, p = .15; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2647). To assess behavioral disturbances following the administration of afternoon or evening light therapy, ABRS scores (Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 199) and NPI scores (Dowling et al. 2007, 968) were pooled. The results revealed that light therapy administered in the afternoon or evening had no effect on reducing behavioral disturbances when assessed during the morning (SMD = 0.16, 95% CI −0.31 to 0.64, p = .50) following 10 to 50 days of light therapy (Ancoli-Israel, Martin et al. 2003, 199; Dowling et al. 2007, 968) or when assessed during the evening (MD 0.07, 95% CI −0.26 to 0.40, p = .67) following 10 days of treatment (Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 199). Similar results were found after five days of follow-up during morning assessments (MD 0.10, 95% CI −0.16 to 0.36, p = .46; Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 199) and during evening assessments (MD 0.11, 95% CI −0.23 to 0.45, p = .53; Ancoli-Israel, Martin et al. 2003, 199). In summary, light therapy whether administered in the morning/all day or afternoon/evening had no significant effect on behavioral disturbances at the end of treatment or on follow-up when assessed during the morning and evening. Psychiatric Disturbances Dowling et al. (2007, 964–965) and Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2646) used the NPI to measure psychiatric disturbances. Their pooled data revealed no significant change after 42 to 50 days of light therapy (MD = 1.77, 95% CI −6.34 to 9.87, p = .67), after one year (MD = −0.30, 95% CI −2.73 to 2.13, p = .81), and after two years (MD = −3.30, 95% CI −7.03 to 0.43, p = .08). In addition, there was no effect when light therapy was administered in the afternoon (MD = 7.90, 95% CI, −0.46 to 16.26, p = .06; Dowling et al. 2007, 969). Gasio et al. (2003, 208) also used the NPI to examine psychiatric symptoms following three weeks of dawn-dusk simulation or dim red light therapy. No effect was observed following the treatment (MD = −3.19, 95% CI −9.83 to 3.45, p = .35) and after three weeks of follow-up (MD = −4.17, 95% CI −13.37 to 5.03, p = .37). Five studies measured depression: Dowling et al. (2007, 964–965) used the depression/dysphoria domain of the NPI–Nursing Home version
32
Dementia
(NPI-NH), a modified version of the NPI (Iverson et al. 2002); Gasio et al. (2003, 208) used the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Sheikh and Yesvage 1986); and Burns et al. (2009, 712), Lyketsos et al. (1999, 522), and Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2646) used the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia (CSDD; Alexopoulos et al. 1988). Lyketsos et al. (1999, 524) reported that no significant differences in scores of depression were found between groups at each time point. However, raw data were not reported and could not be retrieved. Pooled data (Burns et al. 2009, 715; Dowling et al. 2007, 968; Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2647) revealed no effect on depression following 14 to 50 days of light therapy (SMD = 0.06, 95% CI −0.55 to 0.67, p = .85). In addition, Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2647) data revealed no effect on depression using CSDD scores at one year (MD = −.30, 95% CI −4.36 to 3.76, p = .88) and after two years of treatment (MD −4.40, 95% CI −10.82 to 2.02, p = .18). However, administering the light therapy in the afternoon resulted in an effect after 50 days of treatment (MD = 3.20, 95% CI 0.86 to 5.51, p = .007) favoring the control group (Dowling et al. 2007, 968). These results should be viewed with caution due to the small sample size (n = 17). Analysis of the data provided to the authors by Gasio et al. (2003) revealed no effect on depression scores after three weeks of treatment (MD = −0.82, 95% CI −4.33 to 2.69, p = .65) or at follow-up (MD = −1.29, 95% CL −3.99, 1.41, p = .35). Apathy and indifference were measured using a domain of the NPI-NH (Iverson et al. 2002) following 50 days of treatment (Dowling et al. 2007, 969). There was no effect on apathy or indifference in either the morning administration of bright light (MD = 1.00, 95% CI −2.21 to 4.21, p = .54) or afternoon administration (MD = 0.40, 95% CI −3.00 to 3.80, p = .82). In summary, there is no significant evidence that bright light improves psychiatric symptoms in persons with dementia. DISCUSSION The Cochrane Review on light therapy and dementia (Forbes et al. 2009) with the addition of the trial by Burns et al. (2009) revealed little significant evidence of benefit of light therapy on cognition, function, sleep, behavioral disturbances, and psychiatric disturbances associated with dementia. Light therapy was shown to have an effect on two outcomes of interest. The Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2647) data revealed that light therapy had a positive effect in attenuating the increase in functional limitations after six weeks and after two years of light therapy. The sample size was adequate at six weeks (n = 87) but by two years the sample size was only 26 participants. By ensuring an adequate sample size at
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
33
follow-up, the effect of light therapy in limiting functional decline may be even greater as the power to detect a difference would be increased. The Dowling et al. (2007, 968) data revealed that the lack of afternoon brightlight therapy improved depression in the control group. However, these results should be viewed with caution as the sample size was small (n = 17; Dowling et al. 2007, 968). No significant evidence was found that light therapy decreases the decline in cognition, shortens sleep latency time, increases nocturnal sleep time, decreases nighttime activity, decreases behavioral disturbances, or improves psychiatric symptoms. These nonsignificant results may have been related to small sample sizes that contribute to insufficient power to detect a difference, if one is present. Notable exceptions were the Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. (2003, 25) and Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. (2003, 195) trials that included 92 participants and the Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2645) study that included 94 participants at baseline. Clearly further research with larger sample sizes is required which examines all of the outcomes of interest. Only one trial (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2653) examined adverse effects of light therapy. No adverse effects were reported; on the contrary, light therapy significantly reduced the ratings of irritability, dizziness, headache, constipation, and inability to sleep (Riemersma-van der Lek et al. 2008, 2653). Reporting of adverse events should be included in every intervention trial. Unfortunately, Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. (2003), Lyketsos et al. (1999), and Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa (1998) did not report the data needed to conduct a meta-analysis (e.g., means and standard deviations at baseline and endpoints for the treatment and control groups) for some of the outcomes of interest. Authors need to report these data or be willing to provide the data on request. In addition, two studies (Lyketsos et al. 1999, 521; Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa 1998, 649) used crossover designs and did not conduct analyses appropriate to a paired design. Although participants received no light treatment for one to four weeks prior to being crossed over to the other group, it is unknown if there is a carry-over effect from the two to four weeks of exposure to the light therapy. Some studies (e.g., Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003, 32) suggest that the effects of light therapy on nocturnal sleep may persist beyond the five-day treatment, while McCurry et al. (2000, 614) concluded that the benefits to sleep from increased bright light decline almost immediately once exposure is discontinued. Until the evidence is stronger, participants should not be regarded as generating independent data in the two phases of a crossover design. Another plausible reason for the lack of strong evidence of the effectiveness of light therapy was the heterogeneity within several of the trials
34
Dementia
in regard to participants’ diagnosis and severity of dementia. Three trials (Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. 2003, 25; Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 201; Dowling et al. 2005, 2008) were notable exceptions as only participants with AD were included. Persons with AD have a degenerative disease that is characterized by neurofibrillary tangles, usually in the hippocampus as well as the entorhinal cortex. As AD progresses, the pathology spreads to the lateral temporal cortex as well. Senile plaques occur later on (Chertkow et al. 2007, 274). On the other hand, persons with vascular dementia have heterogeneous brain pathology; their response to light therapy may depend on the areas in which ischemic damage has occurred (Mishima, Hishikawa, and Okawa 1998, 653). Few participants were diagnosed with mixed dementia (1%), although it is now recognized that mixed dementia is one of the most common forms of dementia (Zekry, Hauw, and Gold 2002, 1432). It may be that those with AD with a vascular component were included with participants diagnosed with only AD. This was the practice in the Canadian Study of Health and Aging (Zekry, Hauw, and Gold 2002, 1432). Attempts should be made to accurately diagnose the participants with diagnostic procedures such as positron emission tomography (PET) neuroimaging, which have been recognized as key diagnostic modalities (Zekry, Hauw, and Gold 2002, 1435). In addition, accurately determining the severity of the disease is also important as it is possible that persons with mild to moderate AD with more intact SCNs and who are more receptive to other “zeitgebers” or triggers will have a greater response to light therapy than persons with severe AD (Ancoli-Israel, Martin, et al. 2003, 201). Culture and geographic location are other factors that may influence the results of the review as trials were conducted in six different countries. For example, the participants from Japan may have experienced close family ties and well-developed informal care that may mask the symptoms of dementia (Zekry, Hauw, and Gold 2002, 1435). Geographic location is also a potential risk factor as participants in the United Kingdom may have experienced longer winters that resulted in fewer opportunities to be exposed to natural light than participants in more southern countries (Burns et al. 2009, 719). Subgroup analyses in the review could not be completed due to the small sample sizes. Investigators need to be sensitive to the importance of controlling for these differences in pathology, severity of dementia, and culture when designing studies that examine the effectiveness of light therapy. Otherwise, the degree of influence these factors may have on the effectiveness of bright-light exposure is unknown, making it difficult to predict who is most likely to benefit from the treatment.
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
35
Shochat et al. (2000) examined light exposure among elderly residents living in nursing homes. On average, healthy older adults are exposed to light above 2000 lux for 59 minutes a day. People with AD living at home are exposed to 29 minutes a day of light above 2000 lux, while institutionalized residents with dementia spent a median of 10.5 minutes per day (mean = 34; SD = 63) exposed to light above 1000 lux and a median of 4 minutes (mean = 19; SD = 39) per day exposed to light over 2000 lux (Shochat et al. 2000, 373, 374, 375). Clearly, there is a need to enhance residents’ natural exposure to light and to bright-light exposure in long-term care facilities. How best to carry this out is less clear. Most trials in the review incorporated some form of a Brite Lite box, and two trials used other forms of light therapy. Gasio et al. (2003, 211) used dawn-dusk simulated light therapy that exposed the participants to natural amounts of light at dawn and dusk. However, the intensity (<400 lux) and duration of the natural light at dawn and dusk may be insufficient to be effective in changing sleep, behavior, and/or psychiatric disturbances. Indeed, several studies have revealed that the minimum therapeutic dose of light for treating depression and regulating circadian rhythms is approximately 2000 lux over one to two hours (e.g., Sloane et al. 2005, 280–281). Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2643) used ceiling-mounted light fixtures with Plexiglas diffusers in the common living room. These approaches to enhancing light exposure for long-term care residents are less invasive and demanding of the residents and staff than the traditional Brite-Lite box. Use of a Brite Lite box requires participants to sit in front of the box for one to two hours. However, persons with moderate to severe dementia may find it difficult to remain seated and to stay awake for this period of time (Ancoli-Israel et al. 2002, 286; Sloane et al. 2005, 281). Some studies (e.g., Burns et al. 2009, 713) attempted to overcome this problem by having a research nurse present during the treatment period to engage all participants in conversation and to distract them if they attempted to leave. Under usual circumstances, it may be difficult to find the resources to have a staff member sit with and engage the residents for one to two hours. Burns et al. (2009, 719) recommends wall-mounted light boxes placed at eye level for the residents seated during breakfast. Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. (2003, 34) suggests increasing ambient light to 2000 lux in multipurpose rooms where residents spend much of their time may be the most efficient approach for improving the symptoms of dementia related to circadian activity rhythms. Since studies that evaluate the impact of increasing ambient light on the symptoms of dementia often incorporate a non-RCT design (e.g., Sloane et al. 2007, 1525), systematic reviews should
36
Dementia
also include these study designs to ensure that all of the best available evidence is presented. The best time of day to offer light therapy remains inconclusive although trials that administered light therapy in the morning, afternoon, evening, and all day were included in this systematic review. Evening bright-light treatment is beneficial for sleep-maintenance problems in older adults as well as for persons who are phase-advanced, that is, falling asleep in the early evening and awakening too early in the morning (Campbell et al. 1995, 153). Morning light exposure is most beneficial for persons who are phase-delayed, that is whose sleep onset and morning rising are pushed to later hours (McCurry et al. 2000, 613). Individuals with AD have been reported to have phase-delayed activity (Satlin et al. 1995, 769). However, other studies (e.g., Ancoli-Israel et al. 2002, 282) have not supported the expected direction of change in individuals with AD. Ancoli-Israel, Gehrman, et al. (2003, 33–34) report that the timing of light required to achieve a phase advance or phase delay may be different in people with AD owing to the deterioration of the SCN, and recommends increasing light exposure throughout the day and evening. Most recently, Burns et al. (2009, 719) report that their use of light therapy at 10:00 am for two hours reduced the chance of residents receiving light therapy on or before their temperature nadir (lowest point), which has been suggested to shift the diurnal rhythm in the opposite expected direction. Burns et al. (2009, 719) also found that light therapy was only effective during the winter months and not during the summer months when there was increased opportunity to natural exposure of light. Clearly further research is required in this area. In addition, community-based light-therapy research is needed. All of the participants in the included studies resided in long-term care/seniors facilities. However, light-therapy modalities implemented in residential facilities may not translate readily to a home setting as they may be impractical, unacceptable, and/or overly expensive for the family caregiver and person with dementia residing in the community (McCurry et al. 2000, 614). Although there is no known research that has examined the impact of being exposed to natural daylight, persons residing in the community (and those residing in long-term care facilities with assistance of healthcare aides or volunteers) can greatly increase their daily light exposure by spending time outdoors. For example, typical lux levels outside on a cloudy day range from 8,000 to 10,000 lux; interior daytime exposure sitting near windows equals approximately 1000 lux (McCurry et al. 2000, 614). As with all dementia care, a light-therapy plan that makes sense to the person with dementia, the family caregiver and healthcare provider,
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
37
and which targets factors that are both relevant and modifiable in their situation, is more likely to be effective than a “one size fits all” approach (McCurry et al. 2000, 621). CONCLUSION Given the methodological shortcomings of the trials included in this review, there is not good evidence that light therapy is effective or ineffective. An exception is the well-designed longitudinal RCT by Riemersma-van der Lek et al. (2008, 2643), which found that light therapy had a positive effect in attenuating the increase in functional limitations. Further well-designed research is required to compare different light-therapy approaches (e.g., light boxes, ambient light, natural light), to determine the most appropriate illumination intensity, frequency, time of day, and duration of the intervention with persons with AD, vascular dementia, and mixed dementia at different levels of severity of the disease (see Table 2.1). Outcomes that contribute to the quality of life of persons with dementia, cost implications, and adverse effects of light therapy also need to be examined. What is clear from the evidence is that older adults and especially persons with dementia should be spending more time outdoors in natural light (see Table 2.2). Healthcare aides, volunteers, and family caregivers are in an ideal position to take persons with Table 2.1 Recommendations to Improve Methodological Quality of Trials Well-designed RCTs that include random generation of subjects and concealed allocation to groups are the preferred design of choice. When RCTs are not feasible, the best available evidence should also be included in a systematic review, recognizing the potential risk of biases. Sample sizes should be large enough to detect a difference if one is present. Samples should be as homogenous as possible in terms of dementia diagnosis, level of severity, culture, and geography. Trial authors should report the information needed to conduct a meta-analysis or be willing to share this information with the authors of a review. Further research is needed to determine the most effective and feasible bright-light modality, time of day to administer the treatment, length of treatment, and the influence of season on the outcomes. Reporting of adverse events should be included in every intervention trial. Research on the effectiveness of light-therapy modalities in the home setting is also needed.
38
Dementia
Table 2.2 Recommendations for Increasing Exposure of Light for Persons with Dementia In long-term care facilities, increase the use of BriteLite boxes mounted to the wall at residents’ eye level during breakfast. Increase the use of ceiling-mounted light fixtures with Plexiglas diffusers in the common living rooms. Healthcare aides, volunteers, and family caregivers are in an ideal position to take persons with dementia for a daily walk, which will increase their exposure to natural light. Increasing exposure to light is especially important after the autumn equinox and before the spring equinox.
dementia for a daily walk that would increase their exposure to light and possibly alleviate some of their challenging symptoms. This is a potentially easy solution to managing very difficult symptoms of dementia. REFERENCES Alexopoulos, G. S., R. C. Abrams, R. C. Young, and C. A. Shamoian. 1988. Cornell scale for depression in dementia. Biological Psychiatry 23: 271–284. Alzheimer Society of Canada. 2010. The rising tide: The impact of dementia on Canadian society. Executive summary. Toronto: Author. Ancoli-Israel, S., P. Gehrman, J. L. Martin, T. Shochat, M. Marler, J. Corey-Bloom, and L. Levi. 2003. Increased light exposure consolidates sleep and strengthens circadian rhythms in severe Alzheimer ’s disease patients. Behavioural Sleep Medicine 1 (1): 22–36. Ancoli-Israel, S., J. L. Martin, P. Gehrman, T. Shochat, J. Corey-Bloom, M. Marler, S. Nolan, and L. Levi. 2003. Effect of light on agitation in institutionalized patients with severe Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 11 (2): 194–203. Ancoli-Israel, S., J. L. Martin, D. F. Kripke, M. Marler, and M. R. Klauber. 2002. Effect of light treatment on sleep and circadian rhythms in demented nursing home patients. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 50: 282–289. Bliwise, D. L., and K. A. Lee. 1993. Development of an agitated behavior rating scale for discrete temporal observations. Journal of Nursing Measurement 1 (2): 115–124. Burns, A., H. Allen, B. Tomenson, D. Duignan, and J. Byrne. 2009. Bright light therapy for agitation in dementia: A randomized controlled trial. International Psychogeriatrics 21 (4): 711–721. Campbell, S. S., M. Terman, A. J. Lewy, D.-J. Dijk, C. I. Eastman, and Z. Boulos. 1995. Light treatment for sleep disorders: Consensus report. V. Age-related disturbances. Journal of Biological Rhythms 10: 151–154.
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
39
Chertkow, H., Z. Nasreddine, Y. Joanette, V. Drolet, J. Kirk, F. Massoud, S. Belleville, and H. Bergman. 2007. Mild cognitive impairment and cognitive impairment, no dementia: Part A, concept and diagnosis. Alzheimer ’s and Dementia 3: 266–282. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, and A. S. Rosenthal. 1989. A description of agitation in a nursing home. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences 44 (3): M77–84. Cummings, J. L., M. Mega, K. Gray, S. Rosenberg-Thompson, D. A. Carusi, and J. Gornbein. 1994. The neuropsychiatric inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44: 2308–2314. Department of Health. 2009. Living well with dementia: A national strategy: Accessible summary. National Dementia Strategy Working Group: Leeds, UK. Dowling, G. A., R. L. Burr, E. J. W. van Someren, E. M. Hubbard, J. S. Luxenberg, J. Mastick, and B. A. Cooper. 2008. Melatonin and bright-light treatment for rest-activity disruption in institutionalized patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56: 239–246. Dowling, G. A., C. L. Graf, E. M. Hubbard, and J. S. Luxenberg. 2007. Light treatment for neuropsychiatric behaviors in Alzheimer ’s disease. Western Journal of Nursing Research 29 (8): 961–975. Dowling, G. A., J. Mastick, E. M. Hubbard, J. S. Luxenberg, and L. Burr. 2005. Effect of timed bright light treatment for rest-activity disruption in institutionalized patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20: 738–743. Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh. 1975. Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12: 189–198. Forbes, D. A., I. Culum, A. Lischka, D. Morgan, S. Peacock, J. Forbes, and S. Forbes. 2009. Light therapy in managing cognitive, sleep, functional, behavioral, or psychiatric disturbances in dementia. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 4: CD003946. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD003946.pub3. Gasio, P. F., K. Krauchi, C. Cajochen, E. van Someren, I. Amrhein, M. Pache, E. Savaskan, and A. Wirz-Justice. 2003. Dawn-dusk simulation light therapy of disturbed circadian rest-activity cycles in demented elderly. Experimental Gerontology 38 (1–2): 207–216. Graf, A., C. Wallner, V. Schubert, M. Willeit, W. Wlk, P. Fischer, S. Kasper, and A. Neumeister. 2001. The effects of light therapy on Mini-Mental State Examination scores in demented patients. Biological Psychiatry 50 (9): 725–727. Haffmans, P. M. J., R. C. Sival, S. A. P. Lucius, Q. Cats, and L. Van Gelder. 2001. Bright light therapy and melatonin in motor restless behaviour in dementia: A placebo-controlled study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16: 106–110. Hogan, D. B., P. Bailey, A. Carswell, B. Clarke, C. Cohen, D. Forbes, M. Man-SonHing, K. Lanctôt, D. Morgan, and L. Thorpe. 2007. Management of mild to moderate dementia. Alzheimer ’s and Dementia 3: 355–384.
40
Dementia
Holmes, D., J. Teresi, A. Weiner, C. Monaco, J. Ronch, and R. Vickers. 1990. Impacts associated with special care units in long-term care facilities. Gerontologist 30 (2): 178–183. Hux, M. J., B. J. O’Brien, M. Iskedjian, R. Goeree, M. Gagnon, and S. Gauthier. 1998. Relation between severity of Alzheimer ’s disease and costs of caring. Canadian Medical Association Journal 159 (5): 457–465. Iverson, G. L., G. A. Hopp, K. DeWolfe, and K. Solomons. 2002. Measuring change in psychiatric symptoms using the Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Nursing Home version. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17 (5): 438–443. Katz, S., A. B. Ford, R. W. Moskowitz, B. A. Jackson, and M. W. Jaffe. 1963. Studies of illness in the aged: The index of ADL: A standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA 185 (12): 94–99. Liu, R.-Y., J.-N. Zhou, W. J. G. Hoogendijk, J. van Heerikhuize, W. Kamphorst, U. A. Unmehopa, M. A. Hofman, and D. F. Swaab. 2000. Decreased vasopressin gene expression in the biological clock of Alzheimer disease patients with and without depression. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 59 (4): 314–322. Lucassen, P. J., M. A. Hofman, and D. F. Swaab. 1995. Increased light intensity prevents the age-related loss of vasopressin-expressing neurons in the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. Brain Research 693: 261–266. Lyketsos, C. G., L. L. Veiel, A. Baker, and C. Steele. 1999. A randomized, controlled trial of bright light therapy for agitated behaviors in dementia patients residing in long-term care. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14 (7): 520–525. McCurry, S. M., C. F. Reynolds III, S. Ancoli-Israel, L. Teri, and M. Vitiello. 2000. Treatment of sleep disturbance in Alzheimer ’s disease. Sleep Medicine Reviews 4 (6): 603–628. Mishima, K., Y. Hishikawa, and M. Okawa. 1998. Randomized, dim light controlled, crossover test of morning bright light therapy for rest-activity rhythm disorders in patients with vascular dementia and dementia of Alzheimer ’s type. Chronobiology International 15 (6): 647–654. Reisberg, B., J. Borenstein, S. P. Salob, S. H. Ferris, E. Franssen, and G. A. Anastasios. 1987. Behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease: Phenomenology and treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 48 (5 Suppl.): 9–15. Riemersma-van der Lek, R. F., D. F. Swaab, J. Twisk, E. M. Hol, W. J. G. Hoogendijk, and E. J. W. van Someren. 2008. Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and noncognitive function in elderly residents of group care facilities: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 299 (22): 2642–2655. Satlin, A., L. Volicer, E. G. Stopa, and D. Harper. 1995. Circadian locomotor activity and core-body temperature rhythms in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging 16 (5): 765–771. Sheikh, J. I., and J. A. Yesvage. 1986. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In Clinical geronotology:
Light Therapy for Managing Symptoms of Dementia
41
A guide to assessment and intervention, ed. T. L. Brink, 165–173. New York: Harworth. Shochat, T., J. Martin, M. Marler, and S. Ancoli-Israel. 2000. Illumination levels in nursing home patients: Effects on sleep and activity rhythms. Journal of Sleep Research 9: 373–379. Sloane, P. D., E. Noell-Waggoner, S. E. Hickman, C. M. Mitchell, C. S. Williams, J. S. Preisser, A. L. Barrick, S. Zimmerman, and E. Brawley. 2005. Implementing a lighting intervention in public areas of long-term care facilities: Lessons learned. Alzheimer ’s Care Quarterly 6 (4): 280–293. Sloane, P. D., C. S. Williams, C. M. Mitchell, J. S. Preisser, W. Wood, A. L. Barrick, S. E. Hickman, et al. 2007. High-intensity environmental light in dementia: Effect on sleep and activity. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 50: 1524–1533. van Someren, E. J. W., E. E. O. Hagebeuk, C. Lijzenga, P. Scheltens, S. E. J. A. De Rooij, C. Jonker, A.-M. Pot, M. Mirmiran, and D. F. Swaab. 1996. Circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in Alzheimer ’s disease. Biological Psychiatry 40: 259–270. Venes, D. 2001. Taber’s cyclopedic medical dictionary. 19th ed. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis. Wang, P. S., S. Schneeweiss, J. Avorn, M. A. Fischer, H. Mogun, D. H. Solomon, and M. A. Brookhart. 2005. Risk of death in elderly users of conventional vs. atypical antipsychotic medications. New England Journal of Medicine 353 (22): 2335–2341. Zekry, D., J.-J. Hauw, and G. Gold. 2002. Mixed dementia: Epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 50: 1431–1438.
Chapter 3
Nonpharmacological Approaches to Treating Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Advanced Dementia: Person-Centered, Stage-Related Care Karan Kverno
Dementing illnesses are brain diseases that impair cognitive and physical functioning. For most persons, the illnesses also impair aspects of neuropsychiatric (behavioral and psychological) functioning. Neuropsychiatric symptoms such as apathy, depression, and psychosis further decrease functional independence and quality of life (Kaup et al. 2007; Lyketsos 2007; Samus et al. 2005). In addition, they create serious caregiving challenges and are often a primary reason for transition to nursing home care (Chan et al. 2003; Gaugler et al. 2005, 2007, 2009; Volicer, Hurley, and Blasi 2003; Yaffe et al. 2002). Unlike the cognitive and physical functional declines of advanced dementia, neuropsychiatric symptoms can be reduced through effective treatment. Psychotropic medications, especially the antipsychotics, carry risks for morbidity and mortality (Ballard and Waite 2006; Carson, McDonagh, and Peterson 2006; Gill et al. 2007; Salzman et al. 2008; Schneider, Dagerman, and Insel 2005), making nonpharmacological strategies the preferred first line of treatment. The purpose of this chapter is to summarize the evidence for treating neuropsychiatric symptoms with nonpharmacological strategies in nursing home residents with advanced dementia. The goals of all treatments in advanced dementia are to reduce
44
Dementia
symptoms, decrease discomfort or distress, support dignity, and improve quality of life. ADVANCED DEMENTIA Advanced dementia refers to the final stages of dementia. Whether measured by a Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975) score of 0–10 or lower, or some other global measure of functioning (see Table 3.1), advanced dementia is characterized by severe or very severe cognitive impairment, total dependence on others to meet Table 3.1 Dementia Severity Staging Scales: Advanced Dementia
Rating scale table
Domains
Clinical Dementia Rating Cognitive ability to function in: (CDR) (Hughes et al. 1982) 1. Memory 2. Orientation 3. Judgment and Problem-solving 4. Community Affairs 5. Home and Hobbies 6. Personal Care
Functional Assessment of Alzheimer Disease (FAST) (Sclan and Reisberg 1992)
1. Functional performance 2. Activities of daily living skills
Scoring range and description of advanced stage(s) CDR-0.5 (very mild) to CDR-3 (severe) CDR-3 = severe: Severe memory loss; only fragments remain. Orientation to person only. Unable to make judgments or solve problems. No significant function in home outside of own room. Requires much help with personal care; often incontinent. FAST 1 (normal) to FAST 7 (Severe) FAST 6: Requires assistance with dressing, bathing, toileting. Incontinent of urine and stool. FAST 7: Language is limited to a few words, unable to walk, sit, and hold up head.
Table 3.1
(Continued)
Global Deterioration Scale (GDS) (Reisberg et al. 1982)
1. Memory and cognition 2. Functional status 3. Personality and emotional changes
GDS 1 (normal) to GDS 7 (Very Severe) GDS 6: Largely unaware of all recent events and experiences. Retains some knowledge of past life. Unaware of surroundings. Requires substantial assistance with ADLs. May be incontinent. GDS 7: All verbal abilities are lost. Incontinent of urine. Requires assistance in toileting and eating. Loss of psychomotor skills. Generalized cortical and focal neurological signs are frequently present.
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein and McHugh 1975)
1. Orientation 2. Attention 3. Registration and recall 4. Naming 5. Repetition 6. Reading 7. Writing 8. Visual construction
Score range: 0–30 Severe: 0–10
Note: Whereas the CDR, FAST, and GDS measure global changes in functioning, the MMSE is specific to cognitive functioning. Persons with global ratings of severe or very severe score near 0 on the MMSE. A variety of cognitive scales have been developed to measure preserved cognitive functioning that falls below a score of approximately 5 on the MMSE. These scales may be used in clinical practice to monitor response to interventions. Examples include: Severe Impairment Rating Scale (SIRS) (Rabins and Steele 1996); the Severe MiniMental State Exam (SMMSE) (Harrell et al. 2000); the Severe Impairment Battery (SIB) (Panisset et al. 1994), or its shorter version (SIB-S) (Saxton et al. 2005).
46
Dementia
basic needs, and impaired ability to communicate through the use of language. Despite severe cognitive and functional impairment, persons with advanced dementia may still show interest in people and have the ability to attend to and engage in a variety of sensory activities. Approximately 40% of nursing home residents diagnosed with dementia are in the advanced stages with severe or very severe cognitive impairment (Gruneir et al. 2008). Two prospective cohort studies of nursing home residents with advanced dementia have furthered our understanding of the complex clinical course: the Care of Nursing Home Residents with Advanced Dementia (CareAD) study, and the Choices, Attitudes, and Strategies for Care of Advanced Dementia at End-of-Life (CASCADE) study. The CareAD study determined that upwards of 80% of the residents had one or more skin problems, nutrition or hydration problems, gastrointestinal problems, and infections (Black et al. 2006). The average number of medications was 14.6 (SD = 7.4) (Blass et al. 2008). The CASCADE study determined that eating disorders were very common (85.8%) and that pneumonia, dyspnea, and pain affected some 39–41% of residents. The six-month mortality rate for persons with pneumonia, febrile episodes, or eating problems was between 39% and 47% (Mitchell et al. 2009). These studies call attention to the need for considering the treatment of neuropsychiatric symptoms within the context of end-of-life care for persons with advanced dementia. Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Advanced Dementia Neuropsychiatric symptoms describe abnormalities in mood (depression or dysphoria, anxiety, euphoria), behavior (agitation, physical or verbal aggression, apathy or withdrawal, wandering or other aberrant motor), thought process (delusions, hallucinations), sleep (nighttime behaviors) and appetite or eating. Table 3.2 lists the symptom domains from neuropsychiatric assessment and outcome measurement tools that have been used in recent studies of advanced dementia. Following the decline in cognitive functioning, neuropsychiatric symptoms are some of the earliest symptoms to occur and they persist into the advanced stages of dementia. In a U.S. population–based study, Lyketsos and coworkers (2002) found that approximately 50% of persons with mild cognitive impairment had at least one neuropsychiatric symptom, whereas the prevalence increased to 80% with a diagnosis of dementia. Studies of advanced dementia indicate that prevalence may reach as high as 85–90% in advanced dementia (Koopmans et al. 2009; Kverno, Rabins et al. 2008; Zuidema et al. 2007.
1. Aberrant vocalization 2. Motor agitation 3. Aggression 4. Resistance to care
1. Delusions 2. Hallucinations 3. Depression or dysphoria 4. Anxiety 5. Agitation or aggression 6. Elation or euphoria 7. Disinhibition 8. Irritability or lability 9. Apathy 10. Aberrant motor activity 11. Nighttime behavioral disturbances 12. Appetite and eating abnormalities
1. Mood-related signs 2. Behavioral disturbance 3. Physical signs 4. Cyclic functions 5. Ideational disturbance
1. Physically aggressive 2. Physically nonaggressive 3. Verbally aggressive 4. Verbally nonaggressive
1. Paranoid and delusional ideations 2. Hallucinations 3. Activity disturbances 4. Aggressiveness 5. Diurnal rhythm disturbance 6. Affective disturbance 7. Anxieties and phobias And one global rating of severity
14. 15.
11. 12. 13.
10.
9.
7. 8.
6.
5.
1. 2. 3. 4.
Disruptive Manipulative Wandering Socially objectionable Demanding interaction Communication difficulties Noisy Active aggression Passive aggression Verbal aggression Restless Destructive (self) Destructive (property) Affect elated Delusions/ hallucinations
PGDRS
Note: BEHAVE-AD: Behavior Pathology in Alzheimer ’s Disease Rating Scale (Reisberg et al. 1987; Sclan et al. 1996). CMAI: Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (Cohen-Mansfield, Marx, and Rosenthal 1989). CSDD: Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia (Alexopoulos et al. 1988). NPI: Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (Cummings et al. 1994). PAS: Pittsburgh Agitation Scale (Rosen et al. 1994). PGDRS: Psychogeriatric Dependency Rating Scale, Behaviors in Dementia subscale (Wilkinson and Graham-White 1980).
PAS
NPI
CSDD
CMAI
BEHAVE-AD
Table 3.2 Neuropsychiatric Symptom Inventory Domains: Assessment and Outcome Measures Used in Advanced Dementia Clinical Research
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 1. Activity 2. Well-being 3. Non-well-being
1. Positive affect: pleasure, interest, contentment 2. Negative affect: sadness, worry/anxiety, and anger 1. Observable behaviors in affective states: discomfort, activity engagement, and interactions with others in the previous week 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
The objective is to quantify day-to-day living experiences as they are affected by Alzheimer disease.
A method of implementing person-centered care. Systematic observation of factors associated with expressions of well-being are fed back to care staff and their managers to help planning, implementation, and assessment of person-centered care.
Designed for the use of research and other staff in assessing QOL by direct observation of facial expression, body movement, and other cues that do not depend on self-report.
The QUALID was developed to specifically evaluate quality of life in advanced dementia. It is a reliable and valid scale, administered to caregivers, for rating QOL in persons with latestage dementing illness.
Developed to assess QOL of all nursing home residents with dementia, including advanced dementia. Three subscales, “Positive self-image,” “Feeling at home,” and “Having something to do” cannot be observed in very severe cognitive impairment.
Alzheimer Disease-Related Quality of Life (ADRQL) (Kasper et al. 2009)
Dementia Care Mapping (DCM) (Bradford Dementia Group 1997)
Philadelphia Geriatric Center Affect Rating Scale (PGC-ARS) (Lawton, Van Haitsma, and Klapper 1996)
Quality of Life in Late Stage Dementia (Weiner et al. 2000)
QUILIDEM: a Dementia specific quality of life instrument (Ettema et al. 2007).
Care relationship Positive affect Negative affect Restless tense behavior Positive self image Social relations Social isolation Feeling at home Having something to do
Social interaction Awareness of self Feelings and mood Enjoyment of activities Response to surroundings
Domains
Descriptions
Measurement tool and authors
Table 3.3 Observation-Based Measures of Quality of Life Used in Advanced Dementia Clinical Research
Nonpharmacological Approaches
49
Quality of Life in Advanced Dementia With failing cognitive, functional, and physical health, what constitutes quality of life in advanced dementia? How do we, as caregivers, attempt to improve or at least maintain, quality of life? The main difficulty in answering these questions is that quality of life is a subjective experience, and it is difficult to evaluate the quality of life for another person, especially persons with advanced dementia who have severe cognitive and communication impairments and limited insight. Several quality-of-life scales used by clinical researchers that rely on observed behaviors are described in Table 3.3. Directly observable indicators of quality of life include facial expressions (e.g., smiling or showing interest) and nonverbal behaviors (e.g., physical relaxation, engagement in activities, and interaction with others). Persons with advanced dementia are unable to initiate meaningful activities that may improve their well-being or quality of life. Unfortunately, nursing home residents in the United States typically spend over half of their daytime hours doing very little or nothing at all (Buettner and Fitzsimmons 2003; Cohen-Mansfield, Marx, and Werner 1992; Ice 2002; Volicer et al. 2006) and an additional 12% receive activities that are inappropriate for their level of functioning or interests (Buettner and Fitzsimmons 2003). The Omnibus Budget Reconciliation Act (OBRA) of 1987 changed the model of nursing home care from a medical focus to a focus on self-determination and quality of care. It required that nursing home residents be provided with services sufficient to attain and maintain their highest practicable physical, mental, and psychosocial well-being. The current Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS) regulations for long-term care require staff members to carry out meaningful activities that assist the resident to maintain and enhance his or her self-esteem and self-worth (Department of Health and Human Services 2009). This gradual move in nursing homes from impersonal institutional care toward person-centered care has been described as a “culture change” because it represents a fundamental shift in philosophy and practice (White-Chu et al. 2009). PRINCIPLES OF CARE Person-Centered Care Person-centered care is care focused on individual preferences and capabilities. Major components of person-centered care include: (1) Resident direction with the personal representative or proxy decisionmaker
50
Dementia
participating in decisionmaking regarding care; (2) homelike atmosphere; (3) close relationships between resident, family, and a consistent care team; (4) staff empowerment and additional training; (5) collaborative decisionmaking among the healthcare team members; and (6) ongoing qualityimprovement processes (Koren 2010). A person-centered approach is to treat neuropsychiatric symptoms as meaningful and to identify and treat the needs that are being communicated by those symptoms. In recognition of the frailty of persons with advanced dementia, it is essential to work with family members and personal representatives to determine needs and preferences for end-of-life care.
Stage-Specific Care The American Psychiatric Association’s Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Patients with Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias (Rabins and McIntyre 2007) recommends the development and implementation of stage-specific treatment plans. This is in recognition that Alzheimer dementia and most other dementias are progressive and care must be adapted to the level of functioning as personal capacities change. The selection of specific nonpharmacologic therapies should be based upon the unique characteristics of the patient, the caregiver, the availability of the therapy, the severity of the neuropsychiatric symptoms, and the likelihood that the specific symptoms will respond to the specific therapy. The difficulty in quantifying a prognosis of six months or less limits the access of many nursing home residents, especially those with dementia, to hospice care where palliative care is provided, yet as life expectancy becomes shorter, the goals of care must shift from curative to palliative.
The Principles of Care Based upon available evidence, the most recent American Association of Geriatric Psychiatry (AAGP) Position Statement outlines three principles of care regarding neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia: (1) identify and differentiate neuropsychiatric symptoms; (2) consider possible contributions; and (3) make sure that contributing causes are all addressed and that basic needs are met before deciding whether specific additional treatments are indicated (Lyketsos et al. 2006). Medications are recommended when other approaches have failed or when there is sufficient urgency, distress, disability, or danger risk. In general, medication treatment effects for neuropsychiatric symptoms appear modest at best.
Nonpharmacological Approaches
51
Step 1. Identify and differentiate which neuropsychiatric symptoms are present. Symptoms such as physically or verbally aggressive behaviors are easy to detect; however, it is much more difficult to tell the difference between symptoms such as apathy and depression. Detection can be accomplished in everyday clinical practice with high reliability using systematic interviews of patients and caregivers. Observation and caregiver reports may determine the presence and frequency of symptoms; however, there are a variety of valid and reliable assessment tools for more accurately differentiating among neuropsychiatric symptoms. Most must be administered by a professional who has been trained to use the tool. A list of rating scales that are used to identify and differentiate neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia appears in Table 3.2. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia have been examined using several different strategies. The first is to look at single-symptom frequencies. The most frequently occurring neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia are aggressive or agitated behaviors, withdrawal or apathy, depression, and anxiety (Kverno, Rabins, et al. 2008; Lyketsos et al. 2000; Zuidema et al. 2009). The second is to look at how the symptoms cluster together. Five common symptom clusters have been identified that remain fairly stable across all levels of dementia and into advanced dementia, including agitation/aggression, depression, psychosis, psychomotor agitation, and apathy (Lawlor and Bhriain 2001; Zuidema et al. 2007). A third way is to examine syndromal patterns of neuropsychiatric symptoms, the way that people can be grouped into categories having similar patterns of symptoms. A statistical method called latent class analysis has been used to identify clusters of people with similar symptoms. Three distinct syndromes were identified in the CareAD sample of nursing home residents with advanced dementia (Kverno, Black, et al. 2008): one group with few neuropsychiatric symptoms, one characterized by withdrawal and lethargy, and a third characterized by agitation and psychosis (in addition to a high prevalence of refusal and resistance to care). Depressive symptoms frequently co-occurred with withdrawal and lethargy as well as with agitation and psychosis. Whether depression is considered a separate syndrome or not, apathy, agitation, and dysphoric symptoms are dimensions of decreased psychological well-being associated with reduced quality of life in advanced dementia (Volicer, Camberg, et al. 1999). The identification of neuropsychiatric syndromes across different types of dementia diagnoses has important implications for treatments.
52
Dementia
Step 2. Consider possible contributions. Modifiable Conditions. Modifiable conditions that are related to verbal or physically aggressive behaviors are the presence of depression, delusions, hallucinations, lack of understanding, constipation, and pain (Leonard et al. 2006; Volicer, VanderSteen, and Frijters 2009). Knowledge of a resident’s habits and typical behaviors as well as the context of behavioral changes can help providers decipher the meaning. Pain is often evident during periods of physical movement. Pain can be expressed through verbalization (e.g., gasping, groaning, screaming), through facial expressions (e.g., grimacing, frowning), and through behaviors (e.g., guarding, pushing). Several pain scales have been developed specifically for evaluating pain in advanced dementia (see Table 3.4). By integrating a pain assessment into routine care, nurses found that not only were they better able to manage the pain of their patients, but the nurses also experienced decreased stress relative to a control group of nurses (Fuchs-Lacelle, Hadjistavropoulos, and Lix 2008). Between 50% and 70% of aggressive behaviors may be associated with caregiving activities, including behaviors that are uncooperative, resistive, or combative. Resistiveness to care is relatively rare in borderline intact and mildly impaired residents and increases gradually as the ability to understand deteriorates, with the highest prevalence in those with very severe cognitive impairment (Volicer, Bass, and Luther 2007). Combative or resistive behavior on the part of residents may reflect fear, misperceptions of the need for care activities, or misperceptions of the caregiver ’s intent. In a study of 216 nursing home residents with advanced dementia, the majority of verbally and physically aggressive behaviors (making strange noises, grabbing, spitting, hitting, screaming, and pushing) occurred during morning care routines (Koopmans et al. 2009). Unmet Needs. Regardless of the type of dementia, impaired communication is a central problem affecting the ability to express physical, emotional, and social needs. Neuropsychiatric symptoms may represent responses to not having needs met or may represent attempts to express needs. To address unmet needs, caregivers must attempt to understand the meaning of the behaviors. This might include checking for and alleviating possible sources of discomfort, making attempts to decrease sensory irritants by decreasing the level of noise or motion, applying glasses or hearing aids, and talking in a calm reassuring manner. Agitation or aggressive behaviors may represent a need for comfort, given the positive relationship found between discomfort and overall agitation, and in particular, verbally agitated behaviors (Pelletier and Landreville 2007). The goal of care is to maximize the congruence of care with the person’s needs.
A five item observational tool with numerical equivalents for 1. Breathing independent of vocalization each of the domains. Observation of patients during activity (transferring, turning) records behavioral indicators of pain. 2. Negative vocalization 3. Facial expression 4. Body language 5. Consolability 1. Facial expressions A comprehensive checklist of 60 pain behaviors that can be observed to assess both common and subtle pain behaviors. 2. Activity / body movement Shorter versions are being tested. 3. Social / personality / mood 4. Other (eating and sleeping changes, vocal behaviors, and physiologic changes)
Pain Assessment in Advanced Dementia Scale (PAINAD). (Warden, Hurley, and Volicer 2003)
Pain Assessment Checklist for Seniors with Limited Ability to Communicate (PACSLAC) (Fuchs-Lacelle and Hadjistavropoulos 2004)
Note: The listed (English language) scales have demonstrated sufficient reliability and validity in assessing pain in persons with advanced dementia. Though many other scales are available, most require more testing of validity, reliability, and clinical utility in this population. The reader is referred to two systematic reviews of the literature for further discussion of pain scales for nonverbal persons with dementia: Herr, Bjoro, and Decker (2006) and Zwakhalen et al. (2006).
Observation of behavior during gentle mobilization (opening 1 Pain noises hands, stretching arms toward head, stretching and bending 2. Facial expressions 3. Defenses both knees and hips, turning in bed, and sitting).
Mobilization-Observation-BehaviorIntensity-Dementia Pain Scale (MOBID): Development and Validation of a NurseAdministered Pain Assessment Tool for Use in Dementia (Husebo et al. 2007)
Domains
Description
Measurement tool and authors
Table 3.4 Observation-Based Pain Assessment Tools Developed for Advanced Dementia
54
Dementia
The Need-Driven, Dementia-Compromised Behavior (NDB) model (Algase et al. 1996) is a model of care that explains neuropsychiatric symptoms as an expression of needs. The needs may be physical (e.g., pain, hunger, feeling cold) or they may be more psychosocial (e.g., loneliness, fear). The Consequences of Need-Driven, Dementia-Compromised Behavior (C-NDB) theory (Kovach et al. 2005) explains how ineffective caregiver responses to perceived needs may actually increase agitation and behavioral symptoms, resulting in yet more unmet needs. A study of nurses’ responses to behavioral symptoms indicated that repetitive use of ineffective interventions was associated with the most recurrences of problematic behaviors, suggesting a need for more assessment and critical thinking when it comes to responding (Kovach et al. 2006). If an intervention does not reduce the symptom, there may be another source of discomfort or another need that has not been identified, so further assessment and interventions are warranted. Stress Reactivity and Levels of Arousal. The behaviors of persons with advanced dementia may reflect imbalances in arousal and difficulties with self-regulation. The inability to process or regulate sensory, cognitive, social, or affective information may trigger agitation and aggressive behaviors. Likewise, physical, social, or environmental forms of sensory deprivation may result in apathy and withdrawal. Three care models have addressed stress reactivity and arousal levels: the Balancing Arousal Controls Excesses (BACE) model (Kovach et al. 2004), the Stimulation-Retreat model (Lawton et al. 1998), and the Progressive Lower Stress Threshold (PLST) model (Hall and Buckwalter 1987; Smith et al. 2006). Each model advocates using neuropsychiatric symptoms to gauge activity and stimulation levels. Caregivers are responsible for reducing stimuli when behaviors indicate overarousal (agitation, aggressive behaviors), and increasing stimuli when behaviors indicate underarousal (apathy, depression). The Arousal States in Dementia Scale, ranging from sleep to high arousal, was developed by Kovach et al. (2004) to help nursing staff balance the daily arousal levels of persons with advanced dementia. The environments can be modified to be more stimulating by using bright colors and promoting socialization. Environments are less stimulating when they have reduced visual, auditory, and social contact. Step 3. Make sure that contributing causes are all addressed and that basic needs are met, and then decide if a specific additional treatment is needed. Use of Psychotropic Medications. The most recent AAGP Position Statement recommends that, when treating noncognitive neuropsychiatric
Nonpharmacological Approaches
55
symptoms associated with Alzheimer disease, nonpharmacologic interventions be tried prior to medications, unless there is sufficient urgency, distress, disability, or danger risk (Lyketsos et al. 2006). A person-centered, stage-specific treatment plan should take into account that advanced dementia is the final stage of a terminal illness. Geriatric-palliative models of care are appropriate for treating advanced dementia. A geriatricpalliative care approach takes into consideration the life expectancy, time until benefit from medications, goals of care (palliative vs. curative), and treatment targets (symptom management vs. symptom prevention), and attempts to modify medications to discontinue inappropriate medications while adding or increasing medications that may manage treatable symptoms and prevent undue suffering (e.g., pain, depression) (Garfinkel, ZurGil and Ben-Israel 2007; Holmes 2009). Pharmacologic management for neuropsychiatric symptoms will be covered elsewhere in this volume; however, it is clear that some neuropsychiatric symptoms are more appropriately treated with medications than others. Depression, for example, is a disorder that impairs quality of life and reduces a person’s ability to engage in life, and should be treated vigorously if it exists, especially considering that aggressive behaviors may be modifiable consequences of depression (Leonard et al. 2006; Lyketsos et al. 1999; Volicer, VanderSteen, and Frijters 2009). Agitation or aggressive behaviors, however, may be attempts to express needs or resist care attempts and should not automatically be treated with medications. Even psychotic symptoms may represent sensory deprivation and vision loss, inappropriate sensory stimulation, or underlying medical complications, and should not automatically be treated with antipsychotics (Cohen-Mansfield 2003). Psychotropic drugs are associated with significant risks including reduced well-being, increased time spent socially withdrawn, and reduced time engaged in activities (Ballard et al. 2001). Studies of nursing home residents with advanced dementia indicate that 28–65% are taking psychoactive drugs (Koopmans et al. 2009; Zuidema et al. 2009), and the vast majority (78%) of residents with neuropsychiatric symptoms are treated with psychotropics (Kverno, Rabins, et al. 2008). Adverse medication effects such as constipation, sedation, and blurred vision may impair the ability to participate in meaningful activities relevant to quality of life. The use of an evidence-based geriatric-palliative approach provides personcentered, stage-specific medication management that addresses the significant complexities of treating nursing home residents with advanced dementia who have multiple comorbidities and functional impairments. Specific Nonpharmacological Treatments. Nonpharmacological treatments are the treatment of choice for neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced
56
Dementia
dementia. The American Psychiatric Association’s Practice Guideline for the Treatment of Patients with Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias (Rabins and McIntyre 2007) identifies four major categories of treatments, each with specific aims. Cognition-oriented approaches aim to restore cognitive deficits by using strategies such as reality orientation and skills training. Emotionoriented approaches aim to stimulate memory and mood in the context of the patient’s life history. Examples of emotion-oriented approaches include reminiscence, validation, supportive psychotherapy, sensory integration, simulated presence. Behavior-oriented approaches aim to reduce problematic behaviors by identifying the antecedents and then modifying the environment or caregiving strategies to reduce the probability of response. Stimulation-oriented approaches aim to decrease neuropsychiatric symptoms by influencing arousal levels, providing stimulation for enrichment, or reducing stimulation to promote relaxation. Examples of stimulation-oriented approaches include all activity and sensory-based therapies. Person-centered, stage-specific interventions are based upon knowing the preferences and abilities of each resident. Preferences may be gleaned by talking with family members, identifying past interests or hobbies, finding out about preferred types of music, and by observing responses. Inventories such as the CMS-mandated Minimum Data Set (MDS) can also help determine activity preferences. Prescription of stage-specific therapies should also be based upon sound evidence. Unfortunately most dementia intervention studies have not been stage-specific. A recent review (Kverno et al. 2009) of the last ten years of intervention research for treating neuropsychiatric symptoms of advanced dementia with nonpharmacological therapies indicated that out of 215 studies, only 21 specifically studied treatments for persons with moderate to severe cognitive impairment, and only four specifically studied treatments for persons with severe cognitive impairment (MMSE scores of <10). The paucity of stage-specific research in advanced dementia potentially reflects the ethical and logistical complications relevant to including frail elders with advanced dementia in intervention studies. The following section will review the available evidence and present recommendations for caregivers.
EVIDENCE FOR NONPHARMACOLOGICAL TREATMENT OF NEUROPSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMS IN ADVANCED DEMENTIA Cognition-Oriented Therapies There is no evidence that cognition-oriented therapies are useful for reducing neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia. Persons with
Nonpharmacological Approaches
57
advanced dementia no longer have the ability to benefit from languagebased skill training and do not have the memory capacity to benefit from reality orientation. Emotion-Oriented Therapies Persons with advanced dementia demonstrate a desire for social interactions by responding positively to approaching caregivers (Koopmans et al. 2009), and to nonverbal validation of their emotions (Magai, Cohen, and Gomberg 2002). Person-centered emotion-oriented approaches are based on the recognition that feelings persist despite cognitive impairment and that persons continue to have needs for social interactions. Providing emotion-oriented care requires that caregivers learn aspects of the person’s life history and experiences in order to be able to acknowledge them. Family members are best able to reminisce about these past experiences. Simulated presence is the presentation of a personal conversation by audio recording, which may include digital photoframe photographs that accompany the narrative. Garland, Eppingstall, and O’Connor (2007) demonstrated that listening to a family member reminisce on audiotape reduced agitation in comparison with usual care. They also found that an audio recording of a stranger narrating a book on gardening had the same effect. This finding, not altogether surprising given the cognitive and language deficits of advanced dementia, appears to suggest that the social and emotional contact was comforting, whether or not it was recognized as the voice of a family member. Behavior-Oriented Approaches Behavior-oriented approaches require that caregivers identify the antecedents and consequences of problematic behaviors and take steps to produce and reward more positive behaviors. A good example of behavior that appears to have a definitive antecedent is resistiveness or combativeness during care routines. At least half of all aggressive behaviors occur during caregiving activities (Volicer et al. 2007). In a behavioral model, the caregiving would be labeled the “antecedent,” or trigger for the resistive behaviors. How the caregiver responds to the resistance is the “consequence.” Resistiveness to care is most common during morning care routines when caregivers are assisting patients with bathing and dressing (Koopmans et al. 2009). Antecedents may be multiple, including a misunderstanding of the reasons for being touched, feeling cold or uncomfortable, and even a response to infantalizing, disrespectful, communication,
58
Dementia
often referred to as elderspeak (Williams et al. 2009). Without the use of language for communication, persons with advanced dementia may express resistiveness to care verbally (e.g., by screaming or cursing) or physically (e.g., by hitting, grabbing, or scratching). These behaviors are often labeled as aggressive, yet they are a form of communication. Person-centered, stage-specific caregiving methods that have been shown to improve the quality of morning care routines focus more on the personal comfort, preferences, and abilities rather than the task. Sloane et al. (2004) determined patient preferences for bathing by asking family members, modified communication as appropriate for the stage of advanced dementia, and provided warm showers or towel (bed) baths using nonrinse soap. Compared to the usual shower care method, both person-centered approaches resulted in 32–38% reduction in behavioral symptoms and 53–60% reduction in aggressive behaviors compared to the usual care method. The actual increase in time required to perform person-centered care was minimal (approximately 5–6 minutes). Dunn, Thiru-Chelvam, and Beck (2002) describe a towel bathing method that reduced agitation and aggressive behavior in 14/15 residents compared to tub bathing. A nonrinse cleanser was placed in a thermal container with hot water and washcloths and the residents were able to remain warm while being bathed in their own rooms and bed. Stimulation-Oriented Approaches Stimulation-oriented approaches increase or reduce stimulation of the senses. The majority of published nonpharmacological approaches to treating neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia are sensory in nature. Arousing sensory stimulation may be useful for engaging persons when they are withdrawn, depressed, or apathetic. Calming stimuli may be useful for reducing overarousal symptoms such as agitation, aggressive behaviors, and other motor behaviors. Stimulation-oriented approaches that are supported by research evidence are as follows: aromatherapy, music, and some multisensory approaches. In general, the treatments do not have long-lasting effects but show evidence of short-term beneficial effects in reducing neuropsychiatric symptoms. For persons with advanced dementia, stimulation-oriented treatments are stage-appropriate. Aromatherapy. Aromatherapy involves the diffusion of aromatic oils into the environment, or the application of aroma scented lotion to the skin. Two oils have been found to have positive effects on reducing agitation in persons with advanced dementia. Ballard et al. (2002) compared Melissa oil (lemon balm oil) to a placebo oil by applying a 10% oil scented or
Nonpharmacological Approaches
59
unscented (sunflower) lotion directly to the face and arms twice daily for a four-week period. Agitation was improved by 35% in the aromatherapy group compared to 11% in the placebo group. Improvements in agitation were noted in physical nonaggressive agitation (motor restlessness), verbal nonaggression (shouting and screaming), and physically aggressive behaviors. In addition, persons in the aromatherapy condition showed more engagement in constructive activities and less social withdrawal compared to those in the placebo condition. Two studies using lavender oil reported reductions in agitation compared to a placebo condition. Holmes et al. (2002) administered a 2% lavender oil stream into a communal area for a two-hour period alternating with days of a water placebo stream. Of the 15 participants 60% had lower agitation scores during the lavender treatment compared to the placebo treatment, 33% showed no change, and one person actually worsened. In the Lin et al. (2007) study, lavender was administered by diffusers next to the bed at night. The researchers also reported a decrease in agitation (especially agitation or aggression, irritability, and nighttime behavior) during the three weeks of treatment compared to no changes during three weeks of placebo. A third study, with only seven participants, showed no benefits from lavender diffusion (Snow, Hovanec, and Brandt 2004). Altogether the studies do suggest possible benefits in reducing symptoms of agitation, at least for some persons with dementia. Aromatherapy is inexpensive and easy to administer. Music Therapy. Music can be arousing or relaxing. Among persons with advanced dementia, live, interactive, and preferred music have been shown to be beneficial in reducing neuropsychiatric symptoms. Live music produced greater positive engagement in withdrawn persons with advanced dementia than did prerecorded music or silence (Holmes et al. 2006). Interactive familiar live music produced greater reductions in activity disturbances, aggressive behaviors, and anxiety than did usual care (Svansdottir and Snaedal 2006). Preferred music was found to reduce physical agitation by a clinically meaningful extent of 50% or more in approximately half of participating residents (Garland, Eppingstall, and O’Connor 2007). Finally, preferred music resulted in greater reductions in agitation than classical “relaxation” music (Gerdner 2000). These studies suggest that identifying music preferences by talking with knowledgeable family members or friends, and obtaining and playing the recordings for residents during times of agitation or apathy, or at times when resistiveness to care might be expected (e.g., during morning care routines) may be feasible and sensible interventions. In contrast, playing prerecorded music through a loudspeaker system in community rooms may not have any benefits at
60
Dementia
all, or may even exacerbate agitation for some residents (van der Geer et al. 2009). Touch Therapies. Many persons are very sensitive to being touched during bathing and personal care activities, and may find it intrusive or confusing, as evidenced by the high frequency of resistiveness to care. Although it seems intuitive that persons with advanced dementia would be responsive to calming forms of touch such as hand massage or hand holding, there has been very little stage-specific research over the past decade. A very small study (n = 9) using craniosacral therapy demonstrated that an average treatment of five minutes daily for six weeks produced reductions, compared to baseline levels, in physically nonaggressive agitation and verbal agitation that continued throughout a threeweek post-treatment follow-up period (Gerdner, Hart, and Zimmerman 2008). The craniosacral technique used light touch and was accepted by the participants; however, it required a certified therapist to administer it, making it potentially inaccessible in most care homes. Multisensory Approaches. Sensory approaches are clearly routes to communication and treatment of persons with advanced dementia. The research evidence for using auditory and olfactory stimulation is fairly strong. It is less strong for tactile approaches, and limited for visual and taste, yet the lack of evidence appears to be primarily a problem of not having enough participants and enough experimental control to draw firm conclusions. Visual cues such as the use of mirrors and nonverbal gestures are used in routine care. Snacks and drinks, included in a problem-solving needs assessment approach to treating neuropsychiatric symptoms, have occasionally been the crucial treatment (Kovach et al. 2006). Finally, when pain or other needs are driving the behaviors, such as the distressing sensations of wetness or cold, these must be resolved before other sensory approaches will be successful. In practice, we do not generally treat persons with one sensory treatment. Instead, stimulation of the senses is part of a multisensory treatment approach. Therapeutic activities, snoezelen, and Namaste are all examples of multisensory approaches that have been successful in decreasing neuropsychiatric symptoms and improving quality of life for persons with advanced dementia. Therapeutic Activities. Activities have the potential to reduce neuropsychiatric symptoms by providing structure and meaning, and a source of satisfaction and quality of life. Engagement in activities has been associated with the reduction of a variety of neuropsychiatric symptoms, including apathy (Baker et al. 2003; Politis et al. 2004), depression (Meeks et al. 2008), and agitation (Cohen-Mansfield, Libin, and Marx 2007; Kovach et al. 2004). Continuous activity programming during daytime hours has been
Nonpharmacological Approaches
61
shown to increase positive affect and reduce the need for psychotropic medication (Volicer et al. 2006). The U.S. Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services require that nursing homes provide for an ongoing program of activities designed to meet the abilities and preferences of each resident (Department of Health and Human Services 2009). Persons with advanced dementia are capable of active participation in a variety of sensory-based activities (Kovach and Magliocco 1998). Matching therapeutic activities to abilities and preferences is a person-centered, stage-specific approach that is especially important for individuals with advanced dementia because they have lost the ability to initiate activities for themselves. The Comprehensive Process Model of Engagement of Persons with Dementia (CPME-D) predicts that the interactions between stimulus attributes (the type of activity and its sensory qualities) and person or environmental attributes are what determine activity engagement, improved affect, and subsequent reductions in neuropsychiatric symptoms (CohenMansfield, Dakheel-Ali, and Marx 2009). Person attributes include cognitive and physical functioning, and preferences based upon past history and interest. The interaction between person attributes and stimulus attributes may be reflected by the effects of personal preferences on the types of activities that are most engaging. Environmental attributes include the time of day, location of the activity, and other environmental variables such as noise level. The interaction between environmental and stimulus attributes may be reflected in how certain activities such as bathing may not be acceptable if they are offered in a cold unfamiliar room. Engagement is defined as the act of being occupied or involved and affect is the observed outward expression of that engagement. With regard to advanced dementia, engagement may be identified by increases in approach behaviors (e.g., leaning forward, reaching out), physical relaxation, nonverbal signs of connecting (e.g., nodding or turning head toward caregiver, tapping to music), verbal signs (e.g., humming, single words or phrases), and positive affect (Kovach and Magliocco 1998). Findings that support an interaction between person attributes and stimuli attributes suggest that activities that are more meaningful to persons with advanced dementia result in greater engagement than those with less meaning. Persons with advanced dementia preferred familiar work-related activities (e.g., folding, stamping envelopes, sorting cards/jewelry/tools) over manipulative blocks (Cohen-Mansfield, Dakheel-Ali, Thein, and Marx 2009) and familiar materials used in individual Montessori activities over a variety of large group activities (Orsulic-Jeras, Judge, and Camp 2000). Findings that support an interaction between the environment and stimuli attributes demonstrate greater engagement (interest in the environment)
62
Dementia
around mealtimes (Cohen-Mansfield 2007), and greater agitation in the evenings (Cohen-Mansfield 2007), in more crowded living spaces (Morgan and Stewart 1998), and when isolated from staff (Zuidema et al. 2010). As predicted by the CPME-D model, when controlling for level of cognitive impairment, nursing home residents with moderate to severe dementia exhibited significantly greater increases in engagement and affect, and decreased agitation when activities were chosen based upon needs and personal interests (past work, hobbies, current preferences), compared to a control condition (Cohen-Mansfield, Libin, and Marx 2007). Positive affect resulting from engagement in activities may mediate the reduction in neuropsychiatric symptoms. Schreiner et al. (2005) reported that nursing home residents with advanced dementia demonstrated a sevenfold increase in facial expressions of happiness during psychomotor recreational activities (e.g., music with range-of-motion exercises) compared to ordinary time. Furthermore, during the time of positive affect there was a virtual absence of neuropsychiatric symptoms, specifically, agitated, aggressive, or problem behaviors. Some activity stimuli are more engaging than others. Snoezelen (Multisensory Stimulation). Snoezelen, otherwise known as multisensory stimulation (MSS), was originally developed in the Netherlands for persons with sensory and learning disabilities. Snoezelen rooms can be prepared with a variety of unpatterned visual, auditory, olfactory, and tactile stimuli (e.g., mirror balls, sounds of nature, aromatherapy, and interesting textures to touch and manipulate). The goals of snoezelen include the creation of a safe, pleasurable and novel sensory experience as well as the promotion of mental and physical relaxation. In a study comparing the effects of a traditional activity (e.g., playing cards and puzzle games) to MSS, Baker et al. (2003), found that persons with advanced dementia showed a reduction in apathy in the MSS condition compared to persons with more moderate levels of cognitive impairment who showed greater apathy during MSS. These findings emphasize the importance of stage-specific activities for treating neuropsychiatric symptoms. Person-centered multisensory approaches can be integrated into morning care activities to reduce resistiveness to care. A randomized controlled study (van Weert et al. 2005) of 194 residents with moderate to advanced dementia in Dutch nursing homes compared usual care to patientcentered MSS care. With the backing of administration in the nursing homes, the nurses and certified nursing assistants (CNAs) in the experimental groups were trained to integrate MSS into 24-hour care. A qualified trainer provided the CNAs with approximately 16 hours of training and homework. The first step to implementing the program was the completion of a detailed history of each resident participant’s life and preferences
Nonpharmacological Approaches
63
as obtained from family members. In addition, each resident participant completed a preference screening of various sensory stimuli (tactile, visual, auditory, olfactory, and gustatory). Based upon the evaluation information, the CNAs wrote MSS treatment plans describing how to integrate sensory preferences into activities of daily living (e.g., whether the resident liked to be touched, whether the resident liked music or aroma). During personal morning care, a resident might be given the soap (of personal preference) to smell, music (of personal preference) to hear, while the CNA might direct the vision to the colors of clothing or the scenes outside the window. The results of the study showed that, compared to usual care, residents who received MSS during morning care had greater reductions in neuropsychiatric symptoms (less physical aggression, rebellion, apathy, and depression), more positive mood states (happiness and enjoyment), and more interaction with the CNAs. In addition, the CNAs who were trained in person-centered multisensory care continued to exhibit improved person-centered care at approximately three months after full implementation (van Weert et al. 2006). Namaste. Continuous activity programming during daytime hours has been shown to increase positive affect and reduce the need for psychotropic medication (Volicer et al. 2006). Namaste, a Hindu term that means honoring the spirit within, describes a multisensory activity approach for persons with dementia who are nearing the end of life. Persons who may benefit from Namaste care are those with severe cognitive impairment (MMSE <7) and an inability to participate in scheduled activity programs, are nonambulatory, have total dependence in activities of daily living, and have difficulty with communication (Simard 2007). A Namaste care room is a tranquil place where families and caregivers are surrounded by calming sensory stimulation. After cleaning and applying the resident’s glasses, visual senses are stimulated by such events as visits from caregivers, family, and friends, and puppet shows. After checking and applying hearing aids, auditory senses are stimulated by natural sounds (e.g., birds chirping) or soft music. The sense of touch is stimulated by soft clothing and quilts, and gentle caregiving (e.g., applying lotion to hands, passive range of motion in rhythm to music). The sense of smell is stimulated by aromatherapies of lavender or seasonal smells or spices). The sense of taste is stimulated by offering a variety of easily swallowed healthy treats (e.g., yogurt, smoothies, orange slices) and water. Activities focus on gentle caregiving. The evidence for the success of Namaste care is minimal because it is a relatively new concept and program and there has only been one study of the effects on neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia. Simard and Volicer (2010) reported on a sample of 86 nursing
64
Dementia
home residents who participated in a Namaste care program. Data were collected before and 30 days after enrollment in the program. Residents who had apathy or withdrawal at pre-enrollment showed increased social engagement at 30 days. In addition, there was a significant reduction in the use of antianxiety medications over the 30 day period (11 residents at onset and 6 after enrollment). SUMMARY Person-centered, stage-specific treatments of neuropsychiatric symptoms of advanced dementia require knowledge of the patient, accurate assessment skills to identify and differentiate neuropsychiatric symptoms, the flexibility to use a problem-solving approach to identify the meaning of the symptoms, and the consistent delivery of treatment approaches that are individualized. Based on the available evidence, individualized, multisensory-based nonpharmacological approaches are first-line treatments for the neuropsychiatric symptoms of advanced dementia, provided within the context of a socially and emotionally validating environment. Adjusting behavioral caregiving routines to include multisensory stimuli, such as music and aromas, may lessen resistance to care. Of course, more clinical research is needed, especially for new approaches like Namaste, and for methods of combining geriatric-palliative methods of reducing unnecessary medications while using non-pharmacological approaches to treating neuropsychiatric symptoms. Making a culture change in long-term care facilities from institutional practices focused on safety, uniformity, and medical issues toward person-centered care and quality of life will necessarily require continued leadership to redesign the environment, retrain the workforce, implement the process, and change our measures for evaluating outcomes. Initial examinations of the cost of shifting to person-centered care suggest that the cost does not need to be greater than that of usual care (Chenoweth et al. 2009). Most importantly, successful treatment of neuropsychiatric symptoms increases comfort, supports dignity, and improves quality of life. REFERENCES Alexopoulos, G. S., R. C. Abrams, R. C. Young, and C. A. Shamoian. 1988. Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. Biological Psychiatry 23: 271–284. Algase, D. L., et al. 1996. Need-driven dementia-compromised behavior: An alternative view of disruptive behavior. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease 11: 12–19.
Nonpharmacological Approaches
65
Baker, R., et al. 2003. Effects of multi-sensory stimulation for people with dementia. Journal of Advanced Nursing 43: 465–477. Ballard, C., et al. 2001. Quality of life for people with dementia living in residential and nursing home care: The impact of performance on activities of daily living, behavioral and psychological symptoms, language skills and psychotropic drugs. International Psychogeriatrics 13: 93–106. Ballard, C. G., J. T. O’Brien, K. Reichelt, and E. K. Perry. 2002. Aromatherapy as a safe and effective treatment for the management of agitation in severe dementia: The results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial with Melissa. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63: 553–558. Ballard, C. G., and J. Waite. 2006. The effectiveness of atypical antipsychotics for the treatment of aggression and psychosis in Alzheimer ’s disease. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews CD003476. Black, B. S., T. Finucane, A. Baker, and D. Loreck. 2006. Health problems and correlates of pain in nursing home residents with advanced dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 20: 283–290. Blass, D. M., et al. 2008. Medication use in nursing home residents with advanced dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23: 490–496. Bradford Dementia Group. 1997. Evaluating dementia care: The DCM method. 7th ed. Bradford, UK: University of Bradford. Buettner, L. L., and S. Fitzsimmons. 2003. Activity calendars for older adults with dementia: What you see is not what you get. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias 18: 215–226. Carson, S., M. McDonagh, and K. Peterson. 2006. A systematic review of the efficacy and safety of atypical antipsychotics in patients with psychological and behavioral symptoms of dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 54: 354–361. Chan, D.-C., J. D. Kasper, B. S. Black, and P. V. Rabins. 2003. Presence of behavioral and psychological symptoms predicts nursing home placement in community-dwelling elders with cognitive impairment in univariate but not multivariate analysis. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 58: M548–M554. Chenoweth, L., et al. 2009. Caring for Aged Dementia Care Resident Study (CADRES) of person-centered care, dementia-care mapping, and usual care in dementia: A cluster-randomised trial. Lancet Neurology 8: 317–325. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2003. Nonpharmacologic interventions for psychotic symptoms in dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 16: 219–224. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2007. Temporal patterns of agitation in dementia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 15: 395–405. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. Dakheel-Ali, and M. S. Marx. 2009. Engagement in persons with dementia: The concept and its measurement. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 299–307. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. Dakheel-Ali, K. Thein, and M. S. Marx. 2009. The impact of stimulus attributes on engagement of nursing home residents with dementia. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 49: 1–6.
66
Dementia
Cohen-Mansfield, J., A. Libin, and M. Marx. 2007. Non-pharmacological treatment of agitation: A controlled trial of systematic individualized intervention. Journal of Gerontology 62A: 908–916. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, and A. S. Rosenthal. 1989. A description of agitation in a nursing home. Journal of Gerontology 44: 77–86. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, and P. Werner. 1992. Observational data on time use and behavior problems in the nursing home. Journal of Applied Gerontology 11: 111–121. Cummings, J. L., M. Mega, K. Gray, S. Rosenberg-Thompson, D. A. Carusi, and J. Gornbein. 1994. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44: 2308–2314. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. 2009. CMS Manual System. Revisions to Appendix PP, Guidance to Surveyors for Long Term Care Facilities. TagF241-242, §483.15 (a&b). http://www.cms.gov/transmittals/downloads/R48SOMA.pdf. Dunn, J. C., B. Thiru-Chelvam, and C. H. M. Beck. 2002. Bathing: Pleasure or pain? Nontraditional methods of bathing may prove less agitating for individuals with dementia. This study examines a modified thermal bath option. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 28: 6–13. Ettema, T. P., R.-M. Droes, J. de Lange, G. J. Mellenbergh, and M. W. Ribbe. 2007. QUALIDEM: Development and evaluation of a dementia specific quality of life instrument. Scalability, reliability and internal structure. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22: 549–556. Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R McHugh. 1975. Mini-Mental State. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12: 189–198. Fuchs-Lacelle, S., and T. Hadjistavropoulos. 2004. Development and preliminary validation of the Pain Assessment Checklist for Seniors with Limited Ability to Communicate (PACSLAC). Pain Management Nursing 5: 37–49. Fuchs-Lacelle, S., T. Hadjistavropoulos, and L. Lix. 2008. Pain assessment as an intervention. A study of older adults with severe dementia. Clinical Journal of Pain 24: 697–707. Garfinkel, D., S. Zur-Gil, and J. Ben-Israel. 2007. The war against polypharmacy: A new cost-effective geriatric-palliative approach for improving drug therapy in disabled elderly people. Israel Medical Association Journal 9: 430–434. Garland, K., B. Eppingstall, and D. W. O’Connor. 2007. A comparison of two treatments of agitated behavior in nursing home residents with dementia: Simulated family presence and preferred music. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 15: 514–521. Gaugler, J. E., S. Duval, K. A. Anderson, and R. L. Kane. 2007. Predicting nursing home admission in the U.S.: A meta-analysis. BMC Geriatrics 19: 7–13. Gaugler, J. E., R. L. Kane, R. A. Kane, and R. Newcomer. 2005. Unmet care needs and key outcomes in dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 2098–2105.
Nonpharmacological Approaches
67
Gaugler, J. E., F. Yu, K. Krichbaum, and J. F. Wyman. 2009. Predictors of nursing home admission for persons with dementia. Medical Care 47: 191–198. Gerdner, L. A. 2000. Effects of individualized versus classical relaxation music on the frequency of agitation in elderly persons with Alzheimer ’s disease and related disorders. International Psychogeriatrics 12: 49–65. Gerdner, L. A., L. K. Hart, and M. B. Zimmerman. 2008. Craniosacral still point technique. Exploring its effects in individuals with dementia. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 34: 36–45. Gill, S. S., et al. 2007. Antipsychotic drug use and mortality in older adults with dementia. Annals of Internal Medicine 146: 775–786. Gruneir, A., K. L. Lapane, S. C. Miller, and V. Mor. 2008. Is dementia special care really special? A new look at an old question. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56: 199–205. Hall, G. R., and K. C. Buckwalter. 1987. Progressively lowered stress threshold: A conceptual model for care of adults with Alzheimer ’s disease. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 1: 399–406. Harrell, L. E., D. Marson, A. Chatterjee, and J. A. Parrish. 2000. The Severe MiniMental State Examination: A new neuropsychologic instrument for the bedside assessment of severely impaired patients with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 14: 168–175. Herr, K., K. Bjoro, and S. Decker. 2006. Tools for assessment of pain in nonverbal older adults with dementia. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 31: 170–192. Holmes, C., V. Hopkins, C. Hensford, V. MacLaughlin, D. Wilkinson, and H. Rosenvinge. 2002. Lavender oil as a treatment for agitated behaviour in severe dementia: A placebo controlled study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17: 305–308. Holmes, C., A. Knights, C. Dean, S. Hodkinson, and V. Hopkins. 2006. Keep music live: Music and the alleviation of apathy in dementia subjects. International Psychogeriatrics 18: 623–630. Holmes, H. M. 2009. Rational prescribing for patients with a reduced life expectancy. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 85: 103–107. Hughes, C. P., L. Berg, W L.. Danziger, L. A. Coben, and R. L. Martin. 1982. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry: Journal of Mental Science 140: 566–572. Husebo, B. S., L. I. Strand, R. Moe-Nilssen, S. B. Husebo, A. L. Snow, and A. E. Ljunggren. 2007. Mobilization-Observation-Behavior-IntensityDementia Pain Scale (MOBID): Development and validation of a nurseadministered pain assessment tool for use in dementia. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 34: 67–80. Ice, G. H. 2002. Daily life in a nursing home. Has it changed in 25 years? Journal of Aging Studies 16: 345–359. Kasper, J. D., B. S. Black, A. D. Shore, and P. V. Rabins. 2009. Evaluation of the validity and reliability of the Alzheimer Disease-related Quality of Life Assessment Instrument. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 23: 275–284.
68
Dementia
Kaup, B. A., et al. 2007. Dementia and its relationship to function and medical status, by dementia status, in nursing home admissions. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 15: 438–442. Koopmans, R. T., M. van der Molen, M. Raats, and T. P. Ettema. 2009. Neuropsychiatric symptoms and quality of life in patients in the final phase of dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24: 25–32. Koren, M. J. 2010. Person-centered care for nursing home residents: The culturechange movement. Health Affairs 29: 1–6. Kovach, C. R., S. T. Kelber, M. Simpson, and T. Wells. 2006. Behaviors of nursing home residents with dementia. Examing nurse responses. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 32: 13–21. Kovach, C. R., and J. S. Magliocco. 1998. Late-stage dementia and participation in therapeutic activities. Applied Nursing Research 11: 167–173. Kovach, C. R., P. E. Noonan, A. M. Schlidt, and T. Wells. 2005. A model of consequences of need-driven, dementia-compromised behavior. Journal of Nursing Scholarship 37: 134–140. Kovach, C. R., Y. Taneli, P. Dohearty, A. M. Schlidt, S. Cashin, and A. L. SilvaSmith. 2004. Effect of the BACE intervention on agitation of people with dementia. Gerontologist 44: 797–806. Kverno, K. S., B. S. Black, D. M. Blass, J. Geiger-Brown, and P. V. Rabins. 2008. Neuropsychiatric symptom patterns in hospice-eligible nursing home residents with advanced dementia. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 9: 509–515. Kverno, K. S., B. S. Black, M. T. Nolan, and P. V. Rabins. 2009. Research on treating neuropsychiatric symptoms of advanced dementia with non-pharmacological strategies, 1998–2008: A systematic literature review. International Psychogeriatrics 21: 825–843. Kverno, K. S., P. V. Rabins, D. M. Blass, K. L. Hicks, and B. S. Black. 2008. Prevalence and treatment of neuropsychiatric symptoms in advanced dementia. Journal of Gerontological Nursing 34: 8–15. Lawlor, B., and S. N. Bhriain. 2001. Psychosis and behavioural symptoms of dementia: Defining the role of neuroleptic interventions. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16: S2–S6. Lawton, M. P., K. Van Haitsma, and J. Klapper. 1996. Observed affect in nursing home residents with Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 51: P3–14. Lawton, M. P., K. Van Haitsma, J. Klapper, M. H. Kleban, I. R. Katz, and J. Corn. 1998. A stimulation-retreat special care unit for elders with dementing illness. International Psychogeriatrics 10: 379–395. Leonard, R., M. E. Tinetti, H. G. Allore, and M. A. Drickamer. 2006. Potentially modifiable resident characteristics that are associated with physical or verbal aggression among nursing home residents with dementia. Archives of Internal Medicine 166: 1295–1300. Lin, P. W., W. Chan, B. F. Ng, and L. C. Lam. 2007. Efficacy of aromatherapy (lavandula angustifolia) as an intervention for agitated behaviours in Chinese older
Nonpharmacological Approaches
69
persons with dementia: A cross-over randomized trial. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22: 405–407. Lyketsos, C. G. 2007. Neuropsychiatric symptoms (behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia) and the development of dementia treatments. International Psychogeriatrics 19: 409–420. Lyketsos, C. G., et al. 1999. Physical aggression in dementia patients and its relationship to depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 156: 66–71. Lyketsos, C. G., et al. 2006. Position statement of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry regarding principles of care for patients with dementia resulting from Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14: 561–573. Lyketsos, C. G., O. Lopez, B. Jones, A. L. Fitzpatrick, J. Breitner, and S. DeKosky. 2002. Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment. JAMA 288: 1475–1483. Lyketsos, C. G., M. Steinberg, J. T. Tschanz, M. C. Norton, D. C. Steffens, and J. C. S. Breitner. 2000. Mental and behavioral disturbances in dementia: Findings from the Cache County Study on Memory in Aging. American Journal of Psychiatry 157: 708–714. Magai, C., C. I. Cohen, and D. Gomberg. 2002. Impact of training dementia caregivers in sensitivity to non-verbal emotion signals. International Psychogeriatrics 14: 25–38. Meeks, S., S. W. Looney, K. Van Haitsma, and L. Teri. 2008. BE-ACTIV: A staffassisted behavioral intervention for depression in nursing homes. Gerontologist 48: 105–114. Mitchell, S. L., et al. 2009. The clinical course of advanced dementia. New England Journal of Medicine 361: 1529–1538. Morgan, D., and N. J. Stewart. 1998. High versus low density special care units: Impact on the behaviour of elderly residents with dementia. Canadian Journal of Aging 17: 143–165. Orsulic-Jeras, S., K. S. Judge, and C. J. Camp. 2000. Montessori-based activities for long-term care residents with advanced dementia: Effects on engagement and affect. Gerontologist 40: 107–111. Panisset, M., M. Roudier, J. Saxton, and F. Boller. 1994. Severe Impairment Battery. A neuropsychological test for severely demented patients. Archives of Neurology 51: 41–45. Pelletier, I. C., and P. Landreville. 2007. Discomfort and agitation in older adults with dementia. BMC Geriatrics 7. http://www.biomedcentral.com/14712318/7/27. Politis, A. M., S. Vozzella, L. S. Mayer, C. U. Onyike, A. Baker, and C. G. Lyketsos. 2004. A randomized, controlled, clinical trial of activity therapy for apathy in patients with dementia residing in long-term care. International Journal of Geriatriatric Psychiatry 19: 1087–1094. Rabins, P. V., and C. D. Steele. 1996. A scale to measure impairment in severe dementia and similar conditions. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 4: 247–251.
70
Dementia
Rabins, P. V., and J. S. McIntyre, Work Group on Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Guidelines, Steering Committee on Practice. 2007. Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with Alzheimer ’s disease and other dementias. 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Reisberg, B., J. Borenstein, S. P. Salob, S. H. Ferris, and E. Franssen. 1987. Behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease: Phenomenology and treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 48: 9–15. Reisberg, B., S. H. Ferris, M. J. de Leon, and T. Crook. 1982. The Global Deterioration Scale for Assessment of Primary Degenerative Dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry 139: 1136–1139. Rosen, J., et al. 1994. The Pittsburgh Agitation Scale: A user-friendly instrument for rating agitation in dementia patients. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 2: 52–59. Salzman, C., et al. 2008. Elderly patients with dementia-related symptoms of severe agitation and aggression: Consensus statement on treatment options, clinical trials methodology, and policy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69: 889–898. Samus, Q., et al. 2005. The association of neuropsychiatric symptoms and environment with quality of life in assisted living residents with dementia. Gerontologist 45 (Special issue 1): 19–26. Saxton, J., et al. 2005. Development of a short form of the Severe Impairment Battery. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 13: 999–1005. Schneider, L. S., K. S. Dagerman, and P. Insel. 2005. Risk of death with atypical antipsychotic drug treatment for dementia: Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. JAMA 294: 1934–1943. Schreiner, A. S., E. Yamamoto, and H. Shiotani. 2005. Positive affect among nursing home residents with Alzheimer ’s dementia: The effect of recreational activity. Aging and Mental Health 9: 129–134. Sclan, S. G., and B. Reisberg. 1992. Functional Assessment Staging (FAST) in Alzheimer ’s disease: Reliability, validity, and ordinality. International Psychogeriatrics 4 (Suppl. 1): 55–69. Sclan, S. G., A. Saillon, E. Franssen, L. Hugonot-Diener, A. Scaillon, and B. Reisberg. 1996. The Behavior Pathology in Alzheimer ’s Disease Rating Scale (BEHAVE-AD): Reliability and analysis of symptom category scores. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 11: 819–830. Simard, J. 2007. Silent and invisible nursing home residents with advanced dementia. Journal of Nutrition, Health, and Aging 11: 484–488. Simard, J., and L. Volicer. 2010. Effects of Namaste care on residents who do not benefit from usual activities. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s and Other Dementias 25: 46–50. Sloane, P. D., et al. 2004. Effect of person-centered showering and the towel bath on bathing-associated aggression, agitation, and discomfort in nursing home residents with dementia: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 52: 1795–1804.
Nonpharmacological Approaches
71
Smith, M., G. H. Richards, L. Gerdner, and K. C. Buckwalter. 2006. Application of the progressively lowered stress threshold model across the continuum of care. Nursing Clinics of North America 41: 57–81. Snow, A. L., L. Hovanec, and J. Brandt. 2004. A controlled trial of aromatherapy for agitation in nursing home patients with dementia. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 10: 431–437. Svansdottir, H. B., and J. Snaedal. 2006. Music therapy in moderate and severe dementia of Alzheimer ’s type: A case-control study. International Psychogeriatrics 18: 613–621. van der Geer, E. R., A. C. Vink, J. M. G. A. Schols, and J. P. J. Slaets. 2009. Music in the nursing home: Hitting the right note! The provision of music to dementia patients with verbal and vocal agiation in Dutch nursing homes. International Psychogeriatrics 21: 86–93. van Weert, J. C. M., et al. 2006. Nursing assistants’ behaviour during morning care: Effects of the implementation of snoezelen, integrated in 24-hour dementia care. Journal of Advanced Nursing 53: 656–668. van Weert, J. C. M., A. M. van Dulmen, P. M. M. Spreeuwenberg, W. R. Miel, and J. M. Bensing. 2005. Behavioral and mood effects of snoezelen integrated into 24-hour dementia care. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53: 24–33. Volicer, L., E. A. Bass, and S. L. Luther. 2007. Agitation and resistiveness to care are two separate behavioral syndromes of dementia. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 8: 527–532. Volicer, L., L. Camberg, A. C. Hurley, J. Ashley, P. Woods, W. L. Ooi, and K. McIntyre. 1999. Dimensions of decreased psychological well-being in advanced dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 13: 192–201. Volicer, L., A. C. Hurley, and Z. V. Blasi. 2003. Characteristics of dementia end-oflife care across care settings. American Journal of Hospice and Palliative Care 20: 191–200. Volicer, L., J. Simard, J. H. Pupa, R. Medrek, and M. E. Riordan. 2006. Effects of continuous activity programming on behavioral symptoms of dementia. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 7: 426–431. Volicer, L., J. T. VanderSteen, and D. H. M. Frijters. 2009. Modifiable factors related to abusive behaviors in nursing home residents with dementia. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 10: 617–622. Warden, V., A. C. Hurley, and L. Volicer. 2003. Development and psychometric evaluation of the Pain Assessment in Advanced Dementia (PAINAD) scale. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 4: 9–15. Weiner, M. F., K. Martin-Cook, D. A. Svetlik, K. Saine, B. Foster, and C. S. Fontaine. 2000. Quality of Life in Late Stage Dementia (QUALID) scale. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 1: 114–116. White-Chu, E. F., W. J. Graves, S. M. Godfrey, A. Bonner, and P. Sloane. 2009. Beyond the medical model: The culture change revolution in long-term care. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association 10: 370–378.
72
Dementia
Wilkinson, I. M., and J. Graham-White. 1980. Psychogeriatric Dependency Rating Scales (PGDRS): A method of assessment for use by nurses. British Journal of Psychiatry 137: 558–565. Williams, K. N., R. Herman, B. Gajewski, and K. Wilson. 2009. Elderspeak Communication: Impact on dementia care. American Journal of Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias 24: 11–20. Yaffe, K., et al. 2002. Patient and caregiver characteristics and nursing home placement in patients with dementia. JAMA 287: 2090–2097. Zuidema, S. U., J. F. M. de Jonghe, F. R. J. Verhey, and R. T. C. M. Koopmans. 2007. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in nursing home patients: Factor structure invariance of the Dutch nursing home version of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory in different stages of dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 24: 169–176. Zuidema, S. U., J. F. M. de Jonghe, F. R. J. Verhey, and R. T. C. M. Koopmans. 2009. Predictors of neuropsychiatric symptoms in nursing home patients: Influence of gender and dementia severity. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24: 1079–1086. Zuidema, S. U., J. F. M. de Jonghe, F. R. J. Verhey, and R. T. C. M. Koopmans. 2010. Environmental correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in nursing home patients with dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 25: 14–22. Zwakhalen, S. M. G., J. P. H. Hamers, H. H. Abu-Saad, and M. P. F. Berger. 2006. Pain in elderly people with severe dementia: A systematic review of behavioural pain assessment tools. BMC Geriatrics 6. http://www.biomedcentral .com/1471-2318/6/3.
Chapter 4
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia Pamela Lindsey
When a patient is diagnosed with dementia, the symptoms that tend to receive greatest attention are the changes in cognition, primarily the inevitable and irreversible deterioration in memory. A variety of other cognitive changes also eventually result, including trouble performing calculations, problems with word finding, difficulty with routine tasks such as following a recipe or balancing a checkbook, alterations in judgment, or changes in grooming habits. In more recent years, it has become increasingly apparent that persons with dementia also often experience significant noncognitive changes. These symptoms were first depicted in a case description by Dr. Alois Alzheimer (Alzheimer 1906). He described disruptive and troubling behavioral symptoms in a patient with dementia, which included paranoia, delusions of sexual abuse, hallucinations, and screaming. Despite this poignant portrayal, attention to the noncognitive symptoms of dementia did not begin until the 1980s when it became increasingly clear that they had a tremendous impact on the patient and caregiver. Although noncognitive changes began receiving increased attention in the literature, they were often labeled generally as “agitation” and later became referred to as “behavioral disturbances.” A Consensus Conference on Behavioral Disturbances of Dementia was held in 1996 by the International Psychogeriatric Association (IPA). The purposes of the convention were to review current knowledge about behavioral disturbances, to define and describe symptoms, to identify
74
Dementia
causes, and to outline areas for further research. In 1999 the IPA Update Consensus Conference provided the following statement: “The term behavioral disturbances should be replaced by the term behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD), defined as: Symptoms of disturbed perception, thought content, mood or behavior that frequently occur in patients with dementia” (IPA 2002). The consensus panel also determined that it might be helpful to group symptoms into two clusters: behavioral symptoms and psychological symptoms. Thus, the symptoms previously referred to as agitation or behavioral disturbances came to be known as behavior and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD). The symptoms of BPSD tend to have more impact on quality of life for the patient and the caregiver than does the presence of cognitive changes. BPSD often leads to earlier hospitalization and institutionalization (nursing home placement) because the symptoms are more difficult to care for and cope with by the caregiver (Gitlin et al. 2007). Managing behavioral and psychological symptoms can also be challenging for healthcare providers; however, BPSD is often treatable and generally responds better to interventions than the cognitive symptoms of dementia. Unlike the cognitive symptoms of dementia, which progressively worsen during the course of the disease, BPSD tend to vary over time. In addition, there are many benefits to treatment of BPSD including relief of caregiver burden, relief of patient suffering, and decrease in healthcare costs. BEHAVIORAL SYMPTOMS OF BPSD Behavioral symptoms of BPSD are typically those that can be observed, while psychological symptoms are identified based on interviews with the patient and caregivers. Behavioral symptoms include wandering, agitation, catastrophic reactions, complaining, disinhibition, intrusiveness, and negativism. The following provides a more detailed description of these symptoms. Wandering includes a variety of different behaviors. The patient may repeatedly check the location of the caregiver or another person. The patient may follow the caregiver around excessively. He or she may try unsuccessfully to complete household or gardening tasks. The person might walk aimlessly, excessively, and/or walk during the night. He or she also may try to leave the house or wander away, needing to be retrieved. Agitation is characterized by inappropriate verbal, vocal, or physical activity. The Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI), a rating instrument often used to assess agitation in clinical settings, identifies three subtypes of agitation (Cohen-Mansfield 1996). These include physically
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
75
aggressive behaviors (hitting, pushing, kicking, biting, scratching, spitting, grabbing others or things, verbal aggression), physically nonaggressive behavior (handling things inappropriately, hiding things, dressing or undressing inappropriately, pacing, repeating mannerisms or sentences, restlessness, or trying to go to another place), verbally agitated behaviors (complaining, whining, constantly requesting attention, negativity, repeated questions, screaming). Catastrophic or rage reactions are manifested by excessive or abrupt emotional responses or physical behavior. The person may show one or more of the following behaviors: abrupt angry outbursts, verbal aggression, threats of physical aggression, and/or physical aggression. Catastrophic reactions can be triggered by delusions, hallucinations, or other alterations in perceptions. Delirium, pain, infection, and some medications can also precipitate a rage reaction. Complaining in patients with dementia is often characterized by repeated criticism or accusatory statements that are often directed toward the caregiver. In contrast and also troubling, the patient may be unable to complain or to communicate regarding his or her experience of physical pain or discomfort. Disinhibition refers to situations in which the patient with dementia behaves in an impulsive and inappropriate manner. Patients may become easily distracted, emotionally unstable, and demonstrate a lack of insight or judgment. Other related behaviors include crying, euphoria, verbal aggression, physical aggression, self harm, sexual disinhibition, agitation, intrusiveness, impulsiveness, and wandering. Disinhibition can also involve unprecedented shoplifting, gambling, impulsive buying, or excessive alcohol or drug use. Intrusiveness is characterized by the patient being demanding, impatient, or pressuring the caregiver to do something the caregiver would not ordinarily do. The patient may invade situations where he or she is uninvited or latch on to a possession that belongs to another. Negativism refers to situations in which the patient with dementia refuses to cooperate. This behavior often leads to stubbornness and resistance with activities such as assistance with feeding or bathing. PSYCHOLOGICAL SYMPTOMS OF BPSD Psychological symptoms of BPSD include changes in mood (apathy, depression), anxiety, or changes in thought processes and psychosis (altered perceptions of reality) including delusions, delusional misidentifications, and/or hallucinations.
76
Dementia
Patients experiencing apathy demonstrate a lack of interest in usual activities of daily living and grooming, and a reduction in socialization and emotional expression. They may lack motivation and initiative. Up to 50% of patients demonstrate apathy in the early and middle stages of dementia. It is sometime difficult to differentiate apathy from depression (IPA 2002). Both involve a lack of motivation but apathy does not include dysphasia or vegetative symptoms that are apparent in depression. Depression can also be difficult to diagnose in patients with dementia, especially as the dementia progresses into the middle and late stages. Early symptoms may include sudden changes in cognition, decreased appetite, worsening of mood, change in sleep (usually sleeping more or hyper somnolence), withdrawal, decreased energy and activity, crying, references to death and dying, increased irritability, psychosis, or other abrupt changes in behavior. As the dementia progresses, symptoms may be difficult to elicit during assessment and the patient has more difficulty with language and communication. Other symptoms that are common to dementing illnesses (e.g., apathy, weight loss, sleep disturbance, and agitation) make diagnosis of depression more challenging because these symptoms may also be present as part of depression. Symptoms that distinguish a diagnosis of depression are persistent depressed mood, anhedonia (loss of pleasure), talk of death or dying, and a history of depression with the patient or family. Family members are often the first to recognize symptoms of depression. Patients with dementia will manifest anxiety by voicing worry about circumstances that previously were not of great concern to them. The patient will begin to be troubled about finances, health, and the future, as well as worrying about activities they previously did not find distressing. A common form of dementia related anxiety is referred to as “Godot syndrome” and is characterized by repeated questions and worry about an upcoming event. As their cognitive abilities decline, particularly memory, these worries can become relentless, resulting in great stress on the patient’s family and other caregivers. Patients with dementia can also develop phobias, which are characterized by fears that are out of proportion with any real threat or danger. Such phobias include fears of being alone, crowds, travel, dark, and bathing. Delusions refer to false fixed beliefs that are not based on reality. Several different types of delusions may occur in patients with dementia including delusions of theft (people are stealing their belongings), paranoid delusions, delusions of jealousy or that their spouse has been unfaithful, hypochondria (belief that one is or will become ill despite the lack of medical evidence or reassurance), and grandiosity (exaggerated belief of self-importance). Some research has shown that delusions are a risk factor for aggression.
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
77
Patients may also experience misidentification delusions including misidentification of a spouse or family member, self misidentification, or house misidentification. In cases of misidentification, the patient falsely believes that the identity of a person, place, or object has been changed or altered resulting in the patient being unable to recognize them. The person may be unable to recognize him- or herself when looking in the mirror or may not recognize another known person such as a family member. They also may not recognize a familiar place (e.g., their own home) or an event on television (believing it is happening in real time/space). Ellis and Young (1990) identified three forms of delusional misidentifications: Capgras syndrome (belief that others have been replaced by an identical double), Fregoli syndrome (belief that others are disguising themselves to try to trick or influence them), and Intermetamorphosis (belief that a person’s physical appearance corresponds with that of another person). Hallucinations are alterations in the patient’s perception of the environment based on one of the five senses including visual, auditory, gustatory (taste), olfactory (smell), and tactile (touch) hallucinations. The most common type of hallucination in patients with dementia is visual followed by auditory, but patients may experience other types of hallucinations. The most common visual hallucination is called “phantom boarders” in which the patient sees people in their home that are not actually there. Patients with dementia, who also have problems with their vision or hearing, may be at higher risk for experiencing hallucinations; thus it is imperative that careful assessment be completed. PREVALENCE OF BPSD IN DEMENTIA It is estimated that over 80% of patients with dementia experience BPSD: 60% experience delusions, 20% have hallucinations, 33% have verbal outbursts, 35% experience anxiety, and 40% have affective (mood) symptoms (IPA 2002). Physical aggression toward the caregiver, which is often the key factor precipitating nursing home placement, is estimated to occur in about 13% of patients with dementia. However, higher rates have been reported such as in a study that specifically examined the prevalence of physical aggression toward caregivers in 198 dementia patients (O’Leary, Jyringi, and Sedler 2005). Results revealed that 25% of patients demonstrated physical aggression against their caregivers. In this study, those patients with a history of conduct disorder were more likely to demonstrate physical aggression against their partner, indicating this may be a risk factor. In addition, physical aggression was more frequent in the middle stage of dementia (34%) than in early stage (4%). Those patients with
78
Dementia
delusions and paranoia demonstrated higher rates of physical aggression. The prevalence of BPSD in nursing home patients is estimated to range from 50% to 90% (IPA 2002). BPSD symptoms vary at different stages of dementia. Evidence suggests that the symptoms either increase as the dementia progresses or occur with increased frequency during specific stages of the disorder. The prevalence rates of agitation are estimated between 67.5% and 82% (Gerdner, Buckwalter, and Hall 2005). Research results are mixed regarding the prevalence of BPSD in the various types of dementia. Some evidence suggests there is no difference in the prevalence of BPSD in Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) and vascular dementia (VaD), while other researchers have reported higher rates of delusions in AD and higher rates of depression in VaD (IPA 2002). One study by Cohen et al. (1993) found that patients with mixed AD and VaD had the highest rates of psychiatric symptoms. In a review of 55 research studies, it was surmised that 43% of patients with AD experience psychotic symptoms sometime during the course of the illness. Delusions were more commonly experienced than hallucinations. Symptoms appear to increase during the first three years of observation and the majority are reported to occur primarily over a series of several months but usually no longer than one to two years. The presence of psychotic symptoms was significantly associated with faster rate of cognitive decline (Ropacki and Jeste 2005). Symptoms of agitation, aggression, and wandering are reported in at least 75% of patients with AD (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Patients with dementia with Lewy bodies have higher rates (80%) of visual hallucinations than patients with AD (20%) (IPA 2002). Patients with frontotemporal dementia have higher rates of many BPSD symptoms including impulsivity, compulsive behaviors, hypersexuality, and verbal outbursts. Those patients with left temporal involvement in frontotemporal dementia have also demonstrated the appearance of artistic abilities. Disruptive behaviors occur more frequently and are more common in early stages of Huntington’s chorea and Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (IPA 2002). DIAGNOSIS OF BPSD A variety of rating scales have been developed to assess for BPSD. Some of the more common instruments utilized include: 1. Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory is a 29-item instrument intended for use by formal caregivers to assess the frequency of
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
79
certain behaviors. Items are rated on a seven-point scale. The instrument takes about 10–15 minutes to complete. The author recommends use of a training manual (Cohen-Mansfield, Marx, and Rosenthal 1989). 2. Behavioral Pathologic Rating Scale for Alzheimer ’s disease (BEHAVE-AD) is a 25-item instrument intended for use by formal caregivers to assess for a variety of symptoms (delusions, paranoia, hallucinations, aggression, activity, mood state, anxiety, phobias, and alterations in circadian rhythms). Items are rated on a four-point scale. The instrument takes about 20 minutes to complete (Reisberg et al. 1987). 3. Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) is a structured interview of a caregiver that assesses for 10 neuropsychiatric domains. With a positive response to a domain, more in-depth subquestions are asked to determine the frequency (rated 1–4) and severity (rated 1–3) of the behavior. The product (frequency × severity) is calculated for a total score ranging from 1 to 144. Standard and nursing home versions of the instrument are available. It is available in numerous translations and takes about 10 minutes to complete (Cummings et al. 1994).
MANAGEMENT OF BPSD Assessment It is essential that the first step in managing BPSD is a thorough assessment including a detailed history and physical examination of the patient. This approach can often reveal untreated medical conditions that may be contributing to disruptive behavior (Buhr and White 2006). These include delirium, urinary tract infection, medication side effects, depression, or environmental factors (too much or too little stimulation/activity), any of which may be easily treatable. Untreated pain can also be expressed behaviorally in patients who are unable to verbally communicate their discomfort (Cohen-Mansfield 2005). Observing for facial expressions (e.g., grimacing) associated with irritable or agitated behavior may reveal unreported pain. BPSD may also be precipitated or worsened in patients with hearing or vision impairment leading to misinterpretation of surroundings and events. Once other potential causes for problematic behaviors have been ruled out, the focus should turn to clearly defining the target BPSD and it is recommended to focus on one symptom at a time. To assess for patterns or triggers of BPSD, the caregiver should be instructed to keep a log of
80
Dementia
incidents including frequency, duration, intensity, time of day, location, and events that preceded or occurred after the incident (Hwang 2008, 379). Next, goal setting should involve the patient (when possible), the caregiver, and professional staff to work collaboratively to identify creative strategies that are tailored to the individual. If caregivers will be implementing the strategies, they should be provided support, a realistic expectation for when to expect change, and alternatives plans should the strategy be ineffective. Interventions should be routinely evaluated and modified to determine ongoing effectiveness and when a change in strategy may be needed. An excellent resource for caregivers is the book The 36-Hour Day (Mace and Rabins 1991), which provides tips and suggested strategies for managing and coping BPSD in patients with dementia. Nonpharmacologic Treatment Buhr and White suggest five care goals for patients with dementia including “to feel safe, to feel comfortable, to experience a sense of control, to experience minimal stress with adequate positive stimulation, and to experience pleasure” (2006, 181). The authors provide several practical tips to achieve these goals. The first technique suggested is for the caregiver to remain relaxed and flexible, smiling and providing eye contact. Another strategy is referred to as “hand under hand,” which involves the caregiver putting a hand underneath the patient’s hand while leading him or her through an activity. This strategy is believed to provide both direction and support to the client. Finally, when the patient exhibits resistance, it is recommended that the caregiver avoid arguing or reasoning, which are most often ineffective. Instead, it is suggested that the caregiver use distraction and redirection while acknowledging the patient’s feelings. Consistent routines and caregivers are also highly recommended. Learning patient preferences regarding personal care activities can sometimes reduce resistant behaviors in patients with dementia. The authors recommend modifications to the environment such as stimulated presence therapy (a recording of a family member talking about happy memories with the client) calming music, hand massage, or decorating the client’s nursing home room with familiar objects from home. A variety of nonpharmacologic therapeutic interventions been identified in the literature and research regarding their effectiveness is rapidly expanding. These interventions include activity-based therapies (e.g., music, art, dance, drama, and exercise therapies), reminiscence therapy, reality orientation, validation therapy, and multisensory stimulation (snoezelen) therapy (Minardi and Hayes 2003). Activity therapies can be
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
81
incorporated informally by family or professional caregivers (e.g., using music to aid in relaxation). However, when these activities are delivered as a specific therapy, it requires that a qualified therapist administer them. These therapies can be provided individually or in groups. Reminiscence involves reflection back on one’s life to remember life events and associated feelings with the goal of sharing experiences, socialization, and diversion. Reality orientation involves increasing the client’s awareness of present time, place, and person with the goal of reducing confusion and reorienting the client to reality. Validation therapy, in contrast to reality orientation, focuses on the disoriented person’s reality, which may be different than the caregiver ’s reality. Rather than focusing on whether information is correct or accurate, the center of attention is on emotional content being communicated by the client. Multisensory stimulation (snoezelen) involves increasing stimulation of the senses (smell, vision, hearing, and touch). With this type of therapy, a room is created to provide sensory stimulation and may include multicolored light source, a soapbubble machine, music, soft objects, relaxing furniture, and a machine that provides a variety of pleasant fragrances. The aim of this therapy is relaxation, enjoyment, and to provide a calming experience for an agitated client. Research indicates that BPSD is a consequence of the interaction between the disease pathology, the patient’s capabilities, and environmental factors (Gitlin et al. 2007). Environmental modifications have been shown to be effective in reducing agitation in nursing home settings and studies have begun to examine the effectiveness of similar strategies in the home environment (Gitlin et al. 2007). The following environmental modifications are recommended. Obstructions should be removed for patients who like to wander or pace. The patient should be provided ample room to walk and barriers should be removed if possible. Doors should be equipped with locks or alarms to signal when the client has exited the building. Signs should be provided for orientation and to help the patient with way-finding. Efforts should be made to eliminate or reduce extraneous noise (e.g., loud telephones or paging systems). Lighting should be adjusted to reflect typical day-night pattern. In institutional settings, attempts should be made to separate noisy patients from quiet patients. Also, efforts should be made to reduce the risk of visual hallucinations or misinterpretation of the environment (e.g., remove mirrors, use optimal lighting, avoid abstract art or décor). Sleep pattern disruptions often begin in the early stages of dementia and worsen as the course progresses. A variety of strategies are suggested to promote adequate sleep and rest. These include proving a consistent
82
Dementia
daily schedule of activities and rest periods. Changes in regular routines should be introduced gradually if possible. Increasing activity during daytime can promote improved sleep quality at night. Using sleep hygiene strategies are recommended and include regular bedtimes; use of the bedroom mainly for sleep; avoiding late-night eating, fluids, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine; reduction in light and noise at bedtime; and incorporating a bedtime routine. Medications that may interfere with sleep should also be avoided at bedtime. Both patients and caregivers can benefit from psychotherapy. Patients with early or mild dementia can gain from individual psychotherapy to vent feeling about their diagnosis and symptoms, express grief over anticipated losses, and to gain insight from education about the illness. Although patients may not remember specifics about the individual sessions, they do benefit from the caring relationship (IPA 2002). There is some evidence that suggests that clients with early or mild dementia may benefit from group psychotherapy through the experience of being with others who are having similar experiences (Yale 1995). Unfortunately many system barriers exist that prevent the use of nonpharmacologic interventions. First, there is a lack of funding or reimbursement for nonpharmacologic interventions or for associated training and equipment. Other barriers include lack of knowledgeable staff, low staffing levels, and organizational cultures that are not supportive of the associated costs (Cohen-Mansfield 2005). Pharmacologic Treatment Although practice guidelines recommend that nonpharmacologic interventions be attempted first to treat BPSD, this is often not what happens in clinical practice (Wood-Mitchell et al. 2008). In fact, psychotropic medications (particularly antipsychotic medications) are widely used initially for the treatment of BPSD. In addition to antipsychotic medications, other psychotropic medications commonly prescribed to treat BPSD include benzodiazepines, antidepressants, and mood stabilizers. Research has demonstrated limited effectiveness of these medications in treating BPSD. This is coupled with the risk for serious side effects of many of these medications. The reasons for the common use of psychotropic medications despite their limited efficacy and risk for side effects are a variety of factors. Research indicates that psychiatrists often feel pressured to order medication because the symptoms of BPSD are so distressing to patients and caregivers (Wood-Mitchell et al. 2008). In addition, psychiatrists reported
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
83
feeling a lack of viable nonpharmacologic alternatives and that societal influences place pressure on them to prescribe medications as a “quick fix.” This study also revealed inconsistencies in prescribing practices among psychiatrists despite the availability of practice guidelines, reportedly because psychiatrists feel they are unable to implement recommended practice guidelines. Psychiatrists reported a belief that long-term care facilities are not designed to deal with problem behavior nor is the staff adequately trained to implement nonpharmacologic interventions. Regulatory bodies and consumer advocacy groups have placed increased scrutiny and criticism on prescribing practices, complaining that there is a tendency to overprescribe psychotropic medications and to underuse nonpharmacologic interventions. However, despite these criticisms, many barriers prevent the use of nonpharmacologic interventions. In nursing homes, these challenges include poorly designed floorplans, low stimulation environments, insufficient staff training, low staffing levels, staff turnover, and lack of reimbursement for nonpharmacologic interventions and associated staff training (Hwang 2008; Wood-Mitchell et al. 2008). Other challenges to implementation of nonpharmacologic interventions include how practical or efficient the strategies are and the time and amount of training needed for various techniques (Hwang 2008). In situations in which nonpharmacologic interventions are not effective, when patients’ behavior endangers themselves or others, or when the symptoms cause severe distress, the use of psychotropic medications should be considered. The atypical antipsychotics are widely considered the first line of treatment for BPSD. Other medications commonly considered are typical conventional antipsychotics, mood stabilizers, cholinesterase inhibitors, NMDA receptor antagonists (memantine), benzodiazepines, and antidepressants (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Atypical Antipsychotics In 2005 the U.S. Food and Drug Administration issued black box warnings regarding the use of atypical antipsychotic medications in older adults with dementia due to the metabolic, cardiac, cerebrovascular, and mortality risks associated with these medications. Thus, the choice to prescribe these medications to manage BPSD should be weighed carefully against the potential adverse effects of these medications which also include parkinsonism, sedation, edema, chest infections, and cognitive decline (Wood-Mitchell et al. 2008). There is evidence that suggests atypical antipsychotics are moderately effective in treating aggression, agitation, and
84
Dementia
psychosis in patients with dementia; however, they usually are not effective with symptoms such as wandering, social withdrawal, shouting, pacing, touching, cognitive deficits, or incontinence (Fraser and Tilyard 2008). This class of psychotropic medications is not only the most prescribed but also the most researched. They have fewer adverse effects than the typical antipsychotics as well as fewer drug-drug interactions (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Of the atypical antipsychotics, risperidone is the most researched in regards to management of BPSD. The benefits of risperidone are that it causes minimal problems with sedation, weight gain, and metabolic or anticholinergic effects in comparison to other atypical antipsychotics (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). The disadvantages of risperidone are increased risk for extrapyramidal symptoms (EPS), hyperprolactinaemia, osteoporosis, orthostatic hypotension, risk for falls, and the FDA warning about increased risk for stroke and mortality in patients with dementia. The recommended starting dose is 0.25–0.5 mg per day and the targeted maintenance dose is 1–2 mg per day (Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Olanzapine has shown modest efficacy in treating BPSD. The benefit of this medication is a lower rate of EPS in patients with dementia. The disadvantages are increased sedation, rapid and significant weight gain, metabolic adverse effects, orthostatic hypotension, and anticholinergic effects ( Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). In addition, the FDA has issued warnings regarding the increased risk for stroke and mortality in patients with dementia. The recommended starting dose is 2.5 mg per day with a maximum maintenance dose of 10 mg per day (Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Quetiapine has demonstrated minimal effectiveness in treating aggression in patients with dementia. The benefits of this medication are lower rates of EPS, low anticholinergic effects, and less adverse metabolic effects. This medication can cause sedation and significant orthostatic hypotension, and is also included in the FDA warnings. Starting dose is recommended at 12.5–50 mg per day and a maintenance dose is suggested of 150 mg per day (Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Aripiprazole has demonstrated some effectiveness in treating BPSD. The advantages of this medication are low rates for the following potential adverse effects: EPS, anticholinergic effects, sedation, metabolic effects, and hematologic or cardiac effects. This drug also carries the FDA warnings regarding risk for stroke and mortality in patients with dementia (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007).
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
85
Clozapine has not been researched regarding its use in the management of BPSD. Potential benefits for using it with older patients are low risk for EPS, and it is sometimes effective in patients who are not able to tolerate the other antipsychotics. Disadvantages for its use in the elderly are risks for agranulocytosis, lowered seizure threshold, weight gain, metabolic effects, anticholinergic effects, sedation, and orthostatic hypotension, as well as FDA warnings regarding greater risk for stroke and mortality. Monitoring of white blood count and absolute neutrophil count are required. Typical Antipsychotics The typical antipsychotics demonstrate higher rates of EPS, anticholinergic effects, and cardiovascular effects when given to older adults. Haloperidol has been shown to be effective in treatment of aggression but it is questionable whether it has any advantage over atypical antipsychotic medications (Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Haldol has been associated with a greater risk for tardive dyskinesia. The recommended starting dose is 0.25 mg per day and a maintenance dose of up to 2 mg twice per day. The use of atypical antipsychotics should be avoided if possible in patients who have dementia with Lewy bodies or those with Parkinson’s disease or symptoms. The typical antipsychotics can cause dangerous EPS in patients who have dementia with Lewy bodies and there is a greater risk for neuroleptic malignant syndrome. Those with Parkinson’s disease or Parkinson’s-like syndromes are at greater risk for adverse effects of atypical antipsychotics (Fraser and Tilyard 2008). Mood Stabilizers Dilvalproex and carbemazepine have been prescribed to treat agitation in patients with dementia. Research results are weak in demonstrating their effectiveness. Both have serious potential adverse effects and potential drug interactions (Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Cholinesterase Inhibitors In most investigations of donepezil, rivastigmine, and galantamine, they have demonstrated some effectiveness in treating BPSD, particularly psychosis, agitation, or aggression. Use of these medications should be considered when antipsychotic medications have been found ineffective (Fraser and Tilyard 2008; Madhusoodanan et al. 2007).
86
Dementia
NMDA Receptor Antagonists (Memantine) Three studies that examined the use of memantine on BPSD found favorable effect, particularly with symptoms of agitation and aggression (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007; Wilcock et al. 2008). Memantine also demonstrated a positive effect on cognitive, functional, and global outcomes of patients with AD (Wilcock et al. 2008). Benzodiazepines Benzodiazepines can alleviate agitation symptoms temporarily but these medications are associated with an increased risk for sedation and associated fall risk (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). They also can increase confusion, worsen cognition, and cause disinhibition, with the potential for exacerbating behavior disturbances. If used for severe agitation, they should be used for no more than two weeks (Fraser and Tilyard 2008). Antidepressants Some research indicates that citalopram, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), can be effective in treatment of agitation in patients with dementia. Other studies have found that citalopram demonstrated significant improvement in older adults with dementia in their symptoms of depression, anxiety, agitation, and social interactions (IPA 2002). In summary, the use of psychotropic medications for treatment of BPSD should be considered a last alternative after nonpharmacologic interventions have been found ineffective. However, because of the risk of untreated agitation and potential harm to self or others, medications are considered a viable alternative. The atypical antipsychotics are the preferred first alternative but a thorough risk assessment should be considered (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Clinicians should carefully consider individual cases and weigh the potential benefits with potential risks. Use of algorithms to guide treatment decisions, particularly in prescribing antipsychotic medications, is highly recommended (Fraser and Tilyard 2008). Education should be provided to both patients and family and ongoing monitoring should evaluate the ongoing need for medication with the goal of discontinuing when the patient is more stable (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007). Further research is needed to evaluate the effectiveness of other psychotropic medications to identify if there is a safer alternative to the atypical antipsychotics, which thus far have been found to show the most benefit.
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
87
IMPACT OF BPSD ON CAREGIVERS BPSD is stressful for the family caregiver and can lead to caregiver burden. Caregiver burden refers to both the practical problems and the emotional strain of caregiving. Caregiver burden can lead to low morale and has been shown to cause increased rates of anxiety and depression in caregivers (Jeste et al. 2006). The burden of dealing with the symptoms of BPSD often contributes to increased hospitalizations and earlier nursing home placement of the patient with dementia (Matsumoto et al. 2007). This also adds further emotional stress on the caregiver. The increased rates of hospitalization and institutionalization leads to increased healthcare costs. It has been estimated that 30% of the cost of dementia-related health care of patients with AD is directly related to the management of psychiatric symptoms (Jeste et al. 2006). Thus the burden of caring for the patient with BPSD has spiraling effects on the caregiver and the family unit, as well as societal costs. Certain BPSD symptoms contribute to a greater sense of burden and stress by the caregiver. These include screaming, physical aggression, personality conflicts, wandering, depression, resistance to caregiving, paranoia, and insomnia. However, it is important to note that caregivers differ in their reaction to BPSD symptoms and differ in which symptoms they may find distressing. The associated burden does not always depend on the severity or frequency of BPSD but, instead, certain symptoms such as agitation or aggression may be intolerable to some caregivers (Matsumoto et al. 2007). Similarly, symptoms that occur with high frequency or severity are not always viewed as burdensome by the caregiver; thus, it is imperative to assess the caregiver ’s perception of the patient’s behavior and perceived burden. In addition to differing in their response to BPSD, caregivers also vary in their ability to handle the symptoms. Characteristics or behaviors of the caregiver may exacerbate or alleviate BPSD (IPA 2002). Behaviors that can aggravate BPSD include sudden changes in routine or environment, arguing or “power struggles,” placing excessive demands on the patient, being overly critical, ignoring needs, being rigid, repeatedly asking the same question, anger or aggression, and becoming frustrated. Behaviors that can assuage BPSD include a warm caring attitude, attempting to understand the meaning of behavior, flexibility, adaptability, realistic expectations, tolerance, consistency, allowing a sense of control/autonomy, and a sense of humor. A variety of strategies is recommended to lessen caregiver burden including psychological support, education, and development of a social support network. Comprehensive caregiver programs have been
88
Dementia
shown to decrease caregiver burden and delay institutionalization (IPA 2002). Family caregivers can also benefit from family support groups and family psychotherapy to deal with conflicts and issues of loss and/or guilt, to receive information about the illness, and to provide support to one another. Providing educational materials on dementia and the importance of the self-care for the caregiver are also essential. Once such source is the National Institute Health 2002 booklet, Caregiver Guide: Tips for Caregivers of People with Alzheimer ’s Disease. The demands of caregiving can also impact the physical health of the caregiver. Research indicates caregiving has a negative effect on the immune function and blood pressure. Caregiving has also been associated with a lack of sleep or rest and failure to seek medical attention when needed. It is suggested that the prolonged stress of caregiving may combine with inherent vulnerabilities which can lead to more serious health outcomes for the caregiver (IPA 2002). Tolerability of the symptoms of BPSD in patients with dementia is also an issue for the professional caregiver. For professional caregivers in the hospital or nursing home setting, it often is a matter of how disruptive the behavior is to other patients or the patient environment. Different people in the same situation or environment may have different levels of tolerance to the same behavior. Time of day may also be a factor in the tolerability of behavior such that certain behaviors may be more tolerable during daytime than night time hours. Environmental factors and setting may also influence tolerability of behaviors. BPSD can be stressful for the professional caregiver because high dependence needs of the person with dementia, disruptive and repetitive behavior, challenges with communication, and lack of feedback from the person with dementia. These problems can lead to feelings of uncertainty by the staff and lead to the risk of abuse or perceptions of abuse by the client with dementia who may misinterpret what the caregiver is doing to him or her. It has been suggested that use of nonpharmacologic interventions can assist the caregiver as well as the patient by providing a more relaxing environment, increased opportunity for positive interactions and communication, and shared enjoyment. These activities remind the caregiver that the person with dementia does not lose the capacity for enjoyment or the ability to engage in meaningful human interactions (Minardi and Hayes 2003). The potential benefit to the caregiver is that these interactions also provide a positive and meaningful experience that can help alleviate caregiver stress and burnout. Because of the demands and high levels of stress associated with caring for patients with BPSD, support structures should
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
89
be available for staff including education about dementia and BPSD, supervision and counseling, consistency in staff assignments, and collaborative individualized case management.
CONCLUSIONS The behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia can become disruptive and intolerable to patients with dementia and their caregivers. The presence of these symptoms can lead to increased hospitalizations, earlier institutionalization, distress for the patient, and caregiver burden. The first line of treatment for BPSD should incorporate nonpharmacologic interventions that are tailored to the abilities, preferences, and needs of the individual with dementia. When symptoms do not respond to nonpharmacologic interventions or if they become dangerous or intolerable, then pharmacologic interventions become necessary. The use of psychotropic medications to treat BPSD should be done judiciously and the use of bestpractice guidelines is essential. Because of the anguish BPSD can cause caregivers, comprehensive treatment strategies should also be provided to family and professional caregivers.
REFERENCES Alzheimer, A. 1906. Uber einen eigenartigen schweren Er Krankungsprozeb der Hirnrinde. Neurologisches Cetralblatt 23: 1129–1136. Buhr, G. T., and H. K. White. 2006. Difficult behaviors in long-term care patients with dementia. Journal of the American Medical Director ’s Association 7: 180–192. Cohen, D., C. Eisdorfer, P. Gorelick, G. Paveza, D. J. Luchins, S. Freels, J. W. Ashford, et al. 1993. Psychopathology associated with Alzheimer ’s disease and related disorders. Journals of Gerontology 8: M255–M260. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 1996. Conceptualization of agitation results based on the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory and the Agitation Behavior Mapping Instrument. International Psychogeriatrics 8 (Suppl. 3): 309–315. Cohen-Mansfield, J. 2005. Nonpharmacological interventions for persons with dementia. Alzheimer ’s Care Quarterly 6: 129–145. Cohen-Mansfield, J., M. S. Marx, and A. S. Rosenthal. 1989. A description of agitation in a nursing home. Journal of Gerontology 44: M77–M84. Cummings, J. L., M. S. Mega, K. Gray, S. Rosenberg-Thompson, D. A. Carusi, and J. A. Gornbein. 1994. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44: 2308–2314. Ellis, H., and A. W. Young. 1990. Accounting for delusional misidentifications. British Journal of Psychiatry 157: 239–248.
90
Dementia
Fraser, T., and M. Tilyard, eds. 2008. Antipsychotics in dementia. www.bpac.org.nz/ a4d. Gerdner, L. A., K. C. Buckwalter, and G. R. Hall. 2005. Temporal patterning of agitation and stressors associated with agitation: Case profiles to illustrate the progressively lowered stress threshold model. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association 1: 215–222. Gitlin, L. N., L. Winter, M. P. Dennis, and W. W. Hauck. 2007. A nonpharmacologic intervention to manage behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and reduce caregiver distress: Design and methods of Project ACT. Clinical Interventions in Aging 2: 695–703. Hwang, R. 2008. Managing difficult behaviors in patients with dementia. Virtual Mentor 10: 379–382. http://virtualmentor.ama-assn.org/2008/06/jdsc 1-0806.html. International Psychogeriatric Association (IPA). 2002. BPSD online educational pack: Introduction to Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. http:// www.ipaonline.org/ipaonlinev3/ipaprograms/bpsdarchives/bpsdrev/ toc.asp. Jeste, D. V., T. W. Meeks, D. S. Kim, and G. S. Zubenko. 2006. Research agenda for DSM-V: Diagnostic categories and criteria for neuropsychiatric syndromes in dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 19: 160–171. Mace, N. L., and P. V. Rabins. 1991. The 36-hour day. A family guide to caring for persons with Alzheimer ’s disease related dementing illnesses and memory loss in later life. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Madhusoodanan, S., P. Shah, R. Brenner, and S. Gupta. 2007. Pharmacological treatment of the psychosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: What is the best approach? CNS Drugs 21: 101–115. Matsumoto, N., M. Ikeda, R. Fukuhara, S. Shinagawa, T. Ishikawa, T. Mori, Y. Toyota, et al. 2007. Caregiver burden associated with behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in elderly people in the local community. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 23: 219–224. Minardi, H. A., and N. Hayes. 2003. Nursing older adults with mental health problems: Therapeutic interventions—part 2. Nursing Older People 15: 20–24. National Institute of Health. 2002. Caregiver guide: Tips for caregivers of people with Alzheimer ’s disease. http://www.nia.nih.gov/Alzheimers/Publications/ caregiverguide.htm. O’Leary, D., D. Jyringi, and M. Sedler. 2005. Childhood conduct problems, stages of Alzheimer ’s disease, and physical aggression against caregivers. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20: 401–405. Reisberg, B., J. Borenstein, S. P. Salob, S. H. Ferris, E. Franssen, and A. Georgotas. 1987. Behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease: Phenomenology and treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 48 (Suppl.): 9–15. Ropacki, S. A., and D. V. Jeste. 2005. Epidemiology of and risk factors for psychosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: A review of 55 studies published from 1990 to 2003. American Journal of Psychiatry 162: 2022–2030.
Managing Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
91
Wilcock, G. K., C. G. Ballard, J. A. Cooper, and H. Loft. 2008. Memantine for agitation/aggression and psychosis in moderately severe to severe Alzheimer ’s disease: A pooled analysis of 3 studies. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69: 341–348. Wood-Mitchell, A., I. A. James, A. Waterworth, A. Swann, and C. Ballard. 2008. Factors influencing the prescribing of medications by old age psychiatrists for behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia: A qualitative study. Age and Ageing 37: 547–552. Yale, R. 1995. Developing support groups for individuals with early-stage Alzheimer ’s disease. Baltimore: Health Professions Press.
Chapter 5
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia: Treatment with Antipsychotics Rosa Liperoti
In November 1906 the German psychiatrist Alois Alzheimer delivered a lecture at the 37th Conference of South-West German Psychiatrists in Tübingen reporting the case of Auguste D. (Alzheimer 1906). According to Dr. Alzheimer ’s description, the first noticeable symptom of illness shown by this 51-year-old woman was a strong feeling of jealously toward her husband. At times, believing that people were out to murder her, she started to scream loudly. At times, she seemed to have auditory hallucinations. Very soon, she showed rapidly worsening memory loss. She was disoriented in her flat and wandered aimlessly from room to room. After four years of illness, Auguste D. died. Dr. Alzheimer performed an autopsy and found in her brain tissue a large amount of senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) which are known to be the peculiar pathological lesions of Alzheimer ’s disease (AD). More than 100 years ago, Dr. Alzheimer described a case of AD with behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) such as delusions, hallucinations, agitation, and wandering. The term BPSD has been introduced in 1996 in a consensus statement of the International Psychogeriatric Association and describes a wide spectrum of noncognitive symptoms of dementia including agitation, verbal and physical aggression, psychotic symptoms (delusions and hallucinations), oppositional behavior, socially inappropriate behavior, apathy, anxiety, sleep disturbances, and wandering (Finkel 2002). It has been
94
Dementia
estimated that up to 90% of patients with AD may show at least one of these symptoms during the course of the disease (Benoit et al. 2005; Chan et al. 2003; Lyketsos et al. 2002). They may be manifest at any stage of AD although they have been less frequently reported in the early stages of the disease (Bozeat et al. 2000; Cummings et al. 1996). BPSD are also present in dementia syndromes other than AD, such as dementia with Lewy bodies or frontotemporal lobar degeneration, where they may appear also in the early stages. BPSD have a tremendous impact on patients’ and families’ quality of life (Fitten 2006; Levy et al. 1996). Families struggle to manage patients with BPSD. Caregivers of patients with BPSD are at high risk of distress, depression and burn-out (Benoit et al. 2006). These symptoms are a primary reason for patients’ institutionalization (Cohen et al. 1993). Patients with BPSD show accelerated cognitive deterioration; they are at high risk of functional decline, disability, hospitalization, emergency room visits and death (Scarmeas et al. 2005). Finally, BPSD have been associated with an enormous increase in costs of care (Herrmann et al. 2006; Alzheimer ’s Association 2009). In our aging society, where the number of AD cases is growing fast and approaching 35 million worldwide, BPSD represent a major public health issue and a disruptive condition for families and societies (Alzheimer Disease International 2009). Although BPSD are generally considered treatable symptoms, their management represents a serious challenge for physicians and caregivers. Several therapeutic options are available and they include nonpharmacological and pharmacological strategies. According to clinical guidelines, nonpharmacological interventions must represent the first-choice strategy of treatment for BPSD (American Geriatrics Society and American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry 2003; Alexopoulos et al. 2005; Expert Consensus Panel for Dementia 2005). Nonpharmacological treatments for dementia include a variety of interventions targeting the patient, the family, and the environment. They include music therapy, massage/touch therapy, physical exercise, aromatherapy, light therapy, environmental manipulation, reminiscence therapy, behavior management, multisensory stimulation, and validation therapy. To date, evidence supporting the use of such techniques in clinical practice is limited and mostly derives from clinical studies with small sample size and weak study designs (Hulme et al. 2010; Hersch and Falzgraf 2007). Moreover, most nonpharmacological interventions have been tested among community-dwelling patients and require specific caregiver training to be applied. Nonetheless, it is strongly recommended to consider a pharmacological approach to BPSD only after nonpharmacological strategies have failed to control these symptoms.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
95
To date, antipsychotic medications represent the most efficacious pharmacological option for the treatment of BPSD, although several psychotropic medications including cholinesterase inhibitors, benzodiazepines, antidepressants, N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor modulators, and anticonvulsants have been suggested to be beneficial in controlling some specific symptoms (Madhusoodanan et al. 2007; Jeste et al. 2008). The aim of this chapter is to describe the brain anatomical and biochemical abnormalities underlying BPSD and the main pharmacological properties of antipsychotics, to review the evidence regarding the efficacy and safety of antipsychotics in patients with dementia, and to provide the reader with practical recommendations for the daily management of BPSD. ETIOLOGY OF BPSD Etiological processes underlying BPSD are yet to be fully understood. Multiple biologic and nonbiologic factors are believed to contribute to the development of BPSD. Psychological factors such as a patient’s premorbid personality and response to stress, social factors such as environmental changes and stressful events, caregiver factors such as caregiver distress and reaction to patient’s behavior interact with genetic aspects and neurobiological abnormalities in determining the onset of BPSD, the pattern of symptoms, and their severity (Meins, Frey, and Thiesemann 2008; Zuidema et al. 2010). Genetic studies have suggested that several chromosomal abnormalities may represent a risk factor for the development of BPSD. A mutation of the presenilin 1 gene on chromosome 14 has been linked with depression and psychosis in AD (Harvey et al. 1998). Serotonin and dopamine are brain chemical neurotransmitters that bind specific cellular receptors, thus allowing communication among neuronal cells and nerve fibers. In particular, serotonin is believed to play a major role in modulating mood, emotion, and sexuality while dopamine is thought to mediate behavior, some aspects of cognition, sleep, attention, mood, and motor activity. The term genetic polymorphism indicates a specific variation of a gene. Polymorphisms of brain serotonin and dopamine receptor genes may predispose to the development of BPSD. In particular, visual and auditory hallucinations have been associated with polymorphisms of serotonin receptor genes (5HT2A 102-T/C and 5HT2c Cys23ser) (Holmes et al. 1998). Variation of the dopamine receptor DRD1 and DRD3 genes have been associated with an increased risk of developing psychotic and aggressive symptoms in AD patients (Sweet et al. 1998). Psychosis and
96
Dementia
aggressive behavior in combination have been associated with a genetic polymorphism of serotonin transporter (5-HTTPR II genotype) (Sweet et al. 2001). Finally, genetic variations of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) have been related to depression in AD patients (Borroni, Costanzi, and Padovani 2010). It has been shown that the pathology of different brain regions is responsible for the development of different BPSD. It has been documented that relative to patients who do not develop psychosis, those exhibiting psychotic symptoms have a greater density of NFT in the brain neocortex (Farber et al. 2000). Higher NFT concentration has also been reported in the orbitofrontal cortex of AD patients with agitation (Tekin et al. 2001). Neurofunctional-imaging studies have documented that psychosis is associated with a decreased metabolism of the prefrontal, left frontal-temporal and right parietal areas (Lopez et al. 2001; Sultzer et al. 2003). Other psychotic symptoms such hallucination and delusional misidentification have been associated with low neuron count in the CA1 area of the hippocampus and in the dorsal raphe (Forstl et al. 1994). Abnormalities of multiple neurotransmitter systems have been identified in the brain of patients with dementia. The decrease of brain acetylcholine levels is responsible for memory impairment, cognitive symptoms, and delirium in AD (van der Cammen et al. 2006). The decrease in cholinergic activity may result in a relative increase of the dopaminergic and noradrenergic activity, which would lead to psychotic symptoms, behavioral disturbances, and aggression (Engelborghs et al. 2008; Herrmann, Lanctôt, and Khan 2004). Also, reduced levels of serotonin have been found in different brain regions of AD patients (Lanctôt, Herrmann, and Mazzotta 2001). Most pharmacological treatments for BSPD include medications able to either increase or decrease or modulate the activity of such neurotransmitters. PHARMACOLOGY OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS Antipsychotics, also called neuroleptics, are the mainstay of pharmacological treatment for BPSD. A list of the most commonly used antipsychotic medications described by chemical structure and drug name is reported in Table 5.1. The so-called conventional, typical, or first-generation antipsychotics have been used since the 1950s for the treatment of schizophrenia. According to their chemical structure, conventional antipsychotics are classified in phenotiazines, butyrophenones, and thioxanthenes. All conventional agents share high affinity for the D2 dopamine receptor. Although their
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
97
Table 5.1 Antipsychotic Medications Chemical structure
Drug name
Phenothiazines
Chlorpromazine, Promazine, Levomepromazine, Acepromazine, Triflupromazine, Fluphenazine, Perphenazine, Prochlorperazine, Trifluoperazine, Acetophenazine, Periciazine, Thioridazine, Mesoridazine
Butyrophenone derivatives
Haloperidol, Droperidol, Trifluperidol, Bromperidol, Benperidol, Melperone
Indole derivatives
Sertindole, Molindone, Ziprasidone
Thioxantene derivatives
Flupentixol, Clopenthixol, Chloprothixene, Thiothixene, Zuclopenthixol
Benzamides
Sulpiride, Remoxipride, Amisulpride, Levosulpiride
Diazepines, Oxazepines, Thiazepines
Clozapine, Loxapine, Olanzapine, Quetiapine
Benzisoxazole derivatives
Risperidone, Iloperidone, Paliperidone
Quinolinone derivatives
Aripiprazole
mechanism of action has not been fully clarified, they are believed to exert their action by blocking the D2 receptors in the mesolimbic pathway (Xiberas et al. 2001). It has been documented that the efficacy of these drugs is strictly correlated with the occupancy rate of D2 receptors. D2 occupancy rate has been shown to predict the clinical response to haloperidol. A threshold of 65% occupancy rate provides a good separation between responders and nonresponders to treatment (Kapur et al. 2000). Therapeutic action of conventional antipsychotics is often achieved together with the onset of extrapyramidal side effects (EPS) and tardive dyskinesia. EPS include dystonia (uncontrollable muscle contractions that can cause painful twisting of parts of the body, especially the neck), akathisia (a disabling form of internal or external restlessness that can lead to the complete inability to sit still and to the constant urge to be moving), parkinsonism (a set of symptoms that resemble symptoms of Parkinson’s disease, e.g., tremor, stiffness of trunk, arms, or legs, difficulty in starting movement, gait and balance disturbances). Tardive dyskinesia is usually a side effect of long-term treatment
98
Dementia
with antipsychotics and consists of repetitive, involuntary and purposeless body or facial movements such as tongue protrusion, eye blinking, movements of fingers, arms, or legs. These cumbersome side effects as well as other side effects such as hyperprolactinemia result from the occupancy of D2 receptors in the basal ganglia (Nordström et al. 1993; Farde et al. 1992). Second-generation antipsychotic drugs have been described as “atypical” because they were found to exert antipsychotic effect with significant lower propensity to cause EPS and hyperprolactinemia. These advantages can be explained examining the pharmacodynamic characteristics of atypical antipsychotics. These compounds, while showing affinity for D2 dopamine receptors, target multiple receptor systems in the brain including D1, D3, D4, D5 dopamine receptors, 5HT1, 5HT2, 5HT3, 5HT6, 5HT7 serotonin receptors, alpha 1 and 2 adrenergic receptors, H1 histamine receptors, M1 acetylcholine receptors, and glutamate receptors. In particular, reduced EPS observed with second-generation antipsychotic drugs have been explained with their high 5HT2/D2 occupancy ratio (Meltzer, Matsubara, and Lee 1989). According to a recent hypothesis, the fast dissociation of atypical antipsychotics from the D2 receptor would be the potential mechanism by which these drugs have an antipsychotic effect without causing EPS or prolactin elevation (Kapur and Seeman 2001). Clozapine is the prototype of these newer medications. This drug has demonstrated low affinity for both D1 and D2 dopamine receptors, along with high affinity for D4 dopamine receptor and serotonin receptors (5HT2 and 3) (Kapur, Zipursky, and Remington 1999). It also has an antiglutamatergic action (Lidsky et al. 1993), as well as alpha-2 receptor affinity and M1 cholinergic receptor blocking activity (Factor 2002). The blockade of dopamine receptors exerted by clozapine is evident especially in the mesolimbic pathway but not in the nigro-striatal system (Baldessarini and Frankenburg 1991). This selectivity may in part explain the low incidence of side effects. Nearly two decades ago, risperidone was introduced in the market. To date, it is the most frequently prescribed atypical antipsychotic. Its pharmacodynamic profile is characterized by a high 5HT2/D2 affinity ratio. However, a similar proportion of D2 receptors occupied by risperidone and haloperidol has been documented with a dose-dependent propensity of this atypical agent to cause EPS (Kapur et al. 1995). Olanzapine was introduced shortly after risperidone, and it has a pharmacodynamic profile similar to that of clozapine (Factor 2002). Quetiapine was the fourth atypical antipsychotic marketed. It has a chemical structure similar to clozapine and a multireceptor activity but it acts selectively in the limbic system (Richelson 1996; Jibson and Tandon 1998). A 5HT/D2 occupancy ratio similar to that of other atypical antipsychotics has been
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
99
documented for ziprasidone although its affinity for D2 receptors is high (Fischman et al. 1996). Very recently paliperidone, a derivate of risperidone and iloperidone, has been marketed. Their mechanism of action is yet to be established although both drugs have shown affinity for D2 dopamine receptors and 5HT2A serotonin receptors (Madhusoodanan and Zaveri 2010; Citrome 2009). Relative to other drugs of this class, the atypical sertindole exerts a selective activity binding the D2, 5HT2 and alfa-1 adrenergic receptors. Also, its action is prevalent in the limbic system (Hertel 2006). Atypical antipsychotics include sulpiride and amisulpride, which are compounds belonging to the chemical class of benzamides. These drugs are highly selective blockers of the D2 and D3 receptors with a preferential limbic activity. They have also been shown to modulate dopamine release by binding presynaptic receptors (Schoemaker et al. 1997). Aripiprazole is a newer antipsychotic agent that has been identified as the “third-generation” antipsychotic. It is considered a stabilizer of the dopamine/serotonin system due to its peculiar pharmacological profile (Burris et al. 2002). In pharmacodynamics, a drug is defined as an antagonist if it binds to a given receptor and produces a full inhibition of that receptor. A drug is defined as an agonist if it binds to a given receptor and produces a full activation of that receptor. A drug is defined as a partial agonist if it competes with the natural neurotransmitter for a given receptor but it is able to activate that receptor to a lesser degree than its natural neurotransmitter would do, thus causing an attenuated response. Aripiprazole acts as a partial agonist at the D2 dopamine receptors. It also binds serotonin receptors acting as an antagonist of the 5HT2A receptors and as partial agonist of the 5HT1A receptors. EFFICACY OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS None of the available antipsychotics to date has been approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) for the treatment of BPSD. This is due to the fact that, despite a general clinical perception of efficacy reported by physicians, scientific evidence of efficacy is modest and derives from a limited number of randomized clinical trials (RCTs). Nonetheless, antipsychotic medications are widely used off-label as first-line agents for the pharmacological treatment of BPSD. Since their approval in the 1950s for the treatment of schizophrenia, conventional antipsychotics have been systematically used for the treatment of BPSD. They are prescribed in spite of a substantial lack of scientific evidence supporting their use in dementia. Few RCTs investigating
100
Dementia
the efficacy of conventional agents for the treatment of BPSD have been conducted between the 1960s and the late 1980s (Schneider, Pollock, and Lyness 1990; Barnes et al. 1982; Lonergan, Luxenberg, and Colford 2002). These studies have provided evidence on the effect of haloperidol and thioridazine while the effect of other conventional agents in dementia has been neglected by clinical research. Data from these early studies showed a modest advantage of conventional antipsychotics over placebo with a nearly 40% placebo response. Also, according to some of these studies, the observed superiority of conventional antipsychotics over placebo would be limited to symptoms of aggression. The validity of such findings is questionable due to the fact that these studies were characterized by small sample sizes and possible lack of power to detect any effect of antipsychotics. Atypical antipsychotics have been approved by the FDA exclusively for the treatment of schizophrenia and marketed in the 1990s. Although not approved for BPSD, these drugs rapidly became the gold standard of care for BPSD shortly after their introduction in clinical practice. This was due to the fact that atypical antipsychotics had shown a substantial advantage over conventional medications: They were able to exert antipsychotic effect without causing EPS and tardive dyskinesia (Gerlach 2000; Mossman and Lehrer 2000; Collaborative Working Group on Clinical Trial Evaluations 1998). Scientific societies of physicians and experts in the field developed clinical practice guidelines and consensus statements to promote the off-label use of atypical antipsychotics for the treatment of BPSD in spite of the limited number of RCTs documenting the efficacy and safety of these agents in dementia (American Geriatrics Society and American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry 2003). In the late 1990s, atypical agents accounted for more than 80% of antipsychotic prescriptions in dementia (Glick et al. 2001; Liperoti et al. 2003). At this time, risperidone, olanzapine, quetiapine, and aripiprazole are the only atypical antipsychotics that have been investigated in RCTs conducted on patients with dementia (see Table 5.2). Three placebo-controlled RCTs have shown that, compared to placebo, risperidone may be beneficial on psychotic symptoms and aggression at doses of 1 mg and 2 mg per day (Katz et al. 1999; De Deyn et al. 1999; Brodaty et al. 2003). These studies were conducted on patients with Alzheimer ’s disease, vascular dementia, or mixed dementia on a 12-week time period. Two placebo-controlled RCTs have suggested that olanzapine may improve behavioral symptoms, psychosis, and aggression at doses of 5 to 10 mg per day compared with placebo (De Deyn et al. 2004; Street et al. 2000).
Population
N=625; AD, VaD, Mixed dementia
N=344; AD, VaD, Mixed dementia
N=309; AD, VaD, Mixed dementia
N=206; AD
N=652; AD
N=298; AD, VaD, Mixed dementia
N=421; AD
RCT
Katz et al. 1999
De Deyn et al. 1999
Brodaty et al. 2003
Street et al. 2000
De Deyn et al. 2004
Deberdt et al. 2005
Schneider et al. 2006
Risperidone vs. haloperidol vs. placebo Risperidone vs. placebo
Nursing home
Nursing home
Olanzapine equal to risperidone, both superior to quetiapine and placebo
36 Flexible, olanzapine mean dose 5.5 mg; risperidone mean dose 1 mg; quetiapine mean dose 56.5 mg
Olanzapine vs. risperidone vs. quetiapine vs. placebo
Olanzapine equal to risperidone and placebo
10
Flexible, olanzapine mean dose 5.2 mg; risperidone mean dose 1 mg
Olanzapine vs. risperidone vs. placebo
Outpatient and residential
Outpatient
Olanzapine 7.5 mg superior to placebo
10
Olanzapine 5 and 10 mg superior to placebo
Fixed, 1, 2.5, 5, 7.5 mg
6
Risperidone superior to placebo
Olanzapine vs. placebo
Fixed, 5, 10, 15 mg
12
Risperidone superior to placebo and better tolerated than haloperidol
13
Flexible, risperidone mean dose 1.1 mg; haloperidol mean dose 1.2 mg Flexible, mean dose 0.95 mg
Risperidone 1 and 2 mg superior to placebo
Main findings
12
Duration (weeks)
Fixed, 0.5, 1, 2 mg
Daily dose
Nursing home
Olanzapine vs. placebo
Risperidone vs. placebo
Nursing home
Nursing home
Intervention
Setting
Table 5.2 Randomized Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trials of Atypical Antipsychotics in Patients with BPSD
N=93; AD
N=333; AD
N=40; Lewy Body Dementia, PD, AD
N=40; AD
N=208; AD
N=487; AD
N=256; AD
Ballard et al. 2005
Zhong et al. 2007
Kurlan et al. 2007
Paleacu et al. 2008
De Deyn et al. 2005
Mintzer et al. 2007
Steim et al. 2008
Nursing home
Nursing home
Outpatient
Aripiprazole vs. placebo
Aripiprazole vs. placebo
Aripiprazole vs. placebo
Quetiapine vs. placebo
Quetiapine vs. placebo
Outpatient
Not reported
Quetiapine vs. placebo
Quetiapine vs. rivastigmine vs. placebo
Quetiapine vs. haloperidol vs. placebo
Nursing home
Nursing home
Nursing home
Flexible, mean dose 9 mg
Fixed, 5, 10, 15 mg
Fixed, 5, 10, 15 mg
Flexible, median dose 200 mg
Flexible, mean dose 120 mg
10
10
10
6
10
10
26
Flexible, range for quetiapine 50-100 mg Fixed, 100, 200 mg
10
Flexible, quetiapine mean dose 96.9; haloperidol mean dose 1.9 mg
Notes: AD = Alzheimer ’s disease; PD = Parkinson’s disease; VaD = Vascular dementia.
N=284; AD
(Continued)
Tariot et al. 2006
Table 5.2
Aripiprazole superior to placebo in controlling behavioral symptoms, anxiety, depression, agitation
Aripiprazole 10 mg superior to placebo
Aripiprazole equal to placebo
Quetiapine equal to placebo
Quetiapine equal to placebo
Quetiapine 200 mg superior to placebo
No effect of quetiapine or rivastigmine on improving agitation
Quetiapine equal to haloperidol, both superior to placebo
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
103
These studies were conducted among patients with dementia for a 10-week and 6-week period of time, respectively. In contrast with these data, a recent study on patients with moderate to severe psychotic symptoms of dementia randomly assigned to receive a flexible dose of olanzapine (2.5–10.g per day), risperidone (0.5–2 mg per day) or placebo demonstrated similar improvement of BPSD in the three treatment groups with higher discontinuation rate due to adverse events in the olanzapine and risperidone groups (Deberdt et al. 2005). More recently, the impact of stopping long-term antipsychotic treatment was investigated in a small sample of nursing home residents with BPSD who were receiving haloperidol, risperidone, or olanzapine (Ruths et al. 2008). Study participants were randomized to either stop or continue antipsychotic treatment. Findings from this study documented that BPSD remained stable or improved in nearly 50% of residents who discontinued the treatment and in almost all those who continued to receive antipsychotics. A small placebo-controlled RCT conducted on a sample of 40 patients with dementia and parkinsonism found that quetiapine 120 mg per day was not effective for controlling psychotic symptoms and agitation although well tolerated (Kurlan et al. 2007). More recently, the effect of quetiapine (median dose 200 mg per day) was not superior to that of placebo in a small RCT conducted on patients with AD and BPSD for six weeks (Paleacu et al. 2008). Negative findings derived also from a late study that compared the effect of quetiapine, rivastigmine (an inhibitor of the enzyme acetylcholinesterase, which is used for treating cognitive symptoms of dementia), and placebo on agitation and cognition in a sample of 93 institutionalized patients with dementia (Ballard et al. 2005). This study demonstrated no effect of quetiapine or rivastigmine on improving agitation and an increased cognitive decline associated with the use of quetiapine. In contrast with these data, quetiapine at high daily dose (200 mg per day) has been found effective in a placebo-controlled RCT conducted on a sample of 333 institutionalized patients with dementia and agitation who were randomized to quetiapine 200 mg per day, quetiapine 100 mg per day, or placebo for a 10-week period (Zhong et al. 2007). Finally, quetiapine and haloperidol appeared both superior to placebo and effective in controlling psychotic symptoms in a large study conducted on 284 patients with AD and BPSD randomized to flexible doses of quetiapine (median daily dose 96.9 mg), haloperidol (median daily dose 1.9 mg), or placebo for 10 weeks (Tariot et al. 2006). Quetiapine resulted also better tolerated than haloperidol, which was associated with an increased risk of parkinsonism.
104
Dementia
To date, a very limited number of RCTs have examined the effect of aripiprazole on BPSD. Aripiprazole at a mean dose of 10 mg per day appeared as beneficial as placebo on delusions and hallucinations and well tolerated in a 10-week study conducted on 208 outpatients with AD (De Deyn et al. 2005). More recently, the efficacy of aripiprazole has been documented in a large RCT conducted on 487 institutionalized patients with AD and BPSD for 10 weeks (Mintzer et al. 2007). In this study, compared with placebo, aripiprazole was effective at a dose of 10 mg per day for controlling psychotic symptoms and agitation. Finally, aripiprazole 15 mg per day was superior to placebo and effective for treating behavioral symptoms, agitation, anxiety, and depression in 10-week study on 256 institutionalized patients with BPSD. In this study, aripiprazole showed no effect on psychotic symptoms (Steim et al. 2008). According to the CATIE-AD (Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness–Alzheimer ’s Disease), a large multicenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled, effectiveness trial on outpatients with AD and psychosis, aggression or agitation, olanzapine (mean dose 5.5 mg per day) and risperidone (mean dose 1.0 mg per day) for the treatment of BSPD were equally beneficial and superior to placebo and quetiapine (mean dose 56.5 mg per day) (Schnedier et al. 2006). However, these benefits were evident only among those patients who tolerated these medications and did not discontinue them due to side effects. According to authors’ conclusions potential side effects associated with antipsychotic medications in dementia may outweigh possible benefits. A comprehensive review of the available placebo-controlled RCTs has been conducted by Ballard and White for the Cochrane collaboration to determine the effectiveness of atypical antipsychotics for the treatment of psychiatric and behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease (Ballard and Waite 2006). The authors analyzed 16 placebo-controlled RCTs among which only 6 studies were published in full in peerreviewed journals at the time of completion of the review. According to the Cochrane authors, evidence suggests that both risperidone and olanzapine may reduce aggression and risperidone may also reduce psychosis compared to placebo. However, an increased risk of extrapyramidal symptoms and adverse cerebrovascular events associated with atypical antipsychotics would outweigh the modest effectiveness of these medications. For these reasons, authors conclude that the use of atypical antipsychotics in clinical practice would not be suitable and should be limited to those patients presenting with significant distress and risks associated with BPSD.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
105
SAFETY OF ANTIPSYCHOTICS The use of conventional antipsychotics in patients with dementia is problematic because elderly individuals are particularly susceptible to adverse reactions even at therapeutic dosages. In particular, the use of conventional drugs is frequently associated with acute confusion, delirium, sedation, EPS and tardive dyskinesia. Moreover, a high risk of serious cardiovascular adverse reactions such as ventricular arrhythmias and cardiac arrest has been associated with conventional antipsychotics (Kapur et al. 2000; Farde et al. 1992). At the time of their introduction in clinical practice, atypical antipsychotics were described as less-harmful compounds compared with conventional agents, with a better safety profile relative to the old medications, especially with respect to parkinsonism and tardive dyskinesia. Evidence from RCTs confirmed the superior EPS profile of atypical relative to conventional antipsychotics (Brodaty et al. 2003; De Deyn et al. 2004; Street et al. 2000). Risperidone at a dose of 1 mg per day was proven to cause less EPS compared with placebo and haloperidol (Katz et al. 1999; De Deyn et al. 1999; Brodaty et al. 2003). However, this relative benefit of risperidone disappeared at dosages of 2 mg per day or higher (Katz et al. 1999). No increased incidence of EPS in the olanzapine groups (at doses of 5 to 15 mg per day) compared with the placebo group was found in two randomized trials (De Deyn et al. 2004; Street et al. 2000). Overall, the available evidence suggests that, in dementia patients, EPS are less frequently associated with atypical antipsychotics relative to conventional agents. However, EPS and tardive dyskinesia may be caused by atypical antipsychotics, especially at high dosages. Warnings about a possible increased risk of cerebrovascular events (CVEs) and death among patients with dementia being treated with risperidone or olanzapine have been issued by drugs’ manufacturers and health regulatory agencies worldwide since 2002 (Wooltorton 2002, 2004; Hermann and Lanctôt 2005). These concerns arose from re-analyses of both published and unpublished clinical trials on atypical antipsychotics among patients with dementia. According to the results of these analyses, risperidone was associated with a nearly threefold increase in the risk of CVEs in patients with dementia. However, the risk associated with risperidone was similar to that of placebo when serious CVEs including death, life-threatening events, or events leading to permanent disability were considered. Also, a nearly twofold increase in the risk of CVEs associated with olanzapine was calculated but this estimate did not reach statistical significance. In spite of this evidence from clinical trials, data from several
106
Dementia
large observational studies failed to support the conclusion of a possible increased risk of CVEs and death associated with atypical antipsychotics (Liperoti et al. 2005, “Conventional and atypical antipsychotics”; Hermann, Mamdani, and Lanctôt 2004; Gill et al. 2005). Following the dissemination of these findings, a meta-analysis of 15 randomized clinical trials on patients with dementia being treated with atypical antipsychotics was published (Schneider, Dagerman, and Insel 2005). According to the results of this meta-analysis, atypical antipsychotics may be associated with a 65% increased all cause mortality compared with placebo. In April 2005, the FDA reviewed 17 RCTs involving risperidone, olanzapine, quetiapine, and aripiprazole, and issued a public health advisory to warn about a 1.6 to 1.7 times higher risk of all cause mortality associated with these drugs relative to placebo (FDA Public Health Advisory 2005). However, these studies were characterized by short duration and small sample size so that reliable estimates of the risk of death could be generated only when data were combined together in a meta-analysis. The FDA also asked manufacturers of these drugs to include a boxed warning in their labeling describing the risk and reminded that atypical antipsychotics were not approved for dementia, which represents an offlabel indication. Findings from a large discontinuation trial conducted on 165 patients with AD treated with conventional and atypical antipsychotics have been published (Ballard et al. 2009). In this study, participants were randomized to either continue antipsychotics or stop them and receive placebo. Excess mortality was documented among those who continued antipsychotic treatment compared to those who were on placebo at 12, 24 and 36 months of follow-up. More recently, some large observational studies have suggested that conventional antipsychotics may pose an even greater risk of death compared with atypical agents (Wang et al. 2005; Hollis et al. 2007; Kales et al. 2007; Schneeweiss et al. 2007; Gill et al. 2007; Liperoti et al. 2009). A large retrospective cohort study conducted among 22,890 elderly patients newly prescribed with antipsychotics suggested that conventional antipsychotics may indeed carry a nearly 40% excess risk of death compared with atypical (Wang et al. 2005). The excess mortality associated with conventional antipsychotics was 29% in the subgroup of patients with dementia. A population-based study from Ontario conducted among 27,259 patients with dementia has reported an increased risk for death associated with conventional relative to atypical antipsychotics at 30 days of treatment among communitydwelling elderly patients with dementia (31% excess mortality) and among nursing home residents with dementia (55% excess mortality) (Gill et al.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
107
2007). A 47% increased risk of death associated with conventional relative to atypical drugs was also documented in a cohort of 37,241 elderly individuals treated with antipsychotics (Schneeweiss et al. 2007). The greatest risk of death was found during the first 40 days of treatment and for those patients receiving antipsychotics at high doses. In interpreting these findings, it should be taken in consideration that only 10–12% of patients in the cohort had dementia and that the estimated excess mortality linked to conventional antipsychotics was 26% in the subgroup of patients with dementia. Consistent with these findings, a recent cohort study has documented a 26% increased risk of death associated with conventional antipsychotics compared with atypical agents in a sample of 9,279 U.S. nursing home residents with dementia (Liperoti et al. 2009). Only one study including 2,385 patients with dementia has documented similar mortality risks for atypical and conventional antipsychotics (Trifirò et al. 2007). In June 2008, following such observational evidence, the FDA warned healthcare professionals on a possible increased risk of death associated with conventional antipsychotics, therefore extending to these drugs the previously issued warning of excess mortality associated with the use of atypical medications in patients with dementia (FDA Alert 2008). The mechanisms by which antipsychotic medications may contribute to CVEs and death remain to be established. In the FDA meta-analysis, most deaths were due to cardiovascular events (mostly arrhythmias) and infections (pneumonia for the great majority). Treatment with both atypical and conventional antipsychotics has been linked to some risk of lengthening of QTc interval at EKG (Hennessy et al. 2002; Straus et al. 2004). However, an increased risk of clinical outcomes related to QTc prolongation, including ventricular arrhythmias and sudden death, has been documented for conventional antipsychotics but not for atypical agents (Ray et al. 2001; Liperoti et al. 2005, “Conventional and atypical antipsychotics”). Sedation and EPS may contribute to swallowing problems, falls, and, in turn, to an increased risk of pneumonia and subsequent death. Finally, abnormalities of coagulation, increased platelet aggregation and accelerated atherosclerosis have also been suggested to be some mechanism potentially mediating antipsychotic induced CVEs (Canoso, de Oliveira, and Nixon 1990; Davis, Kern, and Asokan 1994; Chengappa et al. 1991; Metzer, Canoso, and Newton 1994; Boullin et al. 1975). While several mechanisms may be in place, individual susceptibility seems to be the strongest determinant of antipsychotic-induced mortality in patients with dementia. In particular, factors such as a previous history of stroke and pre-existing cardiovascular risk factors have been suggested to interact with antipsychotics on increasing the risk of stroke and death (Liperoti et al. 2005, “Conventional and atypical antipsychotics,” 2009).
108
Dementia
A possible association between venous thromboembolism (VTE) and conventional antipsychotics was first suggested in the 1950s among young patients with schizophrenia (Häfner and Brehm 1965; Zornberg and Jick 2000). Similarly, atypical antipsychotics have been recently linked to a possible increased risk of venous thromboembolism in a large observational study of elderly patients (Liperoti et al. 2005, “Cerebrovascular events among elderly”). Abnormalities of platelet aggregation have been indicated has the possible underlying mechanism (Boullin et al. 1975; Wallaschofski et al. 2003). Observational data suggest that a similar increased risk of fall and hip fracture may be associated with the use of conventional and atypical antipsychotics in elderly patients (Liperoti et al. 2007). Antipsychotics may exert these adverse effects through multiple mechanisms. Antipsychotic medications may induce EPS and consequent gait disturbances; they can also lead to an increased risk of acute confusion, delirium, sedation, and orthostatic hypotension. All of these may in turn determine an increased risk of fall and consequent hip fracture. Finally, atypical antipsychotics are known to cause a spectrum of metabolic adverse effects such as diabetes, hyperlipidemia, and weight gain among young and adult patients with schizophrenia (Melkersson and Dahl 2004). To date, there is little evidence that patients with dementia being treated with atypical antipsychotics may experience such metabolic effects (Katz et al. 1999; De Deyn et al. 1999; Brodaty et al. 2003; De Deyn et al. 2004; Street et al. 2000). Also no data are available to investigate the extent to which such metabolic disturbances may contribute to the possible cardiovascular toxicity associated with atypical antipsychotics. CONCLUSIONS BPSD represent one of the most urgent mental issues in the geriatric population. Despite the growing number of elderly individuals with dementia and BPSD, there is limited evidence supporting available therapeutic strategies and current recommendations mainly derive from consensus of experts (Benoit et al. 2006; Alexopoulos et al. 2005; Expert Consensus Panel for Dementia 2005). A summary of the to-date-available recommendations for treating BPSD is reported in Table 5.3. To date, antipsychotics represent the mainstay of pharmacological treatment for BPSD. However, evidence supporting their efficacy in patients with dementia is limited and controversial. Also, growing concern for safety makes particularly problematic the use of these medications in elderly individuals. To date, antipsychotics are used off-label
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
109
Table 5.3 Clinical Recommendations for Physicians to Treat Patients with BPSD 1. Identify and assess target symptoms: type, frequency, intensity, trigger factors. 2. Identify and remove possible reversible factors contributing to BPSD (e.g.. pain, constipation, dehydration, environmental factors). 3. Share decisionmaking with patients and caregivers. 4. Identify time frame in which to evaluate effectiveness of treatment. 5. Adopt nonpharmacological intervention as first line treatment. 6. Reserve pharmacological treatment for those patients who did not improve after nonpharmacological interventions and who manifest severe symptoms that may cause extreme distress and harm to themselves or others. 7. Discuss risks and benefits of antipsychotic therapy with patients and caregivers during the informed consent process and educate caregivers about monitoring safety and actions to take in case of adverse reactions. 8. Use antipsychotics for the short-term treatment (up to three months). 9. Tailor on the individual patient the choice of a specific antipsychotic, taking into account the potential benefits and risks of both classes of drugs as well as the individual cardiovascular risk profile. 10. Use the lowest dosage required and for the shortest time period necessary. 11. Monitor safety (baseline and regular follow-up physical assessment, laboratory tests and EKG). 12. Regularly attempt to discontinue antipsychotic treatment even in those patients who initially manifested improvement. Source: Benoit et al. 2006; Alexopoulos et al. 2005; Expert Consensus Panel for Dementia 2005.
for the treatment of BPSD and their prescription is limited and strictly regulated in many countries. Nonpharmacological approaches are generally recognized as the first-line strategy for the treatment of BPSD. The pharmacological approach based on the use of antipsychotic medications is recommended for the short-term treatment (up to three months) and among those patients who manifest severe symptoms that may cause extreme distress and harm to themselves or others. If treatment with antipsychotics is deemed necessary, the therapeutic choice between atypical and conventional medications should be based on a careful evaluation of the potential benefits and risks of both classes of drugs as well as patients’ individual risk profile. Moreover, regular attempts to discontinue treatment should be performed. Finally, future research efforts should be directed to identify more effective and less harmful pharmacological alternatives to antipsychotics and to promote combined interventions which may include nonpharmacological treatment and drug therapy, both tailored on the individual patient.
110
Dementia
REFERENCES Alexopoulos, G. S., D. V. Jeste, H. Chung, D. Carpenter, R. Ross, and J. P. Docherty. 2005. The expert consensus guideline series. Treatment of dementia and its behavioral disturbances. Introduction: methods, commentary, and summary. Postgrad Med (Spec. No.): 6–22. Alzheimer, A. 1906. Uber einen eigenartigen schweren Erkrankungsprozeb der Hirnrinde. Neurologisches Centralblatt 1906; 23: 1129–1136. Alzheimer Disease International. 2009. Alzheimer world report 2009. http:// www.alz.org/national/documents/report_summary_2009worldalzheime rreport.pdf (accessed January 7, 2010). Alzheimer ’s Association. 2009. Alzheimer ’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement 5 (3): 234–270. American Geriatrics Society and American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. 2003. Consensus statement on improving the quality of mental health care in U.S. nursing homes: Management of depression and behavioral symptoms associated with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 51: 1287–1298. American Geriatrics Society and American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry. 2003. Recommendations for policies in support of quality mental health care in U.S. nursing homes. J Am Geriatr Soc 51: 1299–1304. Baldessarini, R. J., and F. R. Frankenburg. 1991. Clozapine: A novel antipsychotic agent. N Engl J Med 324: 746–754. Ballard, C., M. L. Hanney, M. Theodoulou, S. Douglas, R. McShane, K. Kossakowski, R. Gill, et al. 2009. The dementia antipsychotic withdrawal trial (DART-AD): Long-term follow-up of a randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 8 (2): 151–157. Ballard, C., M. Margallo-Lana, E. Juszczak, S. Douglas, A. Swann, A. Thomas, J. O’Brien, et al. 2005. Quetiapine and rivastigmine and cognitive decline in Alzheimer ’s disease: Randomised double blind placebo controlled trial. BMJ 330 (7496): 874. Ballard, C., and J. Waite. 2006. The effectiveness of atypical antipsychotics for the treatment of aggression and psychosis in Alzheimer ’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (1): CD003476. Barnes, R., R. Veith, J. Okimoto, M. Raskind, and G. Gumbrecht. 1982. Efficacy of antipsychotic medications in behaviorally disturbed dementia patients. Am J Psychiatry 139 (9): 1170–1174. Benoit, M., C. Arbus, F. Blanchard, et al. 2006. Professional consensus on the treatment of agitation, aggressive behaviour, oppositional behaviour and psychotic disturbances in dementia. J Nutr Health Aging 10: 410–415. Benoit, M., P. H. Robert, P. Staccini, P. Brocker, O. Guerin, L. Lechowski, B. Vellas, and REAL.FR Group. 2005. One-year longitudinal evaluation of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease. The REAL.FR Study. J Nutr Health Aging 9 (2): 95–99. Borroni, B., C. Costanzi, and A. Padovani. 2010. Genetic susceptibility to behavioural and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 7 (2): 158–164.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
111
Boullin, D. J., H. F. Woods, R. P. Grimes, and D. G. Grahame-Smith. 1975. Increased platelet aggregation responses to 5-hydroxytryptamine in patients taking chlorpromazine. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2: 29–35. Bozeat, S., C. A. Gregory, M. A. Ralph, and J. R. Hodges. 2000. Which neuropsychiatric and behavioural features distinguish frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer ’s disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 69 (2): 178–186. Brodaty, H., D. Ames, J. Snowdon, M. Woodward, J. Kirwan, R. Clarnette, E. Lee, B. Lyons, and F. Grossman. 2003. A randomized placebo-controlled trial of risperidone for the treatment of aggression, agitation, and psychosis of dementia. J Clin Psychiatry 64 (2): 134–143. Burris, K. D., T. F. Molski, C. Xu, E. Ryan, K. Tottori, T. Kikuchi, F. D. Yocca, and P. B. Molinoff. 2002. Aripiprazole, a novel antipsychotic, is a high-affinity partial agonist at human dopamine D2 receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 302: 381–389. Canoso, R. T., R. M. de Oliveira, and R. A. Nixon. 1990. Neuroleptic-associated autoantibodies. A prevalence study. Biol Psychiatry 27: 863–870. Chan, D. C., J. D. Kasper, B. S. Black, and P. V. Rabins. 2003. Prevalence and correlates of behavioral and psychiatric symptoms in community-dwelling elders with dementia or mild cognitive impairment: The Memory and Medical Care Study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 18 (2): 174–182. Chengappa, K. N., A. B. Carpenter, M. S. Keshavan, Z. W. Yang, R. H. Kelly, B. S. Rabin, and R. Ganguli. 1991. Elevated IGG and IGM anticardiolipin antibodies in a subgroup of medicated and unmedicated schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 30: 731–735. Citrome, L. 2009. Iloperidone for schizophrenia: A review of the efficacy and safety profile for this newly commercialised second-generation antipsychotic. Int J Clin Pract 63 (8): 1237–1248. Cohen, C. A., D. P. Gold, K. I. Shulman, J. T. Wortley, G. McDonald, and M. Wargon. 1993. Factors determining the decision to institutionalize dementing individuals: A prospective study. Gerontologist 33: 714–720. Collaborative Working Group on Clinical Trial Evaluations. 1998. Treatment of special populations with the atypical antipsychotics. J Clin Psychiatry 59 (Suppl. 12): 46–52. Cummings, J. L., C. Diaz, M. Levy, G. Binetti, and I. I. Litvan. 1996. Neuropsychiatric syndromes in neurodegenerative disease: Frequency and significance. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 1 (4): 241–247. Davis, S., H. B. Kern, and R. Asokan. 1994. Antiphospholipid antibodies associated with clozapine treatment. Am J Hematol 46: 166–167. De Deyn, P. P., M. M. Carrasco, W. Deberdt, C. Jeandel, D. P. Hay, P. D. Feldman, C. A. Young, D. L. Lehman, and A. Breier. 2004. Olanzapine versus placebo in the treatment of psychosis with or without associated behavioral disturbances in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 19 (2): 115–126. De Deyn, P., D. V. Jeste, R. Swanink, D. Kostic, C. Breder, W. H. Carson, and T. Iwamoto. 2005. Aripiprazole for the treatment of psychosis in patients
112
Dementia
with Alzheimer ’s disease: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 25 (5): 463–467. De Deyn, P. P., K. Rabheru, A. Rasmussen, J. P. Bocksberger, P. L. Dautzenberg, S. Eriksson, and B. A. Lawlor. 1999. A randomized trial of risperidone, placebo, and haloperidol for behavioral symptoms of dementia. Neurology 53 (5): 946–955. Deberdt, W. G., M. W. Dysken, S. A. Rappaport, P. D. Feldman, C. A. Young, D. P. Hay, D. L. Lehman, M. Dossenbach, E. K. Degenhardt, and A. Breier. 2005. Comparison of olanzapine and risperidone in the treatment of psychosis and associated behavioral disturbances in patients with dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13 (8): 722–730. Engelborghs, S., E. Vloeberghs, N. Le Bastard, M. Van Buggenhout, P. Mariën, N. Somers, G. Nagels, B. A. Pickut, and P. P. De Deyn. 2008. The dopaminergic neurotransmitter system is associated with aggression and agitation in frontotemporal dementia. Neurochem Int 52 (6): 1052–1060. Expert Consensus Panel for Dementia. 2005. The expert consensus guideline series. Treatment of dementia and its behavioral disturbances. Postgrad Med (Spec. No.): 1–111. Factor, S. A. 2002. Pharmacology of atypical antipsychotics. Clin Neuropharmacol 25: 153–157. Farber, N. B., E. H. Rubin, J. W. Newcomer, D. A. Kinscherf, J. P. Miller, J. C. Morris, J. W. Olney, D. W. McKeel Jr. 2000. Increased neocortical neurofibrillary tangle density in subjects with Alzheimer disease and psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 1165–1173. Farde, L., A. L. Nordstrom, F. A. Wiesel, S. Pauli, C. Halldin, and G. Sedvall. 1992. Positron emission tomographic analysis of central D1 and D2 dopamine receptor occupancy in patients treated with classical neuroleptics and clozapine. Relation to extrapyramidal side effects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49: 538–544. FDA Alert. 2008 (June 16). Information for health care professionals. Antipsychotics, conventional and atypical. http://www.fda.gov/Drugs/DrugSafety/Post marketDrugSafetyInformationforPatientsandProviders/DrugSafetyInfor mationforHeathcareProfessionals/PublicHealthAdvisories/UCM053171 (accessed January 24, 2011). FDA Public Health Advisory. 2005 (April 11). Deaths with antipsychotics in elderly patients with behavioral disturbances. http://www.fda.gov/cder/drug/ advisory/antipsychotics.htm (accessed April 11, 2005). Finkel, S. 2000. Introduction to behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 15 (Suppl. 1): S2–S4. Finkel, S. 2002. Behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia: Assisting the caregiver and managing the patient. Geriatrics 57 (11): 44–46. Fischman, A. J., A. A. Bonab, J. W. Babich, et al. 1996. Positron emission tomographic analysis of central 5-hydroxytryptamine2 receptor occupancy in healthy volunteers treated with the novel antipsychotic agent ziprasidone. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 279: 939–947.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
113
Fitten, L. J. 2006. Management of behavioral and psychiatric symptoms of dementia. J Nutr Health Aging 10: 409. Forstl, H., A. Burns, R. Levy, and N. Cairns. 1994. Neuropathological correlates of psychotic phenomena in confirmed Alzheimer ’s disease. Br J Psychiatry 165: 53–59. Gerlach, J. 2000. Atypical antipsychotics: An inspiring but confusing concept. Psychopharmacology 148: 1–2. Gill, S. S., S. E. Bronskill, S. L. Normand, G. M. Anderson, K. Sykora, K. Lam, C. M. Bell, et al. 2007. Antipsychotic drug use and mortality in older adults with dementia. Ann Intern Med 146: 775–786. Gill, S. S., P. A. Rochon, N. Herrmann, P. E. Lee, K. Sykora, N. Gunraj, S. L. Normand, et al. 2005. Atypical antipsychotic drugs and risk of ischaemic stroke: Population based retrospective cohort study. BMJ 330 (7489): 445. Glick, I. D., S. R. Murray, P. Vasudevan, S. R. Marder, and R. J. Hu. 2001. Treatment with atypical antipsychotics: New indications and new populations. J Psychiatr Res 35: 187–191. Häfner, H., and I. Brehm. 1965. Thromboembolic complications in neuroleptic treatment. Compr Psychiatry 6: 25–34. Harvey, R. J., D. Ellison, J. Hardy, M. Hutton., P. K. Roques, J. Collinge, N. C. Fox, and M. N. Rossor. 1998. Chromosome 14 familial Alzheimer ’s disease: The clinical and neuropathological characteristics of a family with a leucineserine (L250S) substitution at codon 250 of the presenilin 1 gene. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 64: 44–49. Hennessy, S., W. B. Bilker, J. S. Knauss, D. J. Margolis, S. E. Kimmel, R. F. Reynolds, D. B. Glasser, M. F. Morrison, and B. L. Strom. 2002. Cardiac arrest and ventricular arrhythmia in patients taking antipsychotic drugs: Cohort study using administrative data. BMJ 325: 1070–1074. Herrmann, N., K. L. Lanctôt, and L. R. Khan. 2004. The role of norepinephrine in the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 16 (3): 261–276. Herrmann, N., K. L. Lanctôt, R. Sambrook, N. Lesnikova, R. Hébert, P. McCracken, A. Robillard, and E. Nguyen. 2006. The contribution of neuropsychiatric symptoms to the cost of dementia care. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 21 (10): 972–976. Herrmann, N., and K. L. Lanctôt. 2005. Do atypical antipsychotics cause stroke? CNS Drugs 19 (2): 91–103. Herrmann, N., M. Mamdani, and K. L. Lanctôt. 2004. Atypical antipsychotics and risk of cerebrovascular accidents. Am J Psychiatry 161 (6): 1113–1115. Hersch, E. C., and S. Falzgraf. 2007. Management of the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Clin Interv Aging 2 (4): 611–621. Hertel, P. 2006. Comparing sertindole to other new generation antipsychotics on preferential dopamine output in limbic versus striatal projection regions: Mechanism of action. Synapse 60 (7): 543–552. Hollis, J., L. Grayson Forrester, H. Brodaty, S. Touyz, and R. Cumming. 2007. Antipsychotics medication dispensing and risk of death in veterans and war widows 65 years and older. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 15: 932–941.
114
Dementia
Holmes, C., M. J. Arranz, J. F. Powell, D. A. Collier, and S. Lovestone. 1998. 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor polymorphisms and psychopathology in late onset Alzheimer ’s disease. Hum Mol Genet 7: 1507–1509. Hulme, C., J. Wright, T. Crocker, Y. Oluboyede, and A. House. 2010. Non-pharmacological approaches for dementia that informal carers might try or access: A systematic review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 25 (7): 756–763. Jeste, D. V., D. Blazer, D. Casey, T. Meeks, C. Salzman, L. Schneider, P. Tariot, and K. Yaffe. 2008. ACNP White Paper: Update on use of antipsychotic drugs in elderly persons with dementia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33 (5): 957–970. Jibson, M. D., and R. Tandon. 1998. New atypical antipsychotic medications. J Psychiatr Res 32: 215–228. Kales, H. C., M. Valenstein, H. Myra Kim, J. F. McCarthy, D. Ganoczy, F. Cunningham, and F. C. Blow. 2007. Mortality risk in patients with dementia treated with antipsychotics versus other psychiatric medications. Am J Psychiatry 164: 1568–1576. Kapur, S., G. Remington, R. B. Zipursky, A. A. Wilson, and S. Houle. 1995. The D2 dopamine receptor occupancy of risperidone and its relationship to extrapyramidal symptoms: A PET study. Life Sci 57: L103–L107. Kapur, S., and P. Seeman. 2001. Does fast dissociation from the dopamine D2 receptor explain the action of atypical antipsychotics?: A new hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158: 360–369. Kapur, S., R. Zipursky, C. Jones, G. Remington, and S. Houle. 2000. Relationship between dopamine D2 occupancy, clinical response, and side effects: A double-blind PET study of first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 157: 514–520. Kapur, S., R. B. Zipursky, and G. Remington. 1999. Clinical and theoretical implications of 5-HT2 and D2 receptor occupancy of clozapine, risperidone, and olanzapine in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 156: 286–293. Katz, I. R., D. V. Jeste, J. E. Mintzer, C. Clyde, J. Napolitano, and M. Brecher. 1999. Comparison of risperidone and placebo for psychosis and behavioral disturbances associated with dementia: A randomized, double-blind trial. Risperidone Study Group. J Clin Psychiatry 60 (2): 107–115. Kurlan, R., J. Cummings, R. Raman, L. Thal, and Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study Group. 2007. Quetiapine for agitation or psychosis in patients with dementia and parkinsonism. Neurology 68 (17): 1356–1363. Lanctôt, K. L., N. Herrmann, and P. Mazzotta. 2001. Role of serotonin in the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 13 (1): 5–21. Levy, M. L., J. L. Cummings, L. A. Fairbanks, D. Bravi, M. Calvani, and A. Carta. 1996. Longitudinal assessment of symptoms of depression, agitation, and psychosis in 181 patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 153 (11): 1438–1443. Lidsky, T. I., E. Yablonsky-Alter, L. Zuck, and S. P. Banerjee. 1993. Anti-glutamatergic effects of clozapine. Neurosci Lett 163: 155–158.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
115
Liperoti, R., G. Gambassi, K. L. Lapane, C. Chiang, C. Pedone, V. Mor, and R. Bernabei. 2005. Cerebrovascular events among elderly nursing home patients treated with conventional or atypical antipsychotics. J Clin Psychiatry 66 (9): 1090–1096. Liperoti, R., G. Gambassi, K. L. Lapane, C. Chiang, C. Pedone, V. Mor, and R. Bernabei. 2005. Conventional and atypical antipsychotics and the risk of hospitalization for ventricular arrhythmias or cardiac arrest. Arch Intern Med 165 (6): 696–701. Liperoti, R., V. Mor, K. L. Lapane, C. Pedone, G. Gambassi, and R. Bernabei. 2003. The use of atypical antipsychotics in nursing home. J Clin Psychiatry 64: 1106–1112. Liperoti, R., G. Onder, F. Landi, K. L. Lapane, V. Mor, R. Bernabei, and G. Gambassi. 2009. All-cause mortality associated with atypical and conventional antipsychotics among nursing home residents with dementia: A retrospective cohort study. J Clin Psychiatry 70 (10): 1340–1347. Liperoti, R., G. Onder, K. L. Lapane, V. Mor, J. H. Friedman, R. Bernabei, and G. Gambassi. 2007. Conventional or atypical antipsychotics and the risk of femur fracture among elderly patients: Results of a case-control study. J Clin Psychiatry 68 (6): 929–934. Liperoti, R., C. Pedone, K. L. Lapane, V. Mor, R. Bernabei, and G. Gambassi. 2005. Venous thromboembolism among elderly patients treated with atypical and conventional antipsychotic agents. Arch Intern Med 165 (22): 2677–2682. Lonergan, E., J. Luxenberg, and J. Colford. 2002. Haloperidol for agitation in dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2): CD002852. Lopez, O. L., G. Smith, J. T. Becker, C. C. Meltzer, and S. T. DeKosky. 2001. The psychotic phenomenon in probable Alzheimer ’s disease: A positron emission tomography study. J Neuropsychiat Clin Neurosci 13: 50–55. Lyketsos, C. G., O. Lopez, B. Jones, A. L. Fitzpatrick, J. Breitner, and S. DeKosky. 2002. Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: Results from the cardiovascular health study. JAMA 288 (12): 1475–1483. Madhusoodanan, S., P. Shah, R. Brenner, and S. Gupta. 2007. Pharmacological treatment of the psychosis of Alzheimer ’s disease: What is the best approach? CNS Drugs 21 (2): 101–115. Madhusoodanan, S., and D. Zaveri. 2010. Paliperidone use in the elderly. Curr Drug Saf 5 (2): 149–152. Meins, W., A. Frey, and R. Thiesemann. 2008. Premorbid personality traits in Alzheimer ’s disease: Do they predispose to noncognitive behavioral symptoms? Int. Psychogeriatr 10: 369–378. Melkersson, K., and M. L. Dahl. 2004. Adverse metabolic effects associated with atypical antipsychotics: Literature review and clinical implications. Drugs 64: 701–723. Meltzer, H. Y., S. Matsubara, and J. C. Lee. 1989. Classification of typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs on the basis of dopamine D-1, D-2 and serotonin2 pKi values. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 251: 238–246.
116
Dementia
Metzer, W. S., R. T. Canoso, and J. E. Newton. 1994. Anticardiolipin antibodies in a sample of chronic schizophrenics receiving neuroleptic therapy. South Med J 87: 190–192. Mintzer, J. E., L. E. Tune, C. D. Breder, R. Swanink, R. N. Marcus, R. D. McQuade, and A. Forbes. 2007. Aripiprazole for the treatment of psychoses in institutionalized patients with Alzheimer dementia: A multicenter, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled assessment of three fixed doses. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 15 (11): 918–931. Mossman, D., and D. S. Lehrer. 2000. Conventional and atypical antipsychotics and the evolving standard of care. Psychiatric Serv 51: 1528–1535. Nordström, A. L., L. Farde, F. A. Wiesel, K. Forslund, S. Pauli, C. Halldin, and G. Uppfeldt. 1993. Central D2-dopamine receptor occupancy in relation to antipsychotic drug effects: A double-blind PET study of schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 33: 227–235. Paleacu, D., Y. Barak, I. Mirecky, and D. Mazeh. 2008. Quetiapine treatment for behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in Alzheimer ’s disease patients: A 6-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 23 (4): 393–400. Ray, W. A., S. Meredith, P. B. Thapa, K. G. Meador, K. Hall, and K. T. Murray. 2001. Antipsychotics and the risk of sudden cardiac death. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58: 1161–1167. Richelson, E. 1996. Preclinical pharmacology of neuroleptics: Focus on new generation compounds. J Clin Psychiatry 57 (Suppl. 11): 4–11. Ruths, S., J. Straand, H. A. Nygaard, and D. Aatsland. 2008. Stopping antipsychotic drug therapy in demented nursing home patients: A randomized, placebocontrolled study—the Bergen District Nursing Home Study (BEDNURS). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 23 (9): 889–895. Scarmeas, N., J. Brandt, M. Albert, G. Hadjigeorgiou, A. Papadimitriou, B. Dubois, M. Sarazin, et al. 2005. Delusions and hallucinations are associated with worse outcome in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 62 (10): 1601–1608. Schneeweiss, S., S. Setoguchi, A. Brookhart, C. Dormuth, and P. S. Wang. 2007. Risk of death associated with the use of conventional versus atypical antipsychotic drugs among elderly patients. CMAJ 176 (5): 627–632. Schneider, L. S., K. S. Dagerman, and P. Insel. 2005. Risk of death with atypical antipsychotic drug treatment for dementia: Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. JAMA 294 (15): 1934–1943. Schneider, L. S., V. E. Pollock, and S. A. Lyness. 1990. A meta-analysis of controlled trials of neuroleptic treatment in dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 38 (5): 553–563. Schneider, L. S., P. N. Tariot, K. S. Dagerman, S. M. Davis, J. K. Hsiao, M. S. Ismail, B. D. Lebowitz, et al. 2006. Effectiveness of atypical antipsychotic drugs in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 355 (15): 1525–1538. Schoemaker, H., Y. Claustre, D. Fage, et al. 1997. Neurochemical characteristics of amisulpride, an atypical dopamine D2/D3 receptor antagonist with both presynaptic and limbic selectivity. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 280: 83–97.
Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia
117
Steim, J. E., A. P. Porsteinsson, C. D. Breder, R. Swanink, R. Marcus, R. McQuade, and W. H. Carson. 2008. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of aripiprazole for the treatment of psychosis in nursing home patients with Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 16 (7): 537–550. Straus, S. M., G. S. Bleumink, J. P. Dieleman, J. van der Lei, G. W. ‘t Jong, J. H. Kingma, M. C. Sturkenboom, and B. H. Stricker. 2004. Antipsychotics and the risk of sudden cardiac death. Arch Intern Med 164: 1293–1297. Street, J. S., W. S. Clark, K. S. Gannon, J. L. Cummings, F. P. Bymaster, R. N. Tamura, S. J. Mitan, et al. 2000. Olanzapine treatment of psychotic and behavioral symptoms in patients with Alzheimer disease in nursing care facilities: A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. The HGEU Study Group. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57 (10): 968–976. Sultzer, D. L., C. V. Brown, M. A. Mandelkern, M. E. Mahler, M. F. Mendez, S. T. Chen, and J. L. Cummings. 2003. Delusional thoughts and regional frontal/temporal cortex metabolism in Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 160: 341–349. Sweet, R. A., V. L. Nimgaonkar, M. I. Kamboh, O. L. Lopez, F. Zhang, and S. T. DeKosky. 1998. Dopamine receptor genetic variation, psychosis, and aggression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 55: 1335–1340. Sweet, R. A., B. G. Pollock, D. L. Sukonick, B. H. Mulsant, J. Rosen, W. E. Klunk, K. B. Kastango, S. T. DeKosky, and R. E. Ferrell. 2001. The 5-HTTPR polymorphism confers liability to a combined phenotype of psychotic and aggressive behavior in Alzheimer disease. Int J Psychogeriatr 13: 401–409. Tariot, P. N., L. Schneider, I. R. Katz, J. E. Mintzer, J. Street, M. Copenhayer, and C. Williams-Hughes. 2006. Quetiapine treatment of psychosis associated with dementia: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 14 (9): 767–776. Tekin, S., M. S. Mega, D. M. Masterman, T. Chow, J. Garakian, H. V. Vinters, and J. L. Cummings. 2001. Orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex neurofibrillary tangle burden is associated with agitation in Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol 49 (3): 355–361. Trifirò, G., K. M. Verhamme, G. Ziere, A. P. Caputi, B. H. Ch. Stricker, and M. C. Sturkenboom. 2007. All-cause mortality associated with atypical and typical antipsychotics in demented outpatients. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf 16: 538–544. van der Cammen, T. J., H. Tiemeier, M. J. Engelhart, and D. Fekkes. 2006. Abnormal neurotransmitter metabolite levels in Alzheimer patients with a delirium. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 21 (9): 838–843. Wallaschofski, H., M. Eigenthaler, M. Kiefer, M. Donne, B. Hentschel, H. J. Gertz, and T. Lohmann. 2003. Hyperprolactinemia in patients on antipsychotic drugs causes ADP-stimulated platelet activation that might explain the increased risk for venous thromboembolism: Pilot study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 23 (5): 479–483.
118
Dementia
Wang, P. S., S. Schneeweiss, J. Avorn, M. A. Fischer, H. Mogun, D. H. Solomon, and M. A. Brookhart. 2005. Risk of death in elderly users of conventional vs. atypical antipsychotic medications. N Engl J Med 353: 2335–2341. Wooltorton, E. 2002. Risperidone (Risperdal): Increased rate of cerebrovascular events in dementia trials. CMAJ 167: 1269–1270. Wooltorton, E. 2004. Olanzapine (Zyprexa): Increased rate of cerebrovascular events in dementia trials. CMAJ 170: 1395. Xiberas, X., J. L. Martinot, L. Mallet, E. Artiges, C. Loc’H, B. Mazière, and M. L. Paillère-Martinot. 2001. Extrastriatal and striatal D2 dopamine receptor blockade with haloperidol or new antipsychotic drugs in patients with schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 179: 503–508. Zhong, K. X., P. N. Tariot, J. Mintzer, M. C. Minkwitz, and N. A. Devine. 2007. Quetiapine to treat agitation in dementia: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Curr Alzheimer Res 4 (1): 81–93. Zornberg, G. L., and H. Jick. 2000. Antipsychotic drug use and risk of first-time idiopathic venous thromboembolism: A case-control study. Lancet 356: 1219–1223. Zuidema, S. U., J. F. de Jonghe, F. R. Verhey, and R. T. Koopmans. 2010. Environmental correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in nursing home patients with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 25 (1): 14–22.
Chapter 6
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly and in Those at Risk for Dementia Patrick McNamara
Evidence suggests that participation in religious activity among the elderly is associated with slower progression of Alzheimer ’s disease (Kaufman et al. 2007), lower morbidity and mortality (Holt and Dellmann-Jenkins 1992; Levin and Schiller 1987; McCullough et al. 2000), reduced cognitive aging (Corsentino et al. 2009), lower levels of depression and psychological stress (Ironson et al. 2002; Koenig et al. 1992), and enhanced quality of life and well-being (Ellison and Levin 1998; Koenig 1994; Koenig 2000; Koenig, Hays, et al. 1999; Koenig, Idler, et al. 1999; Larson et al. 1992; Levin 1994). Religiosity, however, can also sometimes have a deleterious impact on some measures of mental health in some elderly individuals (Krause 2004; Krause et al. 1998). For better or worse, religious practices become increasingly important to many people as they age (Dillon and Wink 2007), yet very little is known about religious cognition in the aging brain/mind. To our knowledge, no neurocognitive model of religious cognition in aging has yet been developed or rigorously tested—this despite the well-attested impact of religiosity on mind, brain, and overall health and well-being in the elderly. There is, therefore, an urgent need to develop explicit models of religious cognition that can focus research efforts on identification of potential mechanisms that mediate the impact of religious cognitions and practices on cognitive aging and on both positive and negative health outcomes. The model I have proposed and will describe here is known as the “decoupling” model (described below and
120
Dementia
in McNamara 2009). It is focused on the impact of religious cognition on self-control and self-regulation, processes known to be crucial for health and well-being (Baumeister and Vohs 2004). It is, therefore, an ideal model to assess potential mediating cognitive pathways of religion’s effects on self-regulation, cognitive aging, and health outcomes. RELIGIOSITY AND HEALTH Adults over age 65 are at increased risk for mood disorder, mild cognitive impairment, and higher rates of chronic disease (Anstey et al. 2009; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2008; Conwell and Duberstein 2001; Hybels and Blazer 2003; Lebowitz et al. 1997; Zihl et al. 2010). Those elderly who engage in regular religious practices are protected to some significant extent against these adverse outcomes (Krause 2004; Krause et al. 1998; Koenig et al. 1992; Corsentino et al. 2009). Elderly people more often rely on religious coping practices than do younger controls (Koenig et al. 1990; Van Ness and Larson 2002; McFarland 2010) and more often engage in religious cognition (Koenig et al. 1990; Van Ness and Larson 2002; McFarland 2010) for problem solving. Negative forms of religious coping, deleterious to health and well being (e.g., obsessing about punitive supernatural agents, etc.), are also all too common among the elderly. Measures of religiosity such as frequent prayer or meditation, frequent attendance at religious services, and “intrinsic” forms of religiosity appear to have beneficial effects on some aspects of physical and mental health in the elderly (Koenig, Hays, et al. 1999; Koenig, Idler, et al. 1999; Musick et al. 2000; Koenig, McCullough, and Larson 2001; Powell, Shahabi, and Thoresen 2003). Other forms of religiosity can negatively impact mental health (Magyar-Russell and Pargament 2006). There have been forceful criticisms of many religion and health studies (see Lawrence 2002; Sloan and Bagiella 2002; Sloan, Bagiella, and Powell 1999; Sloan et al. 2000; Sloan and Ramakrishnan 2006). And there have also been more positive overall assessments (see Koenig 2000; Koenig 2001, “Religion and Medicine II”; Koenig 2001, “Religion and Medicine III”; Koenig 2001, “Religion and Medicine IV”; Koenig, The healing 2001; Miller and Thoresen 2003; Thoresen and Harris 2002; Williams and Sternthal 2007). Yet, while many early studies failed to account for key confounding variables and other covariates, more recent research efforts are less vulnerable to such criticisms. Recent meta-analytic reviews of well-controlled studies and randomized clinical trials of religious practices and health outcomes (Townsend et al. 2002; Powell, Shahabi, and Thoresen 2003; Coruh et al. 2005) all concluded that religious practices of various kinds such as private meditation and
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
121
prayer and frequent attendance at religious services enhance a variety of health outcomes. Despite the apparent protective effects of religiosity on health outcomes in the aged, there are still no neurocognitive models that can address how religious cognition might influence health in aging. We (McNamara 2001, 2002, 2006, 2009; McNamara, Andresen, and Gellard 2003; McNamara, Durso, and Brown 2006; McNamara, Durso, Brown, and Harris 2006; McNamara, Durso, and Harris 2006; Butler, McNamara, and Durso 2009) have been developing a research program that addresses the need for a neurocognitive model of religiosity that can help to account for religion’s effects on the aging brain/mind. We suggest that a distinguishing mark of religious cognition in aging is that religious cognitions involve privileged access to the self and can help promote development of generative aspects of the self in aging. Religious cognition can therefore become a powerful tool in processes of self-regulation and elderly people avail themselves of this tool. This impact of religiousness on self-regulation can also help to explain negative effects of religiosity when they occur. Since we are interested in understanding religion’s effects on cognition and health, we have built upon previous models of various aspects of the religious mind (reviews in Andresen 2001) as well as more general models of self-regulation (Baumeister and Vohs 2004). Consistent with the work of many other investigators (see reviews in McCullough and Willoughby 2009 and McNamara 2009) in these and related fields, we argue that one major pathway by which religion influences health outcomes (particularly in the elderly) is via its effects on self-regulation. At the cognitive level, we argue that religious cognition (e.g., triggered in prayer, meditation, or ritual contexts, etc.) is characterized by a transient “decoupling” of the present self-concept from control over attentional resources of the individual and then a linking up or integration of the current self with some version of an ideal future self or god-concept. When operating normally, this linking of present with ideal selves and/or with supernatural agents promotes positive self-development and enhances control over attentional resources and behavior of the individual. Strengthening selfcontrol, in turn, promotes improved self-regulation and improved health outcomes in the long run (McNamara 2009; McCullough and Willoughby 2009). The transient decoupling of self and attentional mechanisms, however, can also lead to liminal states of consciousness that open the individual to negative outcomes if integration of old self into an ideal generative self is not accomplished. The generative self builds on Erickson’s idea of generativity versus despair as an emotional and spiritual challenge in old age. The generative self delivers wisdom and the blessings and gifts of experience to others.
122
Dementia
POSSIBLE SELVES AND SELF-REGULATION We situate our theory of religion’s effect on self-regulation within the tradition of work on so-called possible selves. According to Markus and Nurius (1986), possible selves are images of what people hope to become, expect to become, or fear becoming in the future. Possible selves appear to be elaborated out of imaginary narratives involving the self both in childhood and in adulthood (e.g., Erikson 2001; Markus and Ruvolo 1989; Whitty 2002). Possible selves consist of a description of a set of behavioral actions aimed at some goal designed to overcome some conflict, along with causes and consequences of those imaginary actions, with an end state that is described as an event. According to narrative theorists (Bruner 1995; Ricoeur 1984; Oatley 2007), narratives about future selves provide interpretations about what we see as possible. As stories they help to integrate material about conflict involving the present self into a resolution of that conflict—a resolution involving a higher, more complete, and more complex self. Empirical work has supported this narrative-related integrative function of possible selves. We evaluate our current and past selves with reference to possible selves. Possible selves become relevant for self-regulation when they are recruited into the subset of self-knowledge that is active in working memory (Markus and Kunda 1986; Markus and Nurius 1986). Obviously when a possible self is periodically or chronically activated it becomes particularly important for evaluation of current representations of the self as well as discrepancy reduction behaviors or engagement of approach and/or avoidance behaviors (Norman and Aron 2003). For example, frequent attendance at religious services or performance of religious rituals will periodically activate a number of possible selves, including an ideal self. The chronically activated ideal self is then in a position to contribute to self-regulation by providing a standard by which to evaluate progress toward a goal and resolution of internal and social conflicts (Oyserman et al. 2004). People use possible selves as behavioral standards to guide conflict resolution and self-regulation more generally (e.g., Hoyle and Sowards 1993; Hoyle and Sherrill 2006; Kerpelman and Lamke 1997; Oyserman et al. 2004). Hoyle and Sherrill (2006) have pointed out that possible selves map particularly well into hierarchically organized control-process models of self-regulation (e.g., Carver and Scheier 1981; Hoyle and Sowards 1993). Behavioral reference points or standards are organized in these models of self-regulation in a hierarchical fashion from abstract and general to concrete and specific. A particular behavioral standard derives from the level above it. The highest levels of standards
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
123
Figure 6.1
Decoupling model of religious cognition. (The Neuroscience of Religious Experience, by Patrick McNamara. Copyright © 2009 Patrick McNamara. Reprinted with the permission of Cambridge University Press.)
are global ideals. In the context of religious ritual, these global standards are the ideal selves that the current self is urged to become or desires to become. The highest global standard is the god-concept toward which the entire religious service is oriented. In our decoupling model of religious cognition (Figure 6.1), religious practices are thought to create a subtle and brief decentering (of the self) effect that leads ultimately to greater self-control. We postulate four basic cognitive processes that occur in religious cognition (situated within the cognitive architecture proposed by Nichols and Stich [2000] to account for pretend play): (1) A transient and subtle reduction in agency such that the self relaxes control over attentional and behavioral responses. (2) The
124
Dementia
self-concept is then placed into a suppositional or liminal state (a possible worlds box); that liminal state is filled with potentially positive and negative consequences. On the positive side, decoupling of the self from cognitive control mechanisms puts the individual into a receptive and integrative mode, thereby allowing the individual to perform a lot of offline maintenance and integrative information-processing tasks. On the negative side, the decoupling process can, if prolonged and depending on context, lead to dangerous disintegrative psychic states including fanaticism and psychotic and delusionary states, particularly if step 4 fails or is delayed. (3) A search is activated in semantic memory for an ideal self that can link up with and integrate the old self. (4) Integration of old and ideal self is accomplished (see Figure 6.1) with the help of normal inferential machinery and an “updater” that deletes nonuseful material and adds as much of the ideal self (in the form of new beliefs, and so on) as possible to the old self construct. The updater also establishes control of the new Self (via belief–desire systems) over behavioral output systems. IMPLICATIONS FOR COGNITIVE AGING AND DEMENTIA The decoupling model predicts enhanced self-control due to religious cognitions. Several lines of research show that subliminal and supraliminal activation of religious semantic networks can modulate subsequent behaviors, such as inducing prosocial behaviors, positive mood, or improved attentional focus (Cahn and Polich 2006; Pichon, Boccato, and Saroglou 2007; Randolph-Seng and Nielsen 2007; Weisbuch-Remington et al. 2005). Because (a) religiousness often revolves around resisting “temptations” by inhibiting certain behavior in the service of other behaviors, (b) automatic activation of religious networks can subtly but significantly impact attention and behavior, (c) neuroscientific research supports that frontal lobe function is crucial to religiosity, and (d) neuropsychological and imaging studies demonstrate a strong role for the frontal lobes in inhibitory control and attentional focus, we find it reasonable to assume that the automatic activation of religious concepts will enhance neuropsychological performance on well validated “inhibitory tasks” (e.g., Stroop and Go/No-Go). The alleged mechanism is that automatic activation of religious concepts places frontal lobe-mediated inhibitory networks into states of sub-threshold activation to be better prepared for action control thus enhancing function of inhibitory networks. The most straightforward way to test this idea is to assess performance of healthy elderly and those at risk for dementia on self-control tasks before and after exposure to religious versus control primes. Another way to test self-control effects of religious coping is to
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
125
assess at risk individuals on temporal discounting tasks. If religious cognition functions to enhance self-control and inhibitory capacities of the brain/mind, then this ought to be detectable on temporal discounting tasks. Delayed rewards are often viewed as less desirable than immediate rewards—a phenomenon known as temporal discounting. Empirical testing has generated hyperbolic models to describe the change in temporal discounting as a function of time. Take the following function as a wellvalidated example: V = A/(1+kD)s, where V refers to the discounted value of a delayed reward A (e.g., money), with D (delay) in standard temporal units, k as the slope of the delay curve, and s a hyperbolic modulating parameter assigned computationally to tighten goodness-of-fit between theoretical and empirical curve setting (Ainslie 2005; Critchfield and Kollins 2001; Frederick, Loewenstein, and Donoghue 2003; Loewenstein, Read, and Baumeister 2003; Mazur 1987; Rachlin, Raineri, and Cross 1991). To my knowledge, no studies have yet been run to measure effects of religiosity on temporal discounting curves in healthy elderly at risk for dementia. The decoupling model predicts that religious cognition promotes an integration of present into future ideal selves and an avoidance of fearedfor selves. This idea can be tested via the use of the possible selves paradigm as follows: Half of all participants write down three negative possible selves and three positive possible selves, then rate (using a 100mm line) how close (congruence) they are to becoming each of these selves. They next are exposed to religious versus control subliminal primes/words with half of the group given subliminal religious primes and the other half subliminal control primes. They then rerate congruence between current and possible selves. We predict enhanced closeness to positive self and reduced closeness to negative self after unconscious religious primes, even in nonbelieving atheists (as measured at baseline by the Brief Multidimensional Measure of Religiousness/Spirituality [BMMRS]; Fetzer Institute 1999). Congruence ratings are done via a mark on a 100mm line. The other half of all participants are given the religious versus control primes first and then asked to perform the possible selves task. The prediction is the same: those exposed to religious primes will evidence congruence with ideal future selves and distance from feared future selves. THE EXAMPLE OF PARKINSON’S DISEASE It is important for an understanding of potential benefits of religious coping in aging and dementia to identify brain correlates of religious cognition as degenerative brain disorders likely influence neural networks that support all forms of complex cognition including religious cognition.
126
Dementia
Based on our studies of religious cognition in neurologic populations (McNamara, Durso, and Brown 2006; McNamara, Durso, Brown, and Harris 2006; McNamara, Durso, and Harris 2006; McNamara 2009), particularly in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD; Butler, McNamara, and Durso 2009), we believe decoupling (and religious cognition more generally) is mediated by widely distributed neural networks with neostriatal right prefrontal cortical networks playing a key role. To identify potential neurologic mediation of religious cognition, my colleagues and I (Butler, McNamara, and Durso 2009) have tested elements of the decoupling model in patients with PD and found that leftonset patients were impaired relative to healthy controls and right-onset patients in automatic access to religious concepts. Because these patients are also at high risk for a later dementing process it may be that some people with dementia will experience greater difficulty utilizing religious coping strategies. PD is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that disrupts proper functioning of mesocortical dopaminergic networks. Interestingly, PD patients self-report lower interest in religion and score lower in measures of religiosity (McNamara, Durso, and Brown 2006; McNamara, Durso, and Harris 2006; McNamara, Durso, Brown, and Harris 2006). PD is characterized by tremor, gait disturbance, rigidity, and slowed movement. The primary neuropathology results from selective degradation of the dopamine cells in the midbrain leading to severe dopamine depletion in the striatum. Apart from the motor symptoms of PD, the disease affects mood, personality, social cognition, and executive function (McNamara 2009). When we (Butler, McNamara, and Durso 2009) assessed the integrity of semantic fields devoted to religious versus equally complex control concepts, PD patients demonstrated deficits in the automatic activation of religious concepts. There was a differential delay in response times to religious versus control concepts. The delay was not due to a general motor slowness due to PD motor symptoms as it occurred in left-onset but not in rightonset patients who had equal motor deficits. Using PD response times as a baseline for comparison, response times were most delayed for the religious prime/target pairs. Left-onset PD patients demonstrated a selective loss in the ability to activate religious knowledge. There was some evidence that patients most at risk for dementia experienced the longest delays in activation of religious concepts. There was a moderate to strong correlation between a measure of executive function (Stroop) and delayed response times to religious concepts in PD participants. Executive function deficits are known to predict earlier onset of dementia in PD patients who get dementia. In addition to the deficits in unconscious and automatic
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
127
activation of religious concepts, PD patients consciously reported lower levels of intrinsic religiosity.
THE EXAMPLE OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a neurodegenerative disorder that is localized primarily to the frontal lobes and the anterior portions of the temporal lobes. In its early stages, before individuals become demented, FTD is associated with early behavioral abnormalities including occasional changes in expressed religiosity. In the so-called temporal variant of FTD (tvFTD), there is often asymmetric, right- or left-sided degeneration of the orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior temporal lobe, and the amygdala. The religious experiential manifestations of the disease are interesting because they contrast with the loss of interest in religious ideas in leftonset PD patients. In tvFTD patients, there is occasionally the relatively sudden acquisition of interest in religious ideas, texts, practices, and so forth. Here is a case from Edwards-Lee et al. (1997, 1030–1031): Patient RTLV 3 A 66-year-old right-handed female had 2 years of behavioural and cognitive change. Previously social, she withdrew and her religious beliefs heightened. She could not recognize her son’s voice on the telephone and developed word-finding trouble. Driving competency remained normal and she continued to manage family finances and housework. On examination she was irritable, remote and easily upset. MMSE score was 23. She had mild anomia and a mild decrease in language comprehension. Memory was impaired but drawings and calculations were normal. She did not cooperate for PIQ but VIQ (78) was in the borderline range. Basic attention (Digit Span = 37%) was normal, but learning and memory ranged from low average to borderline (shopping list Trial 5 = 7; delayed recall= 5; Rey–Osterrieth delay = 14, 25th%). In contrast, information processing speed (Trails A <1st%; Stroop A and B <1st%), word-retrieval (Boston Naming Test = 10, <1st%), and executive skills were severely impaired (FAS <1st%; Stroop C = 200 seconds, 2nd %). MRI was normal, but SPECT revealed bitemporal hypoperfusion, worse on the right than the left. Over the next 2 years she became increasingly irritable and remote. Although too uncooperative for testing, she continued to run her household.
128
Dementia
The heightened religiosity was apparently associated with right temporal pathology or hypoperfusion. The hyperreligiosity was therefore likely mediated either by disinhibited right-sided orbitofrontal and limbic networks or by left-sided temporal and frontal networks.
RECOMMENDATIONS People who undergo a dementing process—no matter how pervasive— nevertheless retain some personal and cognitive strengths. Among the resources these individuals might tap are religious-based coping strategies. Not everyone is religious so physicians and caregivers should not push “religion” on any patient. But it is entirely reasonable to refrain from criticizing religious coping practices as “delusional” if a patient and the patient’s family derive strength therein. If, on the other hand, religious ideation becomes clearly delusional and it does not respond to pharmacotherapy, then the physician and caretaker must monitor the patient carefully. This monitoring must take some inventory of any potentially dangerous religious delusions such as the “command hallucinations” sometimes encountered in the schizophrenias and in bipolar spectrum disorders. We obviously need more research on religious coping strategies in the dementias and in their caregivers. We do not even have the basic information required for understanding the role of religious coping strategies among those with dementia. How many people with a dementing illness use religious coping strategies? To what extent do they help? Is the access and use of religious coping strategies any more difficult for those with dementing illnesses than those without? Do elderly at risk for dementia use religious coping strategies to build a generative Self? How should we study religiosity in the aging brain? Religiosity can be assessed in several ways including religious beliefs, ritual practices, public and private spiritual practices, religious coping strategies, and religious history. In cognitively intact or early stage dementia, standard self-report instruments can probably be used successfully. The BMMRS contains 38 statements with Likert scale formats that cover 11 religious domains. These are daily spiritual experiences, values/beliefs, forgiveness, private religious practices, religious and spiritual coping, religious support, religious/spiritual history, commitment, organizational religiousness, religious preference and overall self ranking (e.g., “To what extent do you consider yourself a religious person?”). The BMMRS was developed by a panel of experts on religion and health convened by the Fetzer Institute and the National
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
129
Institutes of Health and of Aging. It has excellent psychometric properties as well as publicly available norms for healthy older individuals (Fetzer Institute 1999). Developing a science of religiosity in the aging brain, possibly with the help of the decoupling model described above, will lay the groundwork for more fruitful investigations of the use of religious coping strategies in people with dementia. REFERENCES Ainslie, G. 2005. Précis of breakdown of will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (5): 635–650. Andresen, J., ed. 2001. Religion in mind: Cognitive perspectives on religious belief, ritual and experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Anstey, K. J., R. Burns, P. Butterworth, T. D. Windsor, H. Christensen, and P. Sachdev. 2009. Cardiovascular risk factors and life events as antecedents of depressive symptoms in middle and early-old age: Path through life study. Psychosomatic Medicine 71 (9): 937–943. Baumeister, R. F., and K. D. Vohs. 2004. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Press. Bruner, J. 1995. Meaning and self in cultural perspective. In The social self, ed. D. Bakhurst and C. Sypnowich, 18–29. London: Sage. Butler, P. M., P. McNamara, and R. Durso. 2009. Deficits in the automatic activation of religious concepts in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (Dec. 4): 1–10. Cahn, B. R., and J. Polich. 2006. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin 132 (2): 180–211. Carver, C. S., and M. F. Scheier. 1981. Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2008. National diabetes fact sheet, 2007. Atlanta: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. http:// www.cdc.gov/Diabetes/pubs/factsheet07.htm Conwell, Y., and P. R. Duberstein. 2001. Suicide in elders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 932: 132–147. Corsentino, E. A., N. Collins, N. Sachs-Ericsson, and D. G. Blazer. 2009. Religious attendance reduces cognitive decline among older women with high levels of depressive symptoms. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences 64A (12): 1283–1289. Coruh, B., H. Ayele, M. Pugh, and T. Mulligan. 2005. Does religious activity improve health outcomes? A critical review of the recent literature. EXPLORE: The Journal of Science and Healing 1 (3): 186–191. Critchfield, T. S., and S. H. Kollins. 2001. Temporal discounting: Basic research and the analysis of socially important behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 34: 101–122.
130
Dementia
Dillon, M., and P. Wink. 2007. In the course of a lifetime: Tracing religious belief, practice and change. Berkeley: University of California Press. Edwards-Lee, T., B. L. Miller, D. F. Benson, J. L. Cummings, G. L. Russell, K. Boone, and I. Mena. 1997. The temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 120: 1027–1040. Ellison, C. G., and J. S. Levin. 1998. The religion-health connection: Evidence, theory and future directions. Health Education and Behavior 25: 700–720. Erikson, M. G. 2001. Possible selves revisited: Self-narratives about the future. Paper presented at the centenary annual conference of the British Psychological Society, Glasgow, Scotland, March. Fetzer Institute. 1999. Multidimensional measurement of religiousness/ spirituality for use in health research: A report from the Fetzer Institute/ National Institute on aging working group. Bethesda, MD: Fetzer Institute. Frederick, S., G. Loewenstein, and T. Donoghue. 2003. Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. In Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice, ed. G. Loewenstein, D. Read, and R. Baumeister, 13–86. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Holt, M., and M. Dellmann-Jenkins. 1992. Research and implications for practice: Religion, well-being/morale, and coping behavior in later life. Journal of Applied Gerontology 11: 101–110. Hoyle, R. H., and M. R. Sherrill. 2006. Future orientation in the self-system: Possible selves, self-regulation, and behavior. Journal of Personality 74: 1673–1696. Hoyle, R. H., and B. A. Sowards. 1993. Self-monitoring and the regulation of social experience: A control-process model. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 12: 280–306. Hybels, C. F., and D. G. Blazer. 2003. Epidemiology of late-life mental disorders. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine 19: 663–696. Ironson, G., G. F. Solomon, E. G. Balbin, C. O’Cleirigh, A. George, M. Kumar, M., D. Larson, and T. E. Woods. 2002. The Ironson-Woods Spirituality/ Religiousness Index is associated with long survival, health behaviors, less distress, and low cortisol in people with HIV/AIDS. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 24: 34–48. Kaufman, Y., D. Anaki, M. Binns, and M. Freedman. 2007. Cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 68: 1509–1514. Kerpelman, J. L., and L. K. Lamke. 1997. Anticipation of future identities: A control theory approach to identity development within the context of serious dating relationships. Personal Relationships 4: 47–62. Koenig, H. G. 1994. Religion and older men in prison. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10: 219–230. Koenig H. G. 2000. Religion and medicine I. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 30 (4): 385–398. Koenig H. G. 2001. Religion and medicine II. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 31 (1): 97–109.
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
131
Koenig H. G. 2001. Religion and medicine III. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 31 (2): 119–216. Koenig H. G. 2001. Religion and medicine IV. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 31 (3): 321–336. Koenig, H. G. 2001. The healing power of faith: How belief and prayer can help you triumph over disease. New York: Simon and Schuster. Koenig, H. G., H. J. Cohen, D. G. Blazer, C. Pieper, K. G. Meador, F. Shelp, V. Goli, and B. DiPasquale. 1992. Religious coping and depression in elderly hospitalized medical ill men. American Journal of Psychiatry 149: 1693–1700. Koenig, H. G., J. C. Hays, D. B. Larson, L. K. George, H. J. Cohen, M. E. McCullough, K. E. Meador, and D. G. Blazer. 1999. Does religious attendance prolong survival? A six-year follow-up study of 3,968 older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences 54A (7): M370–M376. Koenig H. G., E. Idler, S. Kasl, J. C. Hays, L. K. George, M. Musick, D. B. Larson, T. R. Collins, and H. Benson. 1999. Editorial: Religion, spirituality, and medicine: A rebuttal to skeptics. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 29 (2): 123–131. Koenig, H. K., M. E. McCullough, and D. B. Larson. 2001. Handbook of religion and health. New York: Oxford University Press. Koenig, H. G., I. C. Siegler, K. G. Meador, and L. K. George. 1990. Religious coping and personality in later life. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 5: 123–131. Krause, N. 2004. Religious doubt and health: Exploring the potential dark side of religion. Sociology of Religion 65: 35–56. Krause, N., J. Liang, A. Jain, and H. Sugisawa. 1998. Gender differences in health among the Japanese elderly. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 26: 141–159. Larson, D. B., K. A. Sherrill, J. S. Lyons, F. C. Craige, S. B. Thielman, M. A. Greenwold, and S. S. Larson. 1992. Dimensions and valences of measures of religious commitment found in the American Journal of Psychiatry and the Archives of General Psychiatry, 1978–1989. American Journal of Psychiatry 149: 557–559. Lawrence, R. J. 2002. The witches’ brew of spirituality and medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 24 (1): 74–76. Lebowitz, B. D., J. L. Pearson, L. S. Schneider, C. F. Reynolds III, G. S. Alexopoulos, M. L. Bruce, Y. Conwell, I. R. Katz, B. S. Meyers, M. F. Morrison, J. Mossey, G. Niederene, and P. Parmelee. 1997. Diagnosis and treatment of depression in late life. Consensus statement update. JAMA 278: 1186–1190. Levin, J. 1994. Religion and health: Is there an association, is it valid, and is it causal? Social Science and Medicine 38: 1475–1484. Levin, J., and P. Schiller. 1987. Is there a religious factor in health? Journal of Religion and Health 26: 9–36. Loewenstein, G., D. Read, and R. Baumeister, R., eds. 2003. Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
132
Dementia
Magyar-Russell, G., and K. Pargament. 2006. The darker side of religion: Risk factors for poorer health and well-being. In Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion, vol. 3: The psychology of religious experience, ed. P. McNamara, 91–117. Westport, CT: Praeger. Markus, H., and Z. Kunda. 1986. Stability and malleability of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 858–866. Markus, H., and P. Nurius. 1986. Possible selves. American Psychologist 41: 954–969. Markus, H., and A. Ruvolo. 1989. Possible selves: Personalized representations of goals. In Goal concepts in personality and social psychology, ed. L. A. Pervin, 211–241. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mazur, J. E. 1987. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In Quantitative analysis of behavior: The effects of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value, ed. M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, and H. Rachlin, 5: 55–73. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McCullough, M. E., W. T. Hoyt, D. B. Larson, H. G. Koenig, and C. Thoresen. 2000. Religious involvement and mortality: A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology 19: 211–222. McCullough, M. E., and B. L. B. Willoughby. 2009. Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135: 69–93. McFarland, M. J. 2010. Religion and mental health among older adults: Do the effects of religious involvement vary by gender? Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences 65B (5): 621–630. McNamara, P. 2001. Religion and the frontal lobes. In Religion in mind, ed. J. Andresen, 237–256. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McNamara, P. 2002. The motivational origins of religious practices. Zygon 37 (1): 143–160. McNamara, P., ed. 2006. The frontal lobes, and the evolution of cooperation and religion. In Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion, vol. 2: The neurology of religious experience, 189–204. Westport, CT: Praeger. McNamara, P. 2009. The neuroscience of religious experience. New York: Cambridge University Press. McNamara, P., J. Andresen, and J. Gellard. 2003. Religion of religiosity and scores on fluency tests to subjective reports of health in older individuals. International Journal of the Psychology of Religion 13: 259–271. McNamara, P., R. Durso, and A. Brown. 2006. Religiosity in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2 (3): 341–348. McNamara, P., R. Durso, A. Brown, and E. Harris. 2006. The chemistry of religiosity: Evidence from patients with Parkinson’s disease. In Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion,
Religious Coping Strategies in Healthy Elderly
133
vol. 2: The neurology of religious experience, ed. P. McNamara, 1–14. Westport, CT: Praeger. McNamara, P., R. Durso, and E. Harris. 2006. Frontal lobe mediation of the sense of self: Evidence from studies of patients with Parkinson’s disease. In The concept of self in medicine and health care, ed. A. P. Prescott, 143–161. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. Miller, W. R., and C. E. Thoresen. 2003. Spirituality, religion, and health: An emerging research field. American Psychologist 58 (1): 24–35. Musick, M. A., J. W. Traphagan, H. G. Koenig, and D. B. Larson. 2000. Spirituality in physical health and aging. Journal of Adult Development 7 (2): 73–86. Nichols, S., and S. Stich. 2000. A cognitive theory of pretense. Cognition 74 (2): 115–147. Norman, C. C., and A. Aron. 2003. Aspects of possible self that predict motivation to achieve or avoid it. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39: 500–507. Oatley, K. 2007. Narrative modes of consciousness and selfhood. In The Cambridge handbook of consciousness, ed. P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson, 375–402. New York: Cambridge University Press. Oyserman, D., D. Bybee, K. Terry, and T. Hart-Johnson. 2004. Possible selves as roadmaps. Journal of Research in Personality 38: 130–149. Pichon, I., G. Boccato, and V. Saroglou. 2007. Nonconscious influences of religion on prosociality: A priming study. European Journal of Social Psychology 37: 1032–1045. Powell, L. H., L. Shahabi, and C. E. Thoresen. 2003. Religion and spirituality: Linkages to physical health. American Psychologist 58 (1): 36–52. Rachlin, H., A. Raineri, and D. Cross. 1991. Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 55: 233–244. Randolph-Seng, B., and M. E. Nielsen. 2007. Honesty: One effect of primed religious representations. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 17: 303–315. Ricoeur, P. 1984. Time and narrative. Vol. 1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Sloan, R. P., and E. Bagiella. 2002. Claims about religious involvement and health outcomes. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 24 (1): 14–21. Sloan, R. P., E. Bagiella, and T. Powell. 1999. Religion, spirituality, and medicine. Lancet 353 (9153): 664–667. Sloan, R. P., E. Bagiella, L. VandeCreek, M. Hover, C. Cassalone, T. J. Hirsch, Y. Hasan, R. Kreger, and P. Poulos. 2000. Should physicians prescribe religious activities? New England Journal of Medicine 342: 1913–1916. Sloan, R. P., and R. Ramakrishnan. 2006. Science, medicine, and intercessory prayer. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 49 (4): 504–514. Thoresen, C. E., and A. H. S. Harris. 2002. Spirituality and health: What’s the evidence and what’s needed? Annals of Behavioral Medicine 24 (1): 3–13. Townsend, M., V. Kladder, H. Ayele, and T. Mulligan. 2002. Systematic review of clinical trials examining the effects of religion on health. Southern Medical Journal 95 (12): 1429–1434.
134
Dementia
Van Ness, P. H., and D. B. Larson. 2002. Religion, senescence, and mental health. The end of life is not the end of hope. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 10: 386–397. Weisbuch-Remington, M., W. B. Mendes, M. D. Seery, and J. Blascovich. 2005. The nonconscious influence of religious symbols in motivated performance situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31 (9): 1203–1216. Whitty, M. 2002. Possible selves: An exploration of the utility of a narrative approach. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research 2: 211–228. Williams, D. R., and M. J. Sternthal. 2007. Spirituality, religion and health: Evidence and research directions. Medical Journal of Australia 186 (10 Suppl.): S47–S50. Zihl, J., S. Reppermund, S. Thum, and K. Unger. 2010. Neuropsychological profiles in MCI and in depression: Differential cognitive dysfunction patterns or similar final common pathway disorder? Journal of Psychiatric Research 44 (10): 647–654.
Chapter 7
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis: Compensatory and Regenerative Processes Philippe Taupin
Neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain of mammals and is modulated by a broad range of stimuli and conditions. Drugs used in the treatment of Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) and depression stimulate neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. Neurogenesis is enhanced in the brain of patients with neurological diseases and disorders and in the brain of animal models of neurological diseases and disorders. Despite controversies surrounding such studies and the need to confirm these data, adult neurogenesis and neural stem cells (NSCs) may be the target of drugs used in the treatment of neurological diseases and disorders, particularly AD and depression, and adult neurogenesis and the hippocampus may contribute to the pathology of neurological diseases and disorders. Enhanced neurogenesis in neurological diseases and disorders may represent regenerative attempts by the nervous system. Drug treatments may contribute to compensatory mechanisms in the adult hippocampus. It points to a broader involvement of the adult NSCs and the hippocampus in neurological diseases and drug therapy. Hence, adult NSCs represent not only a promising model for cellular therapy, but may also contribute to the physiopathology of the nervous system and its pharmacology, particularly for AD and depression, the understanding of which will lead to a better understanding of the nervous system, and the development of novel and more effective treatments and cures for neurological diseases and disorders.
136
Dementia
INTRODUCTION In the mammalian brain, neurogenesis occurs throughout adulthood primarily in two regions, the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus and the anterior part of the subventricular zone (SVZ) in various species, including humans (Eriksson et al. 1998; Curtis et al. 2007; Taupin 2008, “Adult neural”). In the DG, newly generated neuronal cells in the subgranular zone (SGZ) migrate to the granule cell layer, where they differentiate into granule-like cells and extend axonal projections the to CA3 region (Cameron et al. 1993; Toni et al. 2007; Taupin 2009). In the SVZ, newly generated neuronal cells migrate to the olfactory bulb, through the rostro-migratory stream, where they differentiate into interneurons (Lois and Alvarez-Buylla 1994; Belluzzi et al. 2003; Curtis et al. 2007). About 0.1% of the granule cell population or 9,000 new neuronal cells are generated per day in the DG of young adult rodents and about 0.004% of the granule cell population is generated per day in the DG of adult macaque monkeys (Kornack and Rakic 1999; Cameron and McKay 2001). Though newly generated neuronal cells in the adult brain undergo programmed cell death rather than achieving maturity, the ones that survive survive for an extended period of time, at least two years in humans (Eriksson et al. 1998; Cameron and McKay 2001). It is postulated that newly generated neuronal cells in the adult brain originate from NSCs. NSCs are the self-renewing multipotent cells that generate the main phenotypes of the nervous system. Because of their potential to generate the main phenotypes of the nervous system, NSCs represent a promising model for cellular therapy for the treatment of a broad range of neurological diseases and injuries, particularly neurodegenerative diseases, cerebral strokes and spinal cord injuries. The stimulation of endogenous neural progenitor or stem cells and the transplantation of adult-derived neural progenitor and stem cells are proposed to restore and repair the degenerated or injured nerve pathways. Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus and SVZ is modulated by a broad range of stimuli and conditions, including environmental enrichment, physiological processes, pathological conditions, trophic factors/ cytokines, and drugs (Taupin 2007). Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus is modulated by drugs used in the treatments of AD and depression, and is modulated in the brain of patients with neurological diseases and disorders and in the brain of animal models of neurological diseases and disorders, like AD, epilepsy, and Huntington’s disease (HD) (Parent et al. 1997; Malberg et al. 2000; Curtis et al. 2003; Jin, Galvan et al. 2004; Jin, Peel et al. 2004; Jin et al. 2006). Do adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain contribute to the pharmacology of drugs
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis
137
used in the treatments of neurological diseases and disorders? Do adult neurogenesis and the hippocampus contribute to the pathology of neurological diseases and disorders? In the following sections, we will review and discuss the potential involvement of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain in the pharmacology of AD and depression, and in the pathology of neurological diseases and disorders. PHARMACOLOGY OF ADULT NEUROGENESIS IN ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE Alzheimer ’s Disease and Drug Therapy Alzheimer ’s disease is a fatal neurodegenerative disease for which there is no cure. It is the most common dementia among elderly, affecting more than 26 million patients worldwide (Ferri et al. 2006). It starts with mild memory problems and ends with severe brain damages. AD is associated with loss of nerve cells, particularly in areas of the brain that are vital to memory and other mental abilities, like the hippocampus. The disease is characterized in the brain by amyloid or senile plaque deposits and neurofibrillary tangles (Caselli et al. 2006). There are two forms of the disease: the late-onset form (LOAD), diagnosed after age 65, and the early-onset form (EOAD), diagnosed at younger age. Most of the cases of LOAD are sporadic forms of the disease, whereas most cases of EOAD are inherited or familial forms of AD (FAD). Risks factors for LOAD include genetic, acquired, and environmental risk factors, like the presence of certain alleles in the genetic makeup of the individual (e.g., the apolipoprotein E varepsilon 4 allele), hypertension, diabetes, and oxidative stress (Raber, Huang, and Ashford 2004). Genetic mutations in the beta-amyloid precursor protein gene (APP), the presenilin-1 gene (PSEN1) and the presenilin-2 gene (PSEN2) have been identified as causative factors for FAD (Schellenberg 1995; St. George-Hyslop and Petit 2005). LOAD represents most cases of AD, with over 93% of all cases of the disease. Actual treatments for AD consist in drug and occupational therapies (Scarpini, Scheltens, and Feldman 2003). Three types of drugs are currently used in the treatment of AD: blockers of the formation of amyloid plaques like alzhemed (Aisen 2005); inhibitors of acetylcholine esterase like galantamine, rivastigmine and tacrine (Wilkinson et al. 2004); and N-methyl-Daspartate glutamate receptor antagonists like memantine (Creeley et al. 2006). Inhibitors of acetylcholine esterase improve cognitive functions by enhancing cholinergic neurotransmission that are important for learning and memory and that are affected in brain regions of patients with AD.
138
Dementia
N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamate receptor antagonists confer protection against excitotoxic neurodegeneration. These drugs produce improvements in cognitive and behavioral symptoms of AD. Other treatments that are considered and are being developed involve drugs for lowering cholesterol levels, anti-inflammatory drugs and protein beta-amyloid vaccination (Estrada and Soto 2007; Solomon 2007). Adult Neurogenesis and Alzheimer ’s Disease Drugs The drugs used in the treatment of AD have been assessed for their effects on adult neurogenesis in rodents. Galantamine and memantine increase neurogenesis in the DG and SVZ of adult rodents by 26–45%, as revealed by bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU) labeling (Jin et al. 2006) (see Table 7.1). This suggests that adult neurogenesis may contribute to the activities of these drugs in the treatment of AD. PHARMACOLOGY OF ADULT NEUROGENESIS IN DEPRESSION Depression and Antidepressants Depression is a major public health issue. It affects an estimated 19 million Americans. Twenty-five percent of adults will have a major depressive episode sometime in their life (Kessler et al. 1994). It is proposed that an imbalance in the 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin or 5-HT) and noradrenaline pathways underlies the pathogenesis of depressive disorders (Hindmarch 2001; Owens 2004). Stress and neuroinflammation are causal factors precipitating episodes of depression in humans (Minghetti 2005; Miura et al. 2008). Actual treatments for depression consist in drug therapy, and psychological support and therapy. Five types of drugs are used in the treatment of depression: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors like fluoxetine, monoamine oxidase inhibitors like tranylcypromine, selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors like reboxetine, tricyclic antidepressants like imipramine and desipramine, and phosphodiesterase-IV inhibitors, like rolipram (Wong and Licinio 2001; Brunello et al. 2002). The efficiency and therapeutic benefits of some of the antidepressants currently prescribed for the treatment of depression have been questioned in recent publications (Kirsch et al. 2008), mandating the development of new drugs for treating depression. Adult Neurogenesis and Antidepressants The activity of antidepressants has been assessed for their effects on adult neurogenesis in rodents and nonhuman primates (see Table 7.1). The chronic administration of fluoxetine and of agomelatine, the melatonergic
Table 7.1 Adult Neurogenesis in Drug Activities and Neurological Diseases and Disorders Alzheimer ’s disease drugs
Galantamine and memantine increase neurogenesis in the DG of adult rodents by 26–45%
Antidepressants
Chronic administration of fluoxetine increases neurogenesis in the DG of adult rodents and nonhuman primatesa
Alzheimer ’s disease
Neurogenesis is enhanced in the hippocampus of AD patientsb Neurogenesis is decreased in the DG of adult mice deficient for APP and/or PSEN1 Neurogenesis is decreased in the DG of adult PDAPP transgenic mice Neurogenesis is increased in the DG of adult transgenic mice that express the Swedish and Indiana APP mutations
Depression
Post-mortem studies do not reveal any increase in neurogenesis in the hippocampus of patients with depressionc
Epilepsy
Neurogenesis is increased in the DG of animal models of epilepsy, like after pilocarpine treatment
Huntington’s disease
Neurogenesis is enhanced in the SVZ of HD patients Neurogenesis is decreased in the DG of adult R6/1 transgenic mice Neurogenesis is increased in the SVZ of adult rodents after quinolinic acid striatal lesioning
Notes: Study and quantification of adult neurogenesis was performed primarily by immunohistology for markers of the cell cycle and for the thymidine analog BrdU. a In other strains of mice, fluoxetine and other antidepressants were reported to produce their activities independently of adult neurogenesis. b Autopsies in this study were performed most likely on patients with the sporadic form of AD. c Autopsies in this study were performed on patients that were on antidepressant medication. Adult neurogenesis may mediate the activities of drugs used in the treatment of AD and depression. The modulation of neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus would represent a phenomenon of plasticity or compensatory mechanisms of recovery. Neurogenesis is enhanced in the brain of patients with and of animal models of neurological diseases and disorders, particularly AD, epilepsy and HD. Enhanced neurogenesis in the DG of the brain with neurological diseases and disorders, particularly neurodegenerative diseases, may contribute to regenerative attempts, to compensate for the neuronal losses. Due to the limitations and pitfalls over the methodologies and paradigms used to study and quantify neurogenesis, these data remain to be validated and confirmed.
140
Dementia
agonist and serotoninergic antagonist, increases neurogenesis in the DG, but not SVZ of adult rats and nonhuman primates (Malberg et al. 2000; Banasr et al. 2006; Perera et al. 2007). The X-irradiation of the hippocampal region inhibits neurogenesis in the DG and prevents the behavioral effect of fluoxetine in adult mice in vivo (Santarelli et al. 2003). In this report, the activity of fluoxetine was reported to be mediated by 5-HT1A receptor in 129SvEvTac mice. In other strains of mice, BALB/cJ mice, fluoxetine activity was reported not to be mediated by 5-HT1A receptor and produces its antidepressant activity independently of neurogenesis (Holick et al. 2008). Autopsy studies report that neurogenesis is not altered in the hippocampus of adult patients who were on antidepressant medication (Reif et al. 2006). The anxiolytic/antidepressant N-[3-(1-{[4-(3,4-difluorophenoxy)phenyl]methyl}(4-piperidyl))-4-methylphenyl]-2-methylpropanamide (SNAP 94847) stimulates the proliferation of progenitor cells in the DG, but its activity is unaltered in mice in which neurogenesis was suppressed by X-irradiation (David et al. 2007). This shows that adult neurogenesis may mediate the activities of antidepressants, particularly selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors like fluoxetine, but that fluoxetine and other antidepressants may also produce their activity via distinct mechanisms, some independently of adult neurogenesis. The mechanisms underlying the activity of antidepressants on adult neurogenesis remain to be determined. It may be mediated by glucocorticoids, stress-related hormones, interleukin-6, a cytokine involved in neuroinflammation, and brain-derived neurotrophic factor, a trophic factor that has antidepressant effects (Siuciak et al. 1997; Cameron, Tanapat, and Gould 1998; Vallieres et al. 2002; Scharfman et al. 2005). DISCUSSION The data reviewed show that adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain contribute to the pharmacology of drugs used in the treatments of AD and depression. There are, however, controversies and debates over the involvement of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells in the activity of drugs used in the treatments of these diseases, particularly in the treatment of depression, and over the involvement of adult neurogenesis in neurological diseases and disorders. Drug Therapy: Compensatory Processes Neurogenesis occurs and is modulated in specialized microenvironments or “niches” (Taupin 2006, “Adult neural stem cells”; Mitsiadis et al.
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis
141
2007). Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus is modulated by drugs used in the treatment of AD and depression. This suggests that adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult hippocampus contribute to and may play an important role in the activities of these drugs. This is particularly striking for antidepressants, as the hippocampus is not the brain region primarily involved in depressive episodes (Campbell and Macqueen 2004). Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus plays a critical role in the activity of antidepressants like fluoxetine. Stress, a causal factor precipitating episodes of depression, decreases neurogenesis in the hippocampus in rodents and nonhuman primates (Gould et al. 1998; Pham et al. 2003), an effect that is reversed after administration of fluoxetine (Malberg and Duman 2003). This led to the theory that the waning and waxing of hippocampal neurogenesis in the adult brain are important causal factors in the precipitation and recovery from episodes of clinical depression (Jacobs, Praag, and Gage 2000). Hence, adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells of the adult hippocampus are targets of drugs used for the treatment of neurological diseases and disorders. The modulation of neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus would represent a phenomenon of plasticity or compensatory mechanisms of recovery involving this area of the brain (Taupin 2006, “Adult neurogenesis”), the role and mechanisms of which remain to be determined. Drugs may act directly or indirectly on newly generated neuronal cells of the adult brain, and they may act via their pharmacological activities on messenger signaling pathways and/or via a neurogenic activity by modulating neurogenesis (Taupin 2008, “Adult neurogenesis”). Adult Neurogenesis in Neurological Diseases and Disorders: Regenerative Processes Autopsy studies reveal that the expression of markers of immature neuronal cells, like doublecortin and polysialylated nerve cell adhesion molecule, is increased in the DG in the brain of patients with AD, with the sporadic form of the disease (Jin, Peel, et al. 2004). Experimental studies in animal models show that neurogenesis is decreased in the DG of adult mice deficient for APP and/or PSEN1, decreased in the DG of adult PDAPP transgenic mice, and increased in the DG of adult transgenic mice that express the Swedish and Indiana APP mutations (Wen et al. 2002; Jin, Galvan, et al. 2004; Donovan et al. 2006; Verret et al. 2007; Zhang et al. 2007; Rodríguez et al. 2008). These studies highlight discrepancies in the effect of AD on adult neurogenesis between patients and animal models of AD.
142
Dementia
They may originate from the validity of the animal models used in those studies, as representative of the diseases, and to study adult phenotypes (German and Eisch 2004). They may also originate from the validity of the protocols used, like immunohistochemistry for markers of the cell cycle and for the thymidine analog BrdU, as paradigms to study adult neurogenesis (Nowakowski and Hayes 2000; Taupin 2007). Despite data suggesting that neurogenesis is enhanced in the hippocampus of AD patients, these studies need to be further confirmed and validated. In patients with depressive disorders, post-mortem studies do not reveal any increase in neurogenesis in the hippocampus (Reif et al. 2006). These data show that neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus may not be modulated by episodes of depression. However, the autopsies in these studies were performed on patients that were on antidepressant medication, known to affect neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus (Malberg et al. 2000). In addition, there are conflicting data on the effects of depression on the hippocampus. On the one hand, the hippocampus of patients with depression show signs of atrophy and neuronal loss (Sheline et al. 1996; Colla et al. 2007). On the other hand, hippocampal volume remains unchanged in depressive patients (Inagaki et al. 2004; Bielau et al. 2005). Therefore, it is unclear how neurogenesis in altered and what is the involvement of the hippocampus in patients with depression. Epilepsy is a brain disorder in which populations of neurons signal abnormally. Neurogenesis is enhanced in the DG of animal models of epilepsy, like after pilocarpine treatment (Parent et al. 1997). Low-dose, whole-brain, X-ray irradiation in adult rats after pilocarpine treatment does not prevent the induction of recurrent seizures or prevent seizureinduced ectopic granule-like cells and MF sprouting (Parent et al. 1999). These data provide a strong argument against a critical role of adult neurogenesis in epileptogenesis. HD is a familial disease, inherited through a mutation; a polyglutamine repeat/expansion that lengthens a glutamine segment in the huntingtin protein (Li and Li 2004). This causes degeneration of neuronal cells in certain areas of the brain, particularly the caudate nucleus and results in uncontrolled movements, loss of intellectual faculties and emotional disturbance in the patients (Sawa, Tomoda, and Bae 2003). Autopsy studies for markers of the cell cycle and of neuronal differentiation, like proliferating cell nuclear antigen and beta-tubulin, show that cell proliferation and neurogenesis are increased in the SVZ of brains of patients with HD (Curtis et al. 2003). Neurogenesis is decreased in the DG in adult R6/1 transgenic mouse model of HD (Lazic et al. 2004). It is increased in the SVZ after quinolinic acid striatal lesioning of the adult brain of rodents,
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis
143
leading to the migration of neuroblasts and the formation of new neuronal cells in damaged areas of the striatum, as observed in the brains of HD patients (Tattersfield et al. 2004). These data suggest that adult neurogenesis is enhanced in the hippocampus of patients with HD. In all, neurogenesis is enhanced in the brain of patients with and of animal models of neurological diseases and disorders, particularly AD, epilepsy and HD (see Table 7.1). Enhanced neurogenesis in the DG of the brain with neurological diseases and disorders, particularly neurodegenerative diseases, may contribute to regenerative attempts, to compensate for the neuronal losses. However, due to the limitations and pitfalls over the methodologies and paradigms used to study and quantify neurogenesis, these data remain to be further validated and confirmed (Taupin 2007). After excitotoxic and mechanical lesions in the dentate granule cell layer of adult rats, neurogenesis is enhanced in the DG (Gould and Tanapat 1997). The increased neurogenesis in many of these illnesses would result from damage or stimulation induction of neurogenesis. This indicates that enhanced neurogenesis may be a result, rather than a cause, of the illnesses (Taupin 2008, “Adult neurogenesis pharmacology”). Adult Neural Stem Cells and Regenerative Medicine NSCs hold the promise to cure a broad range of neurological diseases and injuries. The confirmation that neurogenesis occurs in the adult brain and NSCs reside in the adult CNS of mammals including humans, reveals that the adult brain has the potential for self-repair. Two strategies are envisioned to bring adult NSCs to therapy; the stimulation or transplantation of neural progenitor and stem cells of the adult CNS. The isolation and characterization of adult-derived neural progenitor and stem cells from various regions of the adult CNS show that they may reside throughout the adult CNS. Studies have reported that new neuronal cells are generated at sites of degeneration in the diseased brain and after CNS injuries, like in HD and in experimental models of cerebral strokes. These cells originate from the SVZ; they migrate partially through the rostro-migratory stream to the sites of degeneration (Arvidsson et al. 2002; Curtis et al. 2003). Hence, therapeutic strategies aiming at stimulating endogenous neural progenitor or stem cells to treat neurological diseases or injures could involve either the stimulation, by trophic factor or cytokines, locally of neural progenitor or stem cells or the simulation of neural progenitor or stem cells of the SVZ. Alternatively, adult human-derived neural progenitor and stem cells provide a source of tissue for therapeutic strategies of transplantation,
144
Dementia
to restore and repair degenerated or injured nerve pathways (Roy et al. 2000; Palmer et al. 2001). There are, however, limitations and constraints over the use of adult NSCs for cellular therapy, and particularly for the treatment of AD. First, stem cells, including NSCs, reside in specialized microenvironments or “niches” (Taupin 2006, “Adult neural stem cells”; Mitsiadis et al. 2007). These “niches” control the developmental potential of the stem cells that reside within. This has tremendous consequences, not only for regeneration of tissues by stimulating endogenous stem or progenitor cells, but also when transplanting such cells. The microenvironment may not be permissive, limiting the chance of success of such strategies. Second, current protocols for isolating and culturing NSCs in vitro lead to heterogeneous population of neural progenitor and stem cells, limiting their potential for transplantation therapies. Third, in the case of AD, neurodegeneration is widespread through the brain, targeting areas like the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and neocortex. As consequence, any strategies involving cellular therapy will need to restore and repair multiple neuronal pathways. This would involve the stimulation of endogenous neural progenitor or stem cells, or transplantation of adult-derived neural progenitor and stem cells, at multiple sites to maximize the recovery of deficits and impairments. This makes such strategies rather challenging. To limit the secondary effects associated with intracerebral transplantations, it is proposed to administer the transplanted cells intravenously. Following intravenous administration of adult-derived neural progenitor and stem cells, these cells migrate to sites of degeneration, diseases and injuries, providing a promising method to treat a broad range of neurological diseases and injuries (Brown et al. 2003; Pluchino et al. 2003). Intravenous administration of adult-derived neural progenitor and stem cells would represent a more practical route of administration for NSC-based transplantation strategy for treating AD. In all, adult NSCs provide a promising strategy for the treatment of a broad range of neurological diseases and disorders, but there are limitations and constraints to overcome before such strategy is brought to therapy, particularly for AD. CONCLUSION AND PERSPECTIVES Adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells contribute to the pathology of neurological diseases and disorders, and are targets of drugs used for the treatment of these diseases, particularly AD and depression. This reveals that adult neurogenesis and the hippocampus
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis
145
are involved in a broad range of physiopathological and pharmacological processes. There are, however, many controversies and debates that rise from the studies reported. Beside the validation and confirmation of the techniques and protocols used in these reports, the role of adult neurogenesis and the hippocampus in neurological diseases and disorders, and drug activities, remain to be elucidated. Adult NSCs represent a promising model for cellular therapy. These studies reveal that adult NSCs may be as important for our understanding of development and physiopathology of the nervous system, as for its pharmacology, thereby opening new opportunities for the treatment and cure of neurological diseases and disorders. Future studies will aim at unraveling the contribution of adult neurogenesis and newly generated neuronal cells to the etiology, pathogenesis and pathology of neurological diseases and disorders, and to develop novel and more effective treatments for these diseases and disorders, particularly for AD, for which there is still no cure, and for depression, which mandates the development of new drugs. REFERENCES Aisen, P. S. 2005. The development of anti-amyloid therapy for Alzheimer ’s disease: From secretase modulators to polymerisation inhibitors. CNS Drugs 19: 989–996. Arvidsson, A., T. Collin, D. Kirik, Z. Kokaia, and O. Lindvall. 2002. Neuronal replacement from endogenous precursors in the adult brain after stroke. Nat Med 8: 963–970. Banasr, M., A. Soumier, M. Hery, E. Mocaër, and A. Daszuta. 2006. Agomelatine, a new antidepressant, induces regional changes in hippocampal neurogenesis. Biol Psychiatry 59: 1087–1096. Belluzzi, O., M. Benedusi, J. Ackman, and J. J. LoTurco. 2003. Electrophysiological differentiation of new neurons in the olfactory bulb. J Neurosci 23: 10411–10418. Bielau, H., K. Trübner, D. Krell, M. W. Agelink, H. G. Bernstein, R. Stauch, C. Mawrin, et al. 2005. Volume deficits of subcortical nuclei in mood disorders A postmortem study. Eur Arch Psychiatry. Clin Neurosci 255: 401–412. Brown, A. B., W. Yang, N. O. Schmidt, R. Carroll, K. K. Leishear, N. G. Rainov, P. M. Black, X. O. Breakefield, and K. S. Aboody. 2003. Intravascular delivery of neural stem cell lines to target intracranial and extracranial tumors of neural and non-neural origin. Hum Gene Ther 14: 1777–17785. Brunello, N., J. Mendlewicz, S. Kasper, B. Leonard, S. Montgomery, J. Nelson, E. Paykel, M. Versiani, and G. Racagni. 2002. The role of noradrenaline and selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibition in depression. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 12: 461–475.
146
Dementia
Cameron, H. A., and R. D. McKay. 2001. Adult neurogenesis produces a large pool of new granule cells in the dentate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 435: 406–417. Cameron, H. A., P. Tanapat, and E. Gould. 1998. Adrenal steroids and N-methyl-Daspartate receptor activation regulate neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of adult rats through a common pathway. Neurosci 82: 349–354. Cameron, H. A., C. S. Woolley, B. S. McEwen, and E. Gould. 1993. Differentiation of newly born neurons and glia in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat. Neurosci 56: 337–344. Campbell, S., and G. Macqueen. 2004. The role of the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci 29: 417–426. Caselli, R. J., T. G. Beach, R. Yaari, and E. M. Reiman. 2006. Alzheimer ’s disease a century later. J Clin Psychiatry 67: 1784–1800. Colla, M., G. Kronenberg, M. Deuschle, K. Meichel, T. Hagen, M. Bohrer, and I. Heuser. 2007. Hippocampal volume reduction and HPA-system activity in major depression. J Psychiatr Res 41: 553–560. Creeley, C., D. F. Wozniak, J. Labruyere, G. T. Taylor, and J. W. Olney. 2006. Low doses of memantine disrupt memory in adult rats. J Neurosci 26: 3923–3932. Curtis, M. A., M. Kam, U. Nannmark, M. F. Anderson, M. Z. Axell, C. Wikkelso, S. Holtas, et al. 2007. Human neuroblasts migrate to the olfactory bulb via a lateral ventricular extension. Science 315: 1243–1249. Curtis, M. A., E. B. Penney, A. G. Pearson, W. M. van Roon-Mom, N. J. Butterworth, M. Dragunow, B. Connor, and R. L. Faull. 2003. Increased cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult human Huntington’s disease brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 9023–9027. David, D. J., K. C. Klemenhagen, K. A. Holick, M. D. Saxe, I. Mendez, L. Santarelli, D. A. Craig, et al. 2007. Efficacy of the MCHR1 antagonist N-[3-(1-{[4-(3,4difluorophenoxy)phenyl]methyl}(4-piperidyl))-4-methylphenyl]-2-methylpropanamide (SNAP 94847) in mouse models of anxiety and depression following acute and chronic administration is independent of hippocampal neurogenesis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 321: 237–248. Donovan, M. H., U. Yazdani, R. D. Norris, D. Games, D. C. German, and A. J. Eisch. 2006. Decreased adult hippocampal neurogenesis in the PDAPP mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. J Comp Neurol 495: 70–83. Eriksson, P. S., E. Perfilieva, T. Bjork-Eriksson, A. M. Alborn, C. Nordborg, D. A. Peterson, and F. H. Gage. 1998. Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med 4: 1313–1317. Estrada, L. D., and C. Soto. 2007. Disrupting beta-amyloid aggregation for Alzheimer disease treatment. Curr Top Med Chem 7: 115–126. Ferri, C. P., M. Prince, C. Brayne, H. Brodaty, L. Fratiglioni, M. Ganguli, K. Hall, et al. 2006. The role of oxidative stress in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer ’s disease. Bratisl Lek Listy 107: 384–394. German, D. C., and A. J. Eisch. 2004. Mouse models of Alzheimer ’s disease: Insight into treatment. Rev Neurosci 15: 353–369.
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis
147
Gould, E., and P. Tanapat. 1997. Lesion-induced proliferation of neuronal progenitors in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat. Neurosci 80: 427–436. Gould, E., P. Tanapat, B. S. McEwen, G. Flügge, and E. Fuchs. 1998. Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 3168–3171. Hindmarch, I. 2001. Expanding the horizons of depression: Beyond the monoamine hypothesis. Hum Psychopharmacol 16: 203–218. Holick, K. A., D. C. Lee, R. Hen, and S. C. Dulawa. 2008. Behavioral effects of chronic fluoxetine in BALB/cJ mice do not require adult hippocampal neurogenesis or the serotonin 1A receptor. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 406–417. Inagaki, M., Y. Matsuoka, Y. Sugahara, T. Nakano, T. Akechi, M. Fujimori, S. Imoto, K. Murakami, and Y. Uchitomi. 2004. Hippocampal volume and first major depressive episode after cancer diagnosis in breast cancer survivors. Am J Psychiatry 161: 2263–2270. Jacobs, B. L., H. Praag, and F. H. Gage. 2000. Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: A novel theory of depression. Mol Psychiatry 5: 262–269. Jin, K., V. Galvan, L. Xie, X. O. Mao, O. F. Gorostiza, D. E. Bredesen, and D. A. Greenberg. 2004. Enhanced neurogenesis in Alzheimer ’s disease transgenic (PDGF-APPSw, Ind) mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 13363–13367. Jin, K., A. L. Peel, X. O. Mao, L. Xie, B. A. Cottrell, D. C. Henshall, and D. A. Greenberg. 2004. Increased hippocampal neurogenesis in Alzheimer ’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 343–347. Jin, K., L. Xie, X. O. Mao, and D. A. Greenberg. 2006. Alzheimer ’s disease drugs promote neurogenesis. Brain Res 1085: 183–188. Kessler, R. C., K. A. McGonagle, S. Zhao, C. B. Nelson, M. Hughes, S. Eshleman, H. U. Wittchen, and K. S. Kendler. 1994. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51: 8–19. Kirsch, I., B. J. Deacon, T. B. Huedo-Medina, A. Scoboria, T. J. Moore, and B. T. Johnson. 2008. Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: A meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLoS Med 5: e45. Kornack, D. R., and P. Rakic. 1999. Continuation of neurogenesis in the hippocampus of the adult macaque monkey. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 5768–5773. Lazic, S. E., H. Grote, R. J. Armstrong, C. Blakemore, A. J. Hannan, A. van Dellen, and R. A. Barker. 2004. Decreased hippocampal cell proliferation in R6/1 Huntington’s mice. Neuroreport 15: 811–813. Li, S. H., and X. J. Li. 2004. Huntingtin-protein interactions and the pathogenesis of Huntington’s disease. Trends Genet 20: 146–154. Lois, C., and A. Alvarez-Buylla. 1994. Long-distance neuronal migration in the adult mammalian brain. Science 264: 1145–1148. Malberg, J. E., and R. S. Duman. 2003. Cell proliferation in adult hippocampus is decreased by inescapable stress: Reversal by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacol 28: 1562–1571.
148
Dementia
Malberg, J. E., A. J. Eisch, E. J. Nestler, and R. S. Duman. 2000. Chronic antidepressant treatment increases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 20: 9104–9110. Minghetti, L. 2005. Role of inflammation in neurodegenerative diseases. Curr Opin Neurol 18: 315–321. Mitsiadis, T. A., O. Barrandon, A. Rochat, Y. Barrandon, and C. De Bari. 2007. Stem cell niches in mammals. Exp Cell Res 313: 3377–3385. Miura, H., N. Ozaki, M. Sawada, K. Isobe, T. Ohta, and T. Nagatsu. 2008. A link between stress and depression: Shifts in the balance between the kynurenine and serotonin pathways of tryptophan metabolism and the etiology and pathophysiology of depression. Stress 11: 198–209. Nowakowski, R. S., and N. L. Hayes. 2000. New neurons: Extraordinary evidence or extraordinary conclusion? Science 288: 771. Owens, M. J. 2004. Selectivity of antidepressants: From the monoamine hypothesis of depression to the SSRI revolution and beyond. J Clin Psychiatry 65: 5–10. Palmer, T. D., P. H. Schwartz, P. Taupin, B. Kaspar, S. A. Stein, and F. H. Gage. 2001. Cell culture. Progenitor cells from human brain after death. Nature 411: 42–43. Parent, J. M., E. Tada, J. R. Fike, and D. H. Lowenstein. 1999. Inhibition of dentate granule cell neurogenesis with brain irradiation does not prevent seizureinduced mossy fiber synaptic reorganization in the rat. J Neurosci 19: 4508–4519. Parent, J. M., T. W. Yu, R. T. Leibowitz, D. H. Geschwind, R. S. Sloviter, and D. H. Lowenstein. 1997. Dentate granule cell neurogenesis is increased by seizures and contributes to aberrant network reorganization in the adult rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 17: 3727–3738. Perera, T. D., J. D. Coplan, S. H. Lisanby, C. M. Lipira, M. Arif, C. Carpio, G. Spitzer, et al. 2007. Antidepressant-induced neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult nonhuman primates. J Neurosci 27: 4894–4901. Pham, K., J. Nacher, P. R. Hof, and B. S. McEwen. 2003. Repeated restraint stress suppresses neurogenesis and induces biphasic PSA-NCAM expression in the adult rat dentate gyrus. Eur J Neurosci 17: 879–886. Pluchino, S., A. Quattrini, E. Brambilla, A. Gritti, G. Salani, G. Dina, R. Galli, et al. 2003. Injection of adult neurospheres induces recovery in a chronic model of multiple sclerosis. Nature 422: 688–694. Raber, J., Y. Huang, and J. W. Ashford. 2004. ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk and AD pathology. Neurobiol Aging 25: 641–650. Reif, A., S. Fritzen, M. Finger, A. Strobel, M. Lauer, A. Schmitt, and K. P. Lesch. 2006. Neural stem cell proliferation is decreased in schizophrenia, but not in depression. Mol Psychiatry 11: 514–522. Rodríguez, J. J., V. C. Jones, M. Tabuchi, S. M. Allan, E. M. Knight, F. M. LaFerla, S. Oddo, and A. Verkhratsky. 2008. Impaired adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of a triple transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer ’s disease. PLoS ONE 3: e2935.
Pharmacology of Adult Neurogenesis
149
Roy, N. S., S. Wang, L. Jiang, J. Kang, A. Benraiss, C. Harrison-Restelli, R. A. Fraser, et al. 2000. In vitro neurogenesis by progenitor cells isolated from the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med 6: 271–277. Santarelli, L., M. Saxe, C. Gross, A. Surget, F. Battaglia, S. Dulawa, N. Weisstaub, et al. 2003. Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301: 805–809. Sawa, A., T. Tomoda, and B. I. Bae. 2003. Mechanisms of neuronal cell death in Huntington’s disease. Cytogenet Genome Res 100: 287–295. Scarpini, E., P. Scheltens, and H. Feldman. 2003. Treatment of Alzheimer ’s disease: Current status and new perspectives. Lancet Neurol 2: 539–547. Scharfman, H., J. Goodman, A. Macleod, S. Phani, C. Antonelli, and S. Croll. 2005. Increased neurogenesis and the ectopic granule cells after intrahippocampal BDNF infusion in adult rats. Exp Neurol 192: 348–356. Schellenberg, G. D. 1995. Genetic dissection of Alzheimer disease, a heterogeneous disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92: 8552–8559. Sheline, Y. I., P. W. Wang, M. H. Gado, J. G. Csernansky, and M. W. Vannier. 1996. Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: 3908–3913. Siuciak, J. A., D. R. Lewis, S. J. Wiegand, and R. M. Lindsay. 1997. Antidepressantlike effect of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). Pharmacol Biochem Behav 56: 131–137. Solomon, B. 2007. Intravenous immunoglobulin and Alzheimer ’s disease immunotherapy. Curr Opin Mol Ther 9: 79–85. St. George-Hyslop, P. H., and A. Petit. 2005. Molecular biology and genetics of Alzheimer ’s disease. C R Biol 328: 119–130. Tattersfield, A. S., R. J. Croon, Y. W. Liu, A. P. Kells, R. L. Faull, and B. Connor. 2004. Neurogenesis in the striatum of the quinolinic acid lesion model of Huntington’s disease. Neurosci 127: 319–332. Taupin, P. 2006. Adult neural stem cells, neurogenic niches and cellular therapy. Stem Cell Reviews 2: 213–219. Taupin, P. 2006. Adult neurogenesis and neuroplasticity. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24: 9–15. Taupin, P. 2007. Protocols for studying adult neurogenesis: Insights and recent developments. Regenerative Medicine 2: 51–62. Taupin, P. 2008. Adult neural stem cells: Redefining the physio- and pathology of the CNS. International Journal of Biomedical Science 4: 100–106. Taupin, P. 2008. Adult neurogenesis and drug therapy. Central Nervous System Agents in Medicinal Chemistry 8: 198–202. Taupin, P. 2008. Adult neurogenesis pharmacology in neurological diseases and disorders. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 8: 311–320. Taupin, P. 2009. Characterization and isolation of synapses of newly generated neuronal cells of the adult hippocampus at early stages of neurogenesis. Journal of Neurodegeneration and Regeneration 2: 9–17.
150
Dementia
Toni, N., E. M. Teng, E. A. Bushong, J. B. Aimone, C. Zhao, A. Consiglio, H. van Praag, M. E. Martone, M. H. Ellisman, and F. H. Gage. 2007. Synapse formation on neurons born in the adult hippocampus. Nat Neurosci 10: 727–734. Vallieres, L., I. L. Campbell, F. H. Gage, and P. E. Sawchenko. 2002. Reduced hippocampal neurogenesis in adult transgenic mice with chronic astrocytic production of interleukin-6. J Neurosci 22: 486–492. Verret, L., J. L. Jankowsky, G. M. Xu, D. R. Borchelt, and C. Rampon. 2007. Alzheimer ’s-type amyloidosis in transgenic mice impairs survival of newborn neurons derived from adult hippocampal neurogenesis. J Neurosci 27: 6771–6780. Wen, P. H., X. Shao, Z. Shao, P. R. Hof, T. Wisniewski, K. Kelley, V. L. Friedrich Jr., et al. 2002. Overexpression of wild type but not an FAD mutant presenilin-1 promotes neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult mice. Neurobiol Dis 10: 8–19. Wilkinson, D. G., P. T. Francis, E. Schwam, and J. Payne-Parrish. 2004. Cholinesterase inhibitors used in the treatment of Alzheimer ’s disease: The relationship between pharmacological effects and clinical efficacy. Drugs Aging 21: 453–478. Wong, M. L., and J. Licinio. 2001. Research and treatment approaches to depression. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 343–351. Zhang, C., E. McNeil, L. Dressler, and R. Siman. 2007. Long-lasting impairment in hippocampal neurogenesis associated with amyloid deposition in a knock-in mouse model of familial Alzheimer ’s disease. Exp Neurol 204: 77–87.
Chapter 8
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care Emmelyne Vasse and Myrra Vernooij-Dassen
WHAT ARE PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS? It is probably not difficult for anyone to understand that the term psychosocial is a combination of the words psychological and social. Despite the simple explanation of the word itself, defining what psychosocial interventions exactly are is not so simple. In dementia literature several definitions could be found for psychosocial interventions and no absolute criteria exist to determine whether a certain intervention for people with dementia should be classified as psychosocial or not. Although psychosocial interventions are always nonpharmacological, nonpharmacological interventions are not necessarily psychosocial. They should not simply be seen as an alternative to drugs. Whereas pharmacological treatments are developed to fit symptoms of a disease, psychosocial interventions should fit the person and caregiver who suffer from disease symptoms. The aim of psychosocial interventions is to optimize the quality of life of people with dementia and their caregivers and could be directed to the patient, the caregiver, or both. Psychosocial interventions could be defined as Interventions usually involving interaction between people, to support cognition, emotion, personal relationships and a sense of control in people with dementia and their family caregivers, through valued, meaningful activity and social integration.1
152
Dementia
PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS IN DEMENTIA CARE Psychosocial interventions became part of dementia treatment in the 1970s. At first interventions focused on the functional symptoms of the disease aiming at rehabilitation and compensation of functioning. Later psychosocial interventions became more emotion-oriented, focusing on the person with dementia and his or her own experiences and coping strategies. These days psychosocial interventions focus on the person with dementia as a whole at any stage of the disease, taking into account the persons functional capacities and subjective experiences and feelings. Theoretical models are often used to explain and explore coping with and adaptation to dementia symptoms. A popular model that is used to develop new psychosocial treatment strategies is the stress-coping model of Lazarus and Folkman (1984). In this model feelings of stress are explained by the way a person appraises a stressful situation and the extent to which he or she feels capable of coping with it. Stress is considered a product of the interplay between the environment and the person and reducing stress could be achieved by changing a person’s perception of stressors and providing strategies to cope (for further reading on psychosocial models see Finnema et al. 2000). To date no treatment exists that can cure or at least stop progression of any type of dementia. Logically, the treatments available for people with dementia and their caregivers focus on postponing cognitive decline as long as possible, alleviating behavioral and psychological symptoms during the course of the disease, and alleviating stress of caregivers. Psychosocial interventions could be effective in treating symptoms and problems related to dementia. Professionals working in dementia care often use clinical guidelines to help them decide which treatment is most appropriate in the case of specific symptoms or situations. In many countries dementia guidelines are available for geriatricians, neurologists, nurses, general practitioners, and other professionals working in dementia care. Guidelines ideally summarize the scientific evidence and best practice that is currently available and most dementia guidelines emphasize the importance of psychosocial interventions in the treatment of people with dementia and their caregivers. Many guidelines even recommend that psychosocial interventions should be the first choice when treating behavioral and psychological disease symptoms. The difficulty with recommendations on psychosocial interventions, however, is that these are often stated rather generally. Whereas a recommendation for a pharmaceutical could be very clear about the dose that should be given, for how long, and when the pharmaceutical treatment should be stopped, this is far more difficult for a psychosocial intervention.
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
153
Psychosocial interventions interfere with the interaction between a person’s psychological state and social environment. Although the symptom(s) to be treated could be the same, the psychological state and social environment differ for each person with dementia. It is up to the healthcare professional to match the situation of a single person with the most appropriate psychosocial intervention. Unfortunately, directions on how to individualize psychosocial interventions are not provided by most dementia guidelines. Yet, treatment success largely depends on a personalized approach. Another difficulty for dementia guidelines is that scientific evidence for the effectiveness of psychosocial interventions in dementia care is growing fast. It could take months to years to develop a high-quality and evidencebased guideline, and by the time it is published new evidence is already available. Dementia guidelines should therefore be updated every few years to keep up with scientific evidence but this is done not that often. The aim of this chapter on psychosocial interventions in dementia care is to summarize current knowledge and evidence for the use of psychosocial interventions in dementia care. It is not meant to give a full and systematic overview of all available evidence but to show the broad range of effects that psychosocial interventions could have in the treatment of behavioral, cognitive, and functional symptoms of people with dementia and the treatment of family caregivers. Problems in dementia care are diverse and dementia-care services and organizations should provide access to a range of psychosocial interventions that can be personalized to individual patients and caregivers. Directions on how to personalize psychosocial interventions and the basic outline of a care plan are given at the end of this chapter. These directions are useful for professional caregivers as well as family caregivers, and apply to the different settings where people with dementia reside, at home, day care, residential care, or nursing home.
PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS FOR PEOPLE WITH DEMENTIA AND THEIR CAREGIVERS Psychosocial interventions described in dementia literature comprise a great variety of strategies and techniques and are used to manage dementia symptoms during the whole course of the disease. Psychosocial treatments could start right after diagnosis and be used till the end stage of the disease. Psychosocial interventions are available that help people with dementia to cope after they have received the diagnosis and people are still aware of their memory problems, or teach them how to use memory
154
Dementia
aids. When the disease progresses interventions could focus on meeting the needs of the person with dementia regarding social and recreational activities, self-care, and daily structure. At the more severe stages interventions could help to ease behavioral disturbances and stimulate interaction with others. Scientific evidence for the effectiveness of psychosocial interventions in the treatment of dementia symptoms is scarce when applying the “gold standard” of randomized controlled trials (RCTs). The Cochrane Library,2 which includes systematic reviews and meta-analyses about the efficacy and effectiveness of treatments for many diseases and conditions, is considered a reliable source of evidence-based practice and used as such by many healthcare professionals. Systematic reviews for different psychosocial interventions in dementia care could be found in this database but for all these interventions evidence is inconclusive (see Table 8.1). This lack of evidence is caused by insufficient high-quality research and not because psychosocial interventions are found to be ineffective. The interventions for which some evidence is available show positive results in favor of the psychosocial intervention but more high-quality studies are needed. Negative effects of interventions are seldom reported as side effects, contrary to most pharmacological interventions used in dementia care. Personal privacy and ethical aspects could be an issue when using some psychosocial interventions, like tracking devices or subjective barriers for people who wander or certain sensory stimulation techniques. The effectiveness for specific psychosocial interventions is hard to prove not only because RCTs are difficult to conduct but also because it is increasingly recognized that psychosocial care should be tailor made. Finding no effects or even undesired effects from psychosocial interventions could indicate that the patient’s needs and preferences did not match the specific intervention. For instance, some people prefer doing things alone or being more physically active whereas others prefer group activities or like to listen to music. It is often stated in study reports of psychosocial interventions that although no overall group effect was found there seemed to be a subgroup within the study sample who benefited from that specific intervention. Evidence for the effectiveness of one standard package of psychosocial interventions that can be recommended to all people with dementia will probably never be found. The evidence for tailored psychosocial interventions is growing rapidly. During the last decade, promising effects have been described in systematic reviews and papers reporting high-quality randomized controlled trials. An overview of these findings is described here.
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
155
Table 8.1 Conclusions from Cochrane Reviews on Psychosocial Interventions Intervention
Conclusion of review
Aroma therapy (January 2009)
Limited evidence, more high-quality RCTs needed
Cognitive training (October 2008)
Limited evidence, more high-quality RCTs needed
Individualized cognitive rehabilitation (October 2008)
Inconclusive due to lack of high-quality RCTs
Massage and touch (October 2008)
Limited evidence, more high-quality RCTs needed
Music therapy (January 2009)
Inconclusive due to lack of high-quality RCTs
Physical activity programs (October 2008)
Limited evidence, more high-quality RCTs needed
Prevention of wandering (October 2009)
Inconclusive due to lack of high-quality RCTs
Reminiscence therapy (January 2009)
Promising but inconclusive evidence, need more high-quality RCTs
Respite care (July 2008)
Limited evidence, more high-quality RCTs needed
Snoezelen (January 2009)
Inconclusive due to lack of high-quality RCTs
Special care units on behavioral symptoms (October 2009)
Inconclusive due to lack of high-quality RCTs
Subjective barriers to prevent wandering (July 2009)
Inconclusive due to lack of high-quality RCTs
Validation therapy (January 2009)
Limited evidence, more high-quality RCTs needed
Note: RCTs = randomized controlled trials.
EFFECTIVE PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS FOR COGNITIVE AND FUNCTIONAL SYMPTOMS Most people with dementia are living in the community and have caregivers, mostly spouses and children, who look after them. Institutionalization is considered undesirable by most patients as well as caregivers but often inevitable as the disease progresses and disease symptoms get worse.
156
Dementia
Nevertheless, staying at home and being independent for as long as possible are very important treatment goals in the early stages of dementia. A psychosocial intervention that has shown significant results and is very promising for people with dementia living at home is community based occupational therapy. A Dutch research group (Graff et al. 2006) set up a program for mild-to-moderate dementia patients and their caregivers, providing them 10 sessions of occupational therapy over five weeks. Dementia patients were taught to compensate for cognitive decline by using memory aids and adapting their environment to the cognitive disabilities. At the same time caregivers were trained in coping strategies and effective supervision of the patient. In a randomized controlled trial the program improved patients’ daily functioning and reduced caregiver burden, and significant effects were still present seven weeks after the last session. The program was even found to be cost-effective when compared to usual home-care. A second psychosocial intervention that proved to be an effective treatment for people with mild-to-moderate dementia is group cognitive stimulation therapy. In a randomized controlled trial Spector et al. (2003) provided a program of 14-sessions twice a week to people with dementia. The program combined aspects of reality orientation and cognitive stimulation but also included aspects of other psychosocial strategies like reminiscence and sensory stimulation. Significant positive effects were found for cognitive functioning and quality of life of patients a week after the last session. The results of the program compared favorably with studies of pharmacological treatments for dementia. Both these psychosocial interventions prove that people with mildto-moderate dementia are very well able to learn despite their cognitive impairment. Advantage should be taken of the cognitive and functional capacities the person with dementia still possesses and could use to compensate for losses. EFFECTIVE PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS FOR BEHAVIORAL SYMPTOMS Loss of memory, cognitive decline, and functional disabilities are probably the disease symptoms mentioned as the most prominent features by the general public when asked what dementia means. However, these symptoms are not regarded as the most challenging when it comes to caring for and living with a person with dementia. The behavioral and psychological disease symptoms are found more difficult to manage and are often experienced as problematic by informal caregivers as well as
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
157
professionals. Symptoms like aggressiveness, anxiety, agitation, depression, wandering, apathy, delusions, hallucinations, and sleep and nighttime disturbances could occur repeatedly for several days to weeks in any person with dementia. It cannot be predicted which symptoms will occur in which patient. Some persons suffer from few symptoms whereas others may have several during the course of the disease, although some symptoms are more likely to occur in specific dementia types than others (see McKeith and Cummings 2005). Behavioral patterns could be recognized during the day and could be related to specific care moments. Although the severity of behavioral and psychological symptoms generally increases when the disease progresses, these symptoms are not necessarily related to the level of cognitive impairment and disease pathology is not considered to be the only reason for these symptoms to occur. Environmental, psychological, and psychosocial factors could also influence the symptoms and in a way behavioral disturbances could be seen as a mechanism to compensate for diminishing communicative abilities. Qualitative research investigating patients’ perspectives on the disease often regard these behavioral and psychological symptoms to be ways of coping with the disease instead of behavioral problems. What appears to be problematic behavior to others is not necessarily experienced as such by dementia patients themselves. Behavioral and psychological symptoms should never be taken for granted but need to be explored by others to discover possible reasons and causes for it. Most guidelines on dementia care recommend that people with dementia who show behavioral and psychological symptoms are treated with a psychosocial intervention first or at least alongside pharmacological treatment. Excepted are those cases where there is immediate danger for the person or the environment. Patients living in the community should be treated the same as those living in residential or nursing homes. In case of aggressive behavior, de-escalation strategies should be used but restraint should be avoided and only used as a last resort. So far, there is no convincing evidence that special care units for people with dementia provide better care than regular nursing home units when it comes to treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms. If there is one thing sure about the effectiveness of psychosocial interventions in the treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms it is that no single treatment works for all patients or in all situations. There is a huge amount of literature available on the subject and although most interventions need more high-quality studies to be conclusive about its effectiveness, a lot of interventions are very promising and could be effective treatments for individual patients.
158
Dementia
A comprehensive systematic review focusing on behavioral and psychological symptoms was carried out by Livingston et al. (2005). It was found that behavioral management techniques are successful in treating symptoms when aimed at the behavior of individual patients. Changing caregiver behavior toward the patient through individual psychoeducation could also be an effective approach. Effects for both interventions could last for months when used in the right way. In addition, psychosocial interventions like music therapy, aromatherapy, validation therapy, reminiscence, snoezelen, or sensory stimulation techniques could produce short-term effects as could simple social interaction. Some of these interventions may produce effects that disappear again immediately or within a short time after the intervention. Nevertheless, short moments of relief from symptoms could be very valuable to patients, caregivers, and staff, and on an individual level improve quality of life. In general, psychosocial interventions that focus on problem-solving and behavioral activation have high odds for producing positive effects in the treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms. A systematic review to identify evidence-based treatments for behavioral disturbances by Logsdon, McCurry, and Teri (2007) found that effective components of successful interventions are teaching staff and family caregivers to observe the behavior carefully, identify possible causes of the symptoms, and make changes to the physical environment of the patient. Pleasant events and activities that involve interaction with others should also be introduced to or increased for the patient. A RCT conducted by Teri and colleagues (2003) investigated a combined program of exercise and teaching caregivers behavioral management techniques. The program was found to improve physical health as well as depression in patients with Alzheimer disease living at home. Components that are most effective in the treatment of depression in dementia patients are similar to those that are effective for behavioral and psychological symptoms in general. Caregiver training in problem-solving should be combined with involving the patient in social activities or one-on-one interactive sessions tailored to the individual preferences of the patient (Teri, McKenzie, and LaFazia 2005). The effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for the treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms in patients with moderately severe to very severe dementia was reviewed by Kverno and colleagues (2009). They found that even in the severest stage dementia patients respond positive to interaction with others. Especially sensory-focused approaches, like aromatherapy, music, and multi-sensory stimulation showed positive short-term effects. Nondemanding and nonverbal strategies are preferred
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
159
treatment options for patients who are no longer capable of interacting verbally. An important message that is stressed in the literature described here is that above all psychosocial interventions are the most beneficial when tailored to the individuals’ needs, preferences, and functional capacities. Behavioral changes often happen for a reason other than progression of the disease. The effectiveness of psychosocial interventions highly depends on the process used to choose the best treatment option. Using careful observation of behavior to draw up an individualized care plan proved to be a successful strategy in a RCT in which person-centered care as well as dementia-care mapping reduced agitation in institutionalized dementia patients (Chenoweth et al. 2009; see Brooker 2004 and 2005 for further reading on dementia care mapping and person-centered care). Before any intervention is started to treat behavioral and psychological symptoms, it is essential to establish the factors likely to generate, aggravate, or improve these symptoms. Ideally a broad assessment is carried out by a trained professional considering the following factors: • • • • • • • •
Physical health Depression Pain or discomfort Side effects of medication Unmet personal preferences or needs Psychosocial factors Physical environment Behavioral and functional analysis
The outcomes of this assessment should be used to determine what intervention is most suitable to the symptoms and a care plan tailored to the patient’s needs and symptoms should be drawn up (see section on care plan). EFFECTIVE PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS FOR FAMILY CAREGIVERS A dementia diagnosis does not only affect the life of the person who receives it but also the life of his or her loved ones. Spouses and children often have no choice but to face a future in which they are forced to care and manage the disease symptoms to their best knowledge. Meanwhile they have to cope with the fact that the person they once knew slowly changes into a care-dependent patient who at times might not even recognize
160
Dementia
them. Despite all this, it is very important for most patients and caregivers to postpone institutionalization as long as possible. In accomplishing this task caregivers often forget to take care of themselves. Caregivers of people with dementia are susceptible to high stress levels, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. Psychosocial interventions for caregivers, therefore, not only focus on improving caring skills and preventing institutionalization but also on preventing the caregiver self from getting overburdened. Male and female caregivers often use different care strategies and also higher education influences the way a caregiver manages the care situation. Caregivers who are socially isolated, have to deal with tense interactions with other family members, or are denying that their relative has dementia are considered at higher risk for negative caregiver outcomes. Naturally, no caregiver-patient dyad is the same and like psychosocial interventions for patients, caregiver interventions should be tailored to the needs and preferences of the individual and the situation. The caregiver and the patient should be treated as a system whose optimal functioning depends on the balance between the caregiver feeling competent to manage the disease and the perceived burden of care. Teaching caregivers effective disease management strategies should be an essential part of dementia treatments because ineffective care strategies could increase behavioral and psychological symptoms and the burden perceived by the caregiver. Eventually this increases the chance that the patient has to be institutionalized after all. One of the most important studies for the effects of psychosocial support for caregivers of people with dementia is probably the New York University (NYU) Spouse-Caregiver Intervention Study conducted by Mittelman and colleagues (1996, 2003). The study started in 1987 and still continues, providing a large longitudinal database that shows that psychosocial support for caregivers can postpone or even avoid institutionalization and improve mental health of spouse-caregivers. The intervention includes participation in a support group, tailored counseling sessions, and ongoing ad hoc counseling available by telephone. Other caregiver intervention studies have also shown significant effects but there is no conclusive scientific evidence for any caregiver intervention to be recommended to all caregivers. However, from systematic reviews it is known which intervention components are more effective than others in preventing institutionalization; these are: • Multicomponent interventions • Longer interventions
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
161
• Interventions producing lasting changes • Active involvement of caregivers in choice making • Involving both caregiver and patient in interventions Interventions that are more structured and intensive and require active participation of caregivers are effective in improving caregiving outcomes like psychological health, well-being, coping, and caring skills. Short programs that only focus on improving knowledge, support groups alone, and brief interventions that are not followed by long-term contact are considered not successful. Psycho-education could be beneficial when caregivers are actively involved. Other psychosocial interventions that could be beneficial when tailored to the specific needs and preferences of the caregiver are training courses about dementia, support groups, dementia-care problem solving, counseling, and respite or short-break services. What intervention is the most effective depends on the desired outcome and the preferences of the caregiver and patient. Caregivers should be assessed and monitored for mood and coping periodically to be able to timely intervene and prevent overburdening. Especially when the person with dementia shows problematic behavior this should be watched closely. In the process of delivering optimal care and support it is very important that once interventions are initiated effects are monitored on a regular base. Ideally, interventions are coordinated by a healthcare professional who is responsible for the continuation of care and knows the patient and the caregiver very well. This healthcare professional should also be able to recognize when enough is enough for the caregiver. Although preventing institutionalization could be a goal of psychosocial interventions, it could also be the best thing to do at some point. Institutionalization is probably the most effective intervention when it comes to reducing caregiver burden and depression. Healthcare professionals working with people with dementia should keep this in mind. For many caregivers there comes a point when no intervention is effective anymore and his or her life is taken over by the patient and the dementia. The possibility of institutionalization should be discussed with the caregiver and patient early in the disease process. It should be recognized that institutionalization is an acceptable option and caregivers should not feel like they are letting down their loved one. Institutionalization is not the end-point of caregiver interventions. Caregivers still should be assessed for mood and coping because they can feel guilty or suffer from depression, and counseling could still be very helpful. Furthermore, the caring proceeds because they are still involved in the life of the patient and may be visiting every day.
162
Dementia
Caregiver interventions should be included in care plans for people with dementia (see section on care plan). In the most ideal case the care plan is drawn up on the basis of shared decisionmaking between the person with dementia, the caregiver, and a healthcare professional who coordinates the provision of interventions and care. PROFESSIONAL CAREGIVERS AND PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS Healthcare professionals and nursing staff are very important people in the process of delivering dementia care and interventions. In many cases they are considered the disease experts by patients and caregivers, and health and social-care managers should ensure that all people working with dementia patients and their caregivers have access to dementia care training. Healthcare professionals and nursing staff should develop their skills consistent with their roles and responsibilities in dementia care. This should also include training in the use of psychosocial interventions. For instance, knowing how to use validation and reality orientation techniques could be very useful in the interaction with disoriented persons with dementia. Based on the literature described above it can be concluded that an essential factor for effective disease management is tailoring interventions to the needs and preferences of the patient and the caregiver, and the desired care outcomes. To achieve this, professionals should involve caregivers as well as patients actively in the process of initiating care and interventions. Choice should be offered and decisions should be based on shared decisionmaking. Professionals should be aware that many people with dementia can understand their diagnosis and receive information, especially in the earlier stages. The effects of individualized and intensive care management by healthcare professionals on patient and caregiver outcomes were investigated in RCTs conducted in the home care situation as well as in residential and nursing homes. Callahan and colleagues (2006) investigated the effectiveness of a collaborative care model integrated within primary care on the quality of dementia care. The intervention existed of one year of care management by an interdisciplinary team led by an advanced practice nurse working with the family caregiver. A primary focus of the intervention was the identification and psychosocial treatment of behavioral and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer ’s patients. The intervention resulted in clinically significant improvements in symptoms and a reduction in caregiver stress. This was achieved without significantly increasing the use of pharmacological treatments.
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
163
Person-centered care has proven to be an effective intervention for patients in residential and nursing homes. In an RCT Fossey and colleagues (2006) succeeded in reducing the use of neuroleptics by training and supporting nursing home staff and promoting person-centered care. During the intervention nursing staff received initial skills training, behavioral management techniques, and ongoing training and support. Although use of neuroleptics was reduced in intervention homes this did not affect agitation or aggressive behavior in residents with dementia. Behavioral disturbances often occur during care activities. Introducing psychosocial approaches like person-centered care and dementia-care mapping into residential facilities could be effective interventions for reducing care resistiveness and distress in patients. Teaching care staff to use these tools could improve psychosocial care in these settings and both staff and residents with dementia would benefit (see also Edvardsson, Winblad, and Sandman 2008). Although training and supporting care staff is important, this is easier said than done. Time and money are often too short to assure continuous skill development and training. Introducing and implementing new care strategies is challenging, especially when new approaches are in conflict with previous knowledge and training. As with family caregivers not all professionals use the same care strategies or are aware that their behavior influences that of a person with dementia. Healthcare professionals and nursing staff should keep in mind that they could make a difference in the quality of care and life of dementia patients and their caregivers. CARE PLAN Treatment of dementia always involves at all stages emphasizing the unique qualities of the individual with dementia and recognizing the patient’s personal and social needs. A key issue is to focus on positive behavior and things the person can still do instead of focusing on the inabilities. Activities should be adjusted to ensure that they are achievable with the limitations the patient has. To be able to deliver individualized care it is essential to draw up a combined care plan addressing care and interventions of both the patient and the caregiver. Interventions could start as soon as a person receives the diagnosis and planning care and interventions should continue throughout the disease. In case of problematic behavioral and psychological symptoms that cause distress for the patient and his or her environment a separate care plan should be drawn up to address these specific symptoms (see section on behavioral symptoms).
164
Dementia
It is recommended to at least include the following personal and social information of the patient in the care plan: • • • • • •
Sensory impairment (hearing, sight, changes in taste) Communication difficulties Ill health Life history Social and family circumstances Needs and preferences (important to person with dementia and related to the quality of life) • Cognitive abilities • Physical abilities This basic information should be updated periodically and should be used as the starting point for choosing and offering care and interventions. Ideally for each patient and caregiver the care plan should address: For the patient • Activities of daily living (ADL) that maximize independent activity, enhance function, adapt and develop skills, and minimize the need for support • Periodic assessment for depression and/or anxiety and monitoring of treatment if applicable • Recreational and social activities • Structured day activities For the caregiver • Offering possibilities for respite or short-break care • Offering possibilities for other psychosocial interventions, depending on needs and preferences • Periodic assessment for mood and coping Continuity is essential and without a review and update of at least once a year the combined care plan is of no significance. The plan should be drawn up and monitored by an assigned professional who maintains regular contact with the patient and the main caregiver. Whether this is a general practitioner, geriatric specialist, nurse, case manager, or psychologist depends on the way dementia care is organized in the region where the patient and caregiver live. Most important is that the patient and the caregiver are offered choices between different treatment options and the
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
165
care plan is drawn up in mutual agreement between the patient, the caregiver, and healthcare professional. The care professional is trained in providing dementia specific care, knows what interventions are available in the area, and could be contacted easily by the patient and caregiver in case of sudden changes or problems. The frequency of review of the care plan should be agreed by the caregiver, professional, and preferably also by the patient depending on the disease stage. CONCLUSION/DISCUSSION The purpose of this chapter about psychosocial interventions in dementia care is not to give a systematic overview of the literature but to gain insight in the opportunities that psychosocial interventions offer to people with dementia and their caregivers to maximize their quality of life while living with the disease. Healthcare professionals, family caregivers, and people with dementia should be aware that the dementia diagnosis does not mean the end of life but starting a new one, adapted to the disease symptoms. This is not different from other chronic diseases. The difficulty with dementia, however, is that patients slowly lose the ability to communicate what they need or feel. The use of psychosocial interventions is not limited to a particular stage or type of dementia and it is recommended that psychosocial interventions are offered very soon after the diagnosis. This way the patient and family caregiver will learn about the possibilities for care and interventions. Ideally, this is coordinated by a healthcare professional who knows the patient and caregiver and is able to anticipate future events and problems. At the early stages of the disease people with dementia should be involved in decisionmaking. They are often well aware of their disease and the burden they may cause to family members or others. Psychosocial interventions at this stage could help people with dementia and their caregivers to cope and adapt life to disease symptoms by increasing disease knowledge and optimizing cognitive and functional capacities of the patient. When the disease progresses the focus of psychosocial interventions shifts toward the management of behavioral and psychological symptoms. At a certain stage it is very difficult to involve the patient in decisionmaking and others have to decide instead. Knowing the person with dementia from the very beginning enables the healthcare professional to offer tailored psychosocial interventions at this stage, based on previous experiences and knowledge about the patient’s character and preferences. It should be noted that maximizing quality of life of a person with dementia and the caregiver could also mean stopping interventions and
166
Dementia
setting new goals. This relates to the caregiver in case institutionalization can no longer be postponed as well as the person with dementia in case memory aids are no longer effective and more care is needed. Healthcare professionals involved in dementia care should not hesitate to discuss this with the patient and caregiver. The disease does not stop from progressing over time and psychosocial interventions need to be adapted to a new situation. Based on the literature available to date it can be concluded that the effectiveness of psychosocial interventions is related to many things other than the specific intervention itself. What works for one person is not automatically effective for another. Furthermore, previous success of a specific psychosocial intervention does not warrant future success in the same person. Dementia care services and organizations should ensure that a broad range of psychosocial interventions is available. This collection of potential effective interventions should be seen as a tool box from which psychosocial care could be custom made to fit a person with dementia’s and the caregiver ’s unique situation whether at home or in a residential or nursing home. THE FUTURE FOR PSYCHOSOCIAL INTERVENTIONS IN DEMENTIA CARE It is likely that in the future many more psychosocial interventions will be found effective for the treatment of dementia symptoms. Many highquality studies into the effects of specific psychosocial interventions are currently being conducted and published. With respect to future research it is important that similar outcome measures are used in studies, so that results from different studies could be compared. From the studies that are described in this chapter it could be learned that it is worthwhile to follow up study participants for longer time periods, months or even years. The study from the New York University shows that this provides important insight in long-term effects that would not have been found otherwise. From both the occupational therapy program and the cognitive stimulation therapy program it could be learned that the development of an effective psychosocial intervention takes time. Both programs were based on successful features from earlier intervention studies and piloted thoroughly before the RCT study. In the end these efforts paid off and resulted in significant treatment effects for people with mild-to-moderate dementia. It is slowly becoming accepted that RCTs may not fit the complexity of psychosocial intervention research and that research should also focus on
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
167
effective processes for matching an intervention to the individual needs of a person with dementia and his or her caregiver. Involving people with dementia in research to gain insight in their subjective needs and experiences would provide a good starting point. Stratifying participant samples by severity of disease or including only participants of specific disease stages would provide knowledge about which psychosocial interventions are effective or ineffective in what stages of dementia. Tools and approaches like dementia-care mapping and person-centered care have shown to be very effective in personalizing care and improving outcomes. More research is needed to find out if these techniques indeed are useful for all patients and stages. A last important issue for researchers to recognize is that in dementia care short-term effects could be as important as long-term effects, for instance, in case of wandering during meal times or anxiety and aggressiveness during bathing. Improving these symptoms for a short time would improve quality of life and care and should therefore be considered meaningful. The future for psychosocial interventions in dementia care is not only dependent on the quality of research but also on its widespread implementation in care practice. Dementia guidelines are an important source for healthcare professionals of any discipline and many guidelines include recommendations on the use of psychosocial interventions. However, recommendations are useless if psychosocial care is not actually made available to people with dementia and their caregivers in the region they live. Implementing effective psychosocial interventions needs attention from governments and researchers. Implementation studies could shed light on the barriers and facilitators for successful implementation. Policymakers should provide the means to facilitate implementation and assure continuous training of healthcare professionals. Eventually, all people with dementia and their caregivers should have access to the best quality care available. Recommended further reading: E. Moniz-Cook and J. Manthorpe, eds. 2009. Early psychosocial interventions in dementia: Evidence-based practice. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. NOTES 1. Definition from the April 2010 Mission statement of INTERDEM (early detection and timely INTerventions in DEMentia), a European network of applied researchers into dementia care practice, established in 2000. http://interdem.alzheimer-europe.org.
168
Dementia
2. Abstracts, conclusions and plain language summaries of Cochrane Reviews are accessible via http://www.thecochranelibrary.com/view/0/ index.html.
REFERENCES Ballard, C., A. Corbett, R. Chitramohan, and D. Aarsland. 2009. Management of agitation and aggression associated with Alzheimer ’s disease: Controversies and possible solutions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 22: 532–540. Brodaty, H., A. Green, and A. Koschera. 2003. Meta-analysis of psychosocial interventions for caregivers of people with dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 51: 657–664. Brooker, D. 2004. What is person-centred care in dementia? Reviews in Clinical Gerontology 13: 215–222. Brooker, D. 2005. Dementia care mapping: A review of the research literature. Gerontologist 45 (1): 11–18. Callahan, C. M., M. A. Boustani, F. W. Unverzagt, M. G. Austrom, T. M. Damush, A. J. Perkins, B. A. Fultz, S. L. Hui, S. R. Counsell, and H. C. Hendrie. 2006. Effectiveness of collaborative care for older adults with Alzheimer disease in primary care: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 295: 2148–2157. Chenoweth, L., M. T. King, Y.-H. Jeon, H. Brodaty, J. Stein-Parbury, R. Norman, M. Haas, and G. Luscombe. 2009. Caring for Aged Dementia Care Resident Study (CADRES) of person-centred care, dementia-care mapping, and usual care in dementia: A cluster-randomised trial. Lancet Neurology 8: 317–325. Cohen-Mansfield, J., and J. E. Mintzer. 2005. Time for change: The role of nonpharmacological interventions in treating behavior problems in nursing home residents with dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 19: 37–40. de Boer, M. E., C. M. P. M. Hertogh, R.-M. Droes, I. I. Riphagen, C. Jonker, and J. A. Eefsting. 2007. Suffering from dementia—the patient’s perspective: A review of the literature. International Psychogeriatrics 19: 1021–1039. de Vugt, M. E., F. Stevens, P. Aalten, R. Lousberg, N. Jaspers, I. Winkens, J. Jolles, and F. R. J. Verhey. 2004. Do caregiver management strategies influence patient behaviour in dementia? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 19: 85–92. Edvardsson, D., B. Winblad, and P. O. Sandman. 2008. Person-centred care of people with severe Alzheimer ’s disease: Current status and ways forward. Lancet Neurology 7: 362–367. Finnema, E., R. M. Droes, M. Ribbe, and W. van Tilburg. 2000. A review of psychosocial models in psychogeriatrics: Implications for care and research. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 14: 68–80. Fossey J, C. Ballard, E. Juszczak, I. James, N. Alder, R. Jacoby, and R. Howard. 2006. Effect of enhanced psychosocial care on antipsychotic use in nursing home
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
169
residents with severe dementia: Cluster randomised trial. British Medical Journal 332: 756–761. Gaugler, J. E., D. L. Roth, W. E. Haley, and M. S. Mittelman. 2008. Can counseling and support reduce burden and depressive symptoms in caregivers of people with Alzheimer ’s disease during the transition to institutionalization? Results from the New York University Caregiver Intervention Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56: 412–428. Graff, M. J. L., M. J. M. Vernooij-Dassen, M. Thijssen, J. Dekker, W. H. L. Hoefnagels, and M. G. M. Olde Rikkert. 2006. Community based occupational therapy for patients with dementia and their care givers: Randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 333: 1196 Kverno, K. S., B. S. Black, M. T. Nolan, and P. V. Rabins. 2009. Research on treating neuropsychiatric symptoms of advanced dementia with non-pharmacological strategies, 1998–2008: A systematic literature review. International Psychogeriatrics 21: 825–843. Lazarus, R. S., and S. Folkman. 1984. Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer. Livingston, G., K. Johnston, C. Katona, J. Paton, C. G. Lyketsos, and Old Age Task Force of the World Federation of Biological Psychiatry. 2005. Systematic review of psychological approaches to the management of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry 162: 1996–2021. Logsdon, R. G., S. M. McCurry, and L. Teri. 2007. Evidence-based psychological treatments for disruptive behaviors in individuals with dementia. Psychology and Aging 22: 28–36. McKeith, I., and J. Cummings. 2005. Behavioural changes and psychological symptoms in dementia disorders. Lancet Neurology 4: 735–742. Mittelman, M. S. 2003. Psychosocial intervention for dementia caregivers: What can it accomplish? International Psychogeriatrics 15 (Suppl. 1): 247–249. Mittelman, M. S., S. H. Ferris, E. Shulman, G. Steinberg, and B. Levin. 1996. A family intervention to delay nursing home placement of patients with Alzheimer disease. A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 276: 1725–1731. Moniz-Cook, E., and M. Vernooij-Dassen. 2006. Editorial: Timely psychosocial intervention in dementia: A primary care perspective. Dementia 5: 307–315. O’Connor, D. W., D. Ames, B. Gardner, and M. King. 2009. Psychosocial treatments of behavior symptoms in dementia: A systematic review of reports meeting quality standards. International Psychogeriatrics 21: 225–240. Pinquart, M., and S. Sörensen. 2006. Helping caregivers of persons with dementia: Which interventions work and how large are their effects? International Psychogeriatrics 18: 577–595. Smits, C. H., J. de Lange, R. M. Dröes, F. Meiland, M. Vernooij-Dassen, and A. M. Pot. 2007. Effects of combined intervention programmes for people with dementia living at home and their caregivers: a systematic review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 22: 1181–1193.
170
Dementia
Sörensen, S., P. Duberstein, D. Gill, and M. Pinquart. 2006. Dementia care: Mental health effects, intervention strategies, and clinical implications. Lancet Neurology 5: 961–973. Spector, A., L. Thorgrimsen, B. Woods, L. Royan, S. Davies, M. Butterworth, and M. Orrell. 2003. Efficacy of an evidence-based cognitive stimulation therapy programme for people with dementia: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 183: 248–254. Spijker, A., M. Vernooij-Dassen, E. Vasse, E. Adang, H. Wollersheim, R. Grol, and F. Verhey. 2008. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological interventions in delaying the institutionalization of patients with dementia: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 56: 1116–1128. Teri, L., L. E. Gibbons, S. M. McCurry, R. G. Logsdon, D. M. Buchner, W. E. Barlow, W. A. Kukull, A. Z. LaCroix, W. McCormick, and E. B. Larson. 2003. Exercise plus behavioral management in patients with Alzheimer disease: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 290: 2015–2022. Teri, L., G. McKenzie, and D. LaFazia. 2005. Psychosocial treatment of depression in older adults with dementia. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 12: 303–316. Vasse, E., M. Vernooij-Dassen, E. Moniz-Cook, B. Woods, M. Franco, I. Cantegreil, P. Dorenlot, K. Charras, and M. O’Connell. 2008. European guidelines on psychosocial interventions. In Dementia in Europe Yearbook 2008. Luxembourg.
SYSTEMATIC REVIEWS, COCHRANE LIBRARY Chung, J. C. C., and C. K. Y. Lai. 2002. Snoezelen for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4). Art. No.: CD003152. DOI: 10.1002/14651858. CD003152. Clare, L., and B. Woods. 2003. Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive training for early-stage Alzheimer ’s disease and vascular dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4). Art. No.: CD003260. DOI: 10.1002/14651858. CD003260. Forbes, D., S. Forbes, D. G. Morgan, M. Markle-Reid, J. Wood, and I. Culum. 2008. Physical activity programs for persons with dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (3). Art. No.: CD006489. DOI: 10.1002/14651858. CD006489.pub2. Hansen, N. V., T. Jørgensen, L. Ørtenblad. 2006. Massage and touch for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4). Art. No.: CD004989. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD004989.pub2. Hermans, D., U. H. Htay, and S. J. Cooley. 2007. Non-pharmacological interventions for wandering of people with dementia in the domestic setting. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (1). Art. No.: CD005994. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD005994.pub2. Holt, F. E., T. P. H. Birks, L. M. Thorgrimsen, A. E. Spector, A. Wiles, and M. Orrell. 2003. Aroma therapy for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (3). Art. No.: CD003150. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD003150.
Psychosocial Interventions in Dementia Care
171
Lai, C. K. Y., J. H. M. Yeung, V. Mok, and I. Chi. 2009. Special care units for dementia individuals with behavioural problems. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4). Art. No.: CD006470. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006470.pub2. Lee, H., and M. H. Cameron. 2004. Respite care for people with dementia and their carers. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (1). Art. No.: CD004396. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD004396.pub2. Neal, M., and P. Barton Wright. 2003. Validation therapy for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (3). Art. No.: CD001394. DOI: 10.1002/14651858. CD001394. Price, J. D., D. Hermans, and J. Grimley Evans. 2001. Subjective barriers to prevent wandering of cognitively impaired people. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (1). Art. No.: CD001932. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD001932. Thompson, C. C. A., and K. Spilsbury. 1998. Support for carers of people with Alzheimer ’s type dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (3). Art. No.: CD000454. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD000454. Vink, A. C., J. Birks, M. S. Bruinsma, and R. J. P. M. Scholten. 2003. Music therapy for people with dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (4). Art. No.: CD003477. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD003477.pub2. Woods, B., A. E. Spector, C. A. Jones, M. Orrell, and S. P. Davies. 2005. Reminiscence therapy for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (2). Art. No.: CD001120. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD001120.pub2.
Chapter 9
Depression and Dementia Jane S. Saczynski and Rosanna M. Bertrand
It is currently estimated that worldwide approximately 29 million adults suffer from dementia with 4.6 million new cases occurring every year (Ferri et al. 2005). Projections indicate that by the year 2040 this estimate will jump to a staggering 81.1 million adults who suffer from dementia with Alzheimer ’s disease being the most common form (Ferri et al. 2005; World Health Organization 2003). The burden of dementia on the patient is tremendous in that it generally transcends all aspects of the patient’s life including social, psychological, and eventually physical functioning. In addition to the patient and those providing informal care, society overall encumbers the high cost of dementia in terms of providing and financing health care to these patients. It cannot be disputed that the burden of dementia is high, yet the impact of dementia is compounded when depression is present as a co-existing condition. Depression is one of the most common neuropsychiatric comorbidities of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) (Amore et al. 2007). Although it is well known that depression is a common disorder that affects about 121 million people worldwide (World Health Organization 2003), the prevalence of depression in the presence of dementia is less well understood. Reported statistics on prevalence rates of the co-occurrence of depression and dementia vary considerably, with outer estimates appearing as high as 87%, although most estimates fall between 30% and 50% (Amore et al. 2007; Lee and Lyketsos 2003; Wright and Persad 2007). Prevalence rates of depression specifically in AD suggest a comorbitidy of 20–25% with major depressive episodes and an additional 20–30% with other depressive syndromes including minor depression (Amore et al. 2007; Olin et al. 2002).
174
Dementia
The primary reason for the variability in prevalence rates is the use of different types of research designs, sample populations, and assessment tools to generate population statistics. For example, many of the prevalence estimates use community-based epidemiological samples that are large and generalizable, but lack specificity in measurement. In contrast, other estimates are based on clinical examinations using patient-based populations where a structured interview is conducted. In general, higher rates of depression in dementia are obtained from clinical populations compared to epidemiologic samples, due to the more advanced stage of disease and more severe depression in the former. Not only is the prevalence of depression in dementia unclear, the relationship between these conditions is also murky and remains controversial. For example, some evidence points to depression as a risk factor for dementia (Devanand et al. 1996; Berger et al. 1999; Lockwood et al. 2000; Saczynski et al. 2010), while other investigations suggest a reverse causality where depression is a symptom of the prodromal phase of dementia pathology in older adulthood (Chen et al. 2009; Alexopoulos et al. 1997; O’Brien, Ames, and Schweitzer 1996; Hickie et al. 1997). Because dementia is a degenerative condition with a long prodromal phase, temporality is difficult to resolve. The complexity of these comorbid conditions has implications for diagnosis and treatment and has far-reaching consequences for the patient, the caregivers, and society overall. Combined, the high estimated prevalence of depression and dementia, as well as the complexity of their relationship, make diagnosis and treatment difficult and has created great public health concern. It is incumbent upon healthcare providers who treat older adults to understand the possible mechanisms and underlying factors through which depression and dementia may be linked, and to conduct a thorough neuropsychological evaluation when either depression or dementia are suspected. In this chapter we present issues relevant to the co-occurrence of depression and dementia that are clinically relevant to healthcare providers of older adults. We first provide a discussion of investigations that have explored how to distinguish depression from dementia based on neurological test performance and by examining brain structure and function. We then present research that underscores the complexity of diagnosing depression in patients with dementia, taking into consideration common symptoms and an inability of demented patients to communicate symptoms of depression. In the next two sections we consider factors that have been demonstrated to have an independent relationship with depression and/or dementia and that are postulated to impact the
Depression and Dementia
175
relationship between these conditions as they co-occur or as they occur in a temporal order. The factors that we consider include demographic characteristics and personal and family history of depression. Studies on factors and potential mechanisms that underlie the association between depression and dementia, including vascular changes, chronic inflammation, and brain-derived neurotrophic factor, are reviewed. Treatment for depression in dementia, including pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic interventions, is discussed. We also examine the consequences of depression in dementia. In this section we demonstrate that the burden of the co-occurrence of depression and dementia impacts not only the patient, but also the caregiver and the greater society through increased healthcare utilization and overall healthcare costs. Finally, we summarize the current state of the research on the co-occurrence of depression and dementia, and make suggestions for the direction of future research. DISTINGUISHING DEPRESSION FROM DEMENTIA Due to the high prevalence of comorbidity among older adults (Parekh and Barton 2010), clinicians treating this population are frequently faced with questions about the genesis or etiology of symptoms in the presence of multiple conditions. One particularly difficult question of significant importance that is often presented to practicing geriatricians and psychiatrists is whether cognitive symptoms exhibited by a depressed patient are related to the mood disorder or are reflective of neurodegeneration. The ability to distinguish cognitive deficits associated with depression from those associated with the dementing process has implications for depression treatment and the potential amelioration or retardation of cognitive impairment. Neuropsychological Test Performance Neuropsychological assessments have been widely examined as a way to distinguish cognitive deficits associated with depression as compared to those reflective of the early stages of neurodegeneration (Jorm 2000; Christensen et al. 1997; Crowe and Hoogenraad 1999; King et al. 1990; Dannenbaum, Parkinson, and Inman 1988; Gray, Rattan, and Dean 1986). It has been well documented that, compared to nondepressed persons, depressed individuals have poorer performance in a number of cognitive domains including memory, processing speed, verbal fluency, attention, executive function, and visuospatial ability. While some of these domains may also be impaired in dementia (Looi and Sachdev 1999; Backman et al.
176
Dementia
2005), a number of studies have demonstrated that the pattern of cognitive impairment in depression is distinct from that of dementia (Christensen et al. 1997; Crowe and Hoogenraad 1999; Gray, Rattan, and Dean 1986). The varying pattern of domain specific performance in neuropsychological tests in persons with dementia as compared to those who are depressed offers the ability to distinguish the two conditions. Two hypotheses have been suggested for the underlying differences in domain specific performance on cognitive tasks between persons with depression and dementia. The first is the effortful processing hypothesis and the second is that the overall pattern of impairment differs in the two conditions. Each is described below. The effortful processing hypothesis postulates that depressed persons will perform more poorly than nondepressed individuals proportional to the amount of effort the task requires (Bazin et al. 1994; Roy-Byrne et al. 1986). That is, depressed persons may not have an impairment in function per se, but rather do not vary the amount of effort they put into a task depending on the level of difficulty of the task. In contrast, demented persons will vary the amount of effort they exert appropriately, but may have impairments in function resulting in poor performance. Comparisons of the pattern of responses on tests requiring varying levels of effort lend insight into the etiology of differences in performance between demented and depressed individuals. Indeed, several studies have found that tasks of effortful processing (the amount of effort needed to complete a task) mediate the association between depression and cognitive function (Palsson et al. 2000; Nebes et al. 2000; Butters et al. 2004). In contrast, the effort required to complete a task is not related to the performance of patients with dementia; rather poor performance is associated with cognitive deficits. The second hypothesis postulates that there may be domain-specific deficits in cognitive performance among adults who are depressed which are different from the deficits in adults with dementia. In general, studies have found that deficits in speed or attention and executive function are indicative of depression while memory impairments are more likely typical in dementia (Christensen et al. 1997; Herrmann, Goodwin, and Ebmeier 2007). Several studies have shown that while depressed persons exhibit somewhat attenuated performance on memory tasks, executive function is the cognitive ability most severely impaired in depression (ElderkinThompson et al. 2004; Rapp et al. 2005). In contrast, memory performance is often impaired early in dementia. Direct comparisons of memory performance in persons with depression to those with dementia have shown that demented individuals have poorer performance, particularly in
Depression and Dementia
177
delayed recall, retrieval of learned information, and recognition memory tasks (King et al. 1990; Gray, Rattan, and Dean 1986; Jean et al. 2005; Hart et al. 1987; La Rue et al. 1986). Despite a large body of evidence on differences in the neuropsychological profiles of depressed and demented persons, there is still ambiguity. Some studies of neuropsychological profiles among persons with depression and dementia have also found that demented patients perform more poorly overall, irrespective of the cognitive ability examined (Nelson 1993), while others have suggested that neuropsychological tests do not distinguish depressed from demented persons (O’Carroll et al. 1994). In addition, among depressed individuals, neuropsychological performance may predict dementia (Jean et al. 2005). Jean and colleagues found that among depressed persons, those who had more impairment on tasks of attention and memory were more likely to develop dementia over a seven-year follow-up period (Jean et al. 2005). Brain Structure and Function Many structural brain changes associated with dementia, including rate and severity of white matter hyperintensities and infarctions, and hippocampal, grey- and white-matter volume, are associated with latelife depression (Dotson et al. 2009; Bremner et al. 2000; Iidaka et al. 1996; Taylor et al. 2005). Therefore, it may be difficult to distinguish depression from dementia based on brain structure and function. However, several differences in the pattern of changes in brain structure and function between depressed and demented older individuals have been noted. One functional difference is that decreased cerebral glucose metabolism is observed in dementia and AD (Buckner et al. 2005), while increased metabolism has been reported from studies of geriatric depression (Smith et al. 2009). In addition, persons with AD and concomitant depression have been found to exhibit more severe neuropathologic indicators of AD than persons with AD alone (Rapp et al. 2006, 2008). Further, depression is associated with AD neuropathology even in the absence of a diagnosis of dementia (Butters et al. 2008). However, to our knowledge, direct comparisons of neuropathologic profiles in depression and dementia have not been conducted. DIAGNOSING DEPRESSION IN DEMENTIA Similar to distinguishing depression from dementia, the diagnosis of depression in the setting of dementia can be complex. Among demented
178
Dementia
patients, many symptoms of depression, such as mood swings, weight loss, psychomotor slowing and insomnia, are common irrespective of whether patients are depressed (Small 2009). In addition, some of the early behavior changes common in depression, such as reduced social engagement and apathy, are also common in the early stages of dementia, often making the two conditions difficult to distinguish. Symptoms of depression may fluctuate over time in patients with dementia, particularly those with a history of depression or mood disorders. These fluctuations in symptoms may make it difficult to diagnose depression since symptoms may not meet established criteria for intensity, duration, or functional impact required for diagnosis. This is particularly problematic when diagnosing major depressive disorder and less of a problem when diagnosing high depressive symptomatology. For these reasons, depression in the setting of dementia can be very subtle and difficult to detect, even for experienced psychiatrists, neurologists, and geriatricians. These clinical ambiguities have resulted in the underdiagnosis of depression in many settings including primary care practices, nursing homes, and hospitals (Mulsant and Ganguli 1999). Another factor that complicates the diagnosis of depression in demented persons is that often a demented patient does not communicate his or her depressive symptoms in a way that is amenable to diagnosis. It has been hypothesized that demented patients may not be aware of their depressive symptoms (Chemerinski et al. 2001), and in many cases, caregiver accounts are necessary in order to accurately assess whether or not a patient is depressed. Demented patients often report themselves to be less depressed than do their caregivers and clinicians. For instance, a study of 75 outpatients with diagnosed AD compared concordance of ratings of depression between the patient, caregiver, and clinician. They found that patients rated themselves as less depressed than did their caregivers and clinicians and that results did not vary by severity of dementia (Teri and Wagner 1991). Similar results were found in a smaller study of 31 AD patients and their caregivers; caregivers reported more depressive symptoms than did the patients themselves and concordance between ratings did not vary according to the depression status of the caregiver (Moye, Robiner, and Mackenzie 1993). Another study examined whether there were specific symptoms of depression that differed between caregiver and patient reports (Mackenzie, Robiner, and Knopman 1989). They found that patient ratings identified only 14% of the sample as depressed while caregiver ratings identified one-half of the sample as depressed. Discordance was noted for specific symptoms including patients’ loss of interest or pleasure, irritability, fatigue, and feelings of worthlessness.
Depression and Dementia
179
Another complexity to diagnosis of depression in demented patients is that many depression screening instruments were not developed specifically for older populations and the validation of these scales among demented older persons has been sparse. The Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Sheikh and Yesavage 1986), Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) (Hedlund 1979), the Cornell Scale for Depression (Alexopoulos et al. 1988), and the DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association 1994) have been most widely compared in the published literature. Differences in the rates of depression among demented persons based on these various different screening tools have been compared in a number of studies. A large study of 288 outpatients with dementia found the prevalence of depression to be 8.0% according to the GDS, 7.4% using the HRSD, and 6.3% according to DSM-IV criteria (Brodaty and Luscombe 1996). Rates of depression also differed by dementia subtype among the scales. Depression was more likely to be diagnosed in vascular dementia than in Alzheimer ’s disease using the HRSD and the GDS, whereas rates according to the DSM-IV criteria did not differ by dementia subtype. Of note is the low prevalence of depression in this study compared to the previously described literature in this chapter. Provisional criteria for the diagnosis of depression of Alzheimer ’s disease (NIMH-dAD) were developed as part of a workshop sponsored by the National Institute of Mental Health (Olin, Katz, et al. 2002; Olin, Schneider, et al. 2002). The NIHM-dAD criteria are similar to the DSM-IV criteria for major depression, but incorporate modifications to address specific characteristics of depression in AD. For instance, the NIMHdAD require three or more symptoms of depression rather than the five required for major depression; these criteria include irritability and social isolation or withdrawal as candidate symptoms of depression, and include ‘decreased positive affect or pleasure’ instead of loss of interest or pleasure, and require that the symptoms occur during the prior two-week period and represent change from previous function, but do not require that their symptoms occur every day. Validation studies of the NIMHdAD are underway. A recent study compared the NIMH-dAD to the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia, the GDS and the DSM-IV criteria among 101 patients with AD (Teng et al. 2008). They found that the frequency of depression was significantly higher using the NIMH-dAD (44%) than that obtained using the DSM-IV criteria for major or minor depression (36%) or using the established cut-points on the GDS (33%) and the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia (30%). The authors suggest that, compared to the DSM-IV criteria and the other scales, the NIMH-dAD criteria are less
180
Dementia
stringent with respect to the requirements for frequency and duration of symptoms, resulting in higher prevalence estimates. One recent study examined the validity of the Cornell Scale for Depression and the GDS in 145 patients over the age of 65 who were either depressed only, demented only, demented and depressed, or control subjects (Korner et al. 2006). They found that while the scales were equally valid for assessing depression in an elderly population, the Cornell Depression Scale retained its sensitivity and specificity (93% and 97%, respectively using a cut-point of ≥6) in demented subjects. In contrast, the validity of the GDS diminished in the demented population. It is possible that the symptoms assessed using the GDS are more ambiguous in dementia or may overlap with symptoms experienced in dementia, altering the validity of this instrument among demented persons. Overall, these findings suggest that diagnosis of depression in patients with dementia is challenging and may require consideration of results from numerous depression screening tools as well as caregiver reports. Use of depression scales that focus on symptoms not shared by depression and dementia may also enhance reliability and validity of assessment. Validity of screening tools in demented populations should be considered before choosing a screening instrument. Incorporating reports from patients, caregivers, and clinicians may provide the most complete and accurate picture of the patient’s emotional state. DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS AND DEPRESSION IN DEMENTIA As the research that we present throughout this chapter will demonstrate, the relationship between depression and dementia in older adults is complex, unclear and currently remains largely controversial. Although several prospective studies demonstrate that baseline depressive symptoms are associated with an increased risk for cognitive decline, dementia, and Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) (Berger et al. 1999; Lockwood et al. 2000; Devanand et al. 1996; Jorm 2001; Green et al. 2003), others fail to confirm this relationship (Henderson et al. 1997; Dufouil et al. 1996; Chen et al. 1999). Furthermore, even among studies where a relationship between depression and dementia has been established, there is conflicting evidence regarding the nature and direction of the relationship. The preponderance of the evidence points to depression as a risk factor for dementia (Berger et al. 1999; Lockwood et al. 2000; Saczynski et al. 2010; Devanand et al. 1996; Green et al. 2003), yet numerous investigations suggest a theory of reverse causality where depression is a symptom of the prodromal phase
Depression and Dementia
181
of dementia in older adulthood (Chen et al. 2009; Alexopoulos et al. 1997; O’Brien et al. 1996; Hickie et al. 1997). That is, structural changes in the brain associated with dementia may lead to symptoms of late-life depression (Boland 2000). Some of the conflicting evidence can be attributed to whether the findings are generated from community-based epidemiological samples or clinical studies using patient-based populations. Indeed, these two study designs vary significantly in the types of samples that are used, and in the precision and control over measurement. Many scientists agree that epidemiological studies are superior to clinical investigations because they are larger and therefore more generalizable. Adding to their generalizability is the fact that epidemiological studies are conducted using community-based rather than patient-based samples, the latter often considered biased. Furthermore, epidemiological studies are typically prospective in nature, resulting in data that can be used to assess the direction of the association and reverse causality. Although the external validity of findings from epidemiologic studies may be stronger, the precision of measurement is often weaker. Epidemiological studies survey large populations, often using self-report questionnaires, whereas clinical studies may employ more specific and time-consuming measurements, such as structured interviews, on a much smaller sample of patients. For example, to explore the relationship between depression and dementia, a measure of depressive symptomatology might be administered in a community-based sample, followed five years later by assessment of dementia. Given the generally large size of an epidemiological study, there is likely sufficient power to find a statistically significant difference if one exists. These findings would be generalizable to the larger population of community-dwelling adults. On the other hand, clinical studies typically include a structured interview with a psychiatrist to diagnose depression and follow patients through a clinic rather than as part of the research study. Findings from clinical prospective studies are very informative, but not generalizable beyond the clinical sample of patients with the same disease/disorder. There are also often differences in measurement of depression between the two study designs; clinical studies often examine major depressive disorder, assessed by a psychiatrist, while epidemiologic studies measure depressive symptomatology, assessed via questionnaire. Thus, it is difficult to directly compare results from clinical and epidemiologic studies. Nonetheless, regardless of the differences between these two types of investigations, both have contributed significantly to this body of literature and are explored in this chapter.
182
Dementia
To add to the ambiguity and the complexity involved in understanding the relationship between depression and dementia, differences based on specific subgroup characteristics such as sex, age, and education have been identified. In the following section we present an overview of the findings of the relationship between depression and dementia from epidemiologic and clinical studies with respect to demographic characteristics. Sex Differences Sex has been examined in a number of studies as a risk factor for depression in the context of dementia as well as the reverse causality, as a risk factor for dementia in the context of depression. In general, women appear to be at higher risk of concurrent depression and dementia (Lyketsos et al. 1996; Migliorelli et al. 1995). Lyketsos and colleagues (1996) found that demented women were nearly three times as likely to develop a major depressive episode than were demented men. A large community-based study in France explored predictors of the progression of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to dementia and found significant effects for sub-clinical depression in women, but not in men (Artero et al. 2008). In contrast, epidemiologic studies examining whether depression is a risk factor for dementia generally find that the risk of developing dementia subsequent to depression is stronger in men compared to women. A large community-based study found that men with high depressive symptomatology were more than three times as likely as men with low depressive symptomatology to develop incident dementia over an eightyear follow-up period, and four times as likely to develop AD (Fuhrer, Dufouil, and Dartigues 2003). The incidence of dementia and AD did not vary according to depression among women. Within the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging, the prospective relationship between premorbid symptoms of depression and clinical dementia and AD was examined with similar results (Dal Forno 2005). The risk of dementia and AD was significantly higher with increasing depression symptoms for men but not for women. Additional support comes from a prospective study of cognitive decline in elderly hypertensives in the United Kingdom (Cervilla et al. 2000) that found that sex modified the effect of depression on cognitive outcomes with poorer results in depressed men than depressed women, and from a Chinese community-based study that demonstrated a stronger correlation between levels of depression and dementia in men than in women (Chen et al. 2009). In contrast, a clinical study failed to reveal significant effects of sex on the rate of incident dementia over a 29-year
Depression and Dementia
183
follow-up among patients hospitalized for depressive disorder (Kessing and Andersen 2004). Overall, the risk of developing dementia appears to be stronger in depressed men compared to depressed women (Henderson et al. 1997; Geerlings et al. 2000). Although it is generally believed that rates of depression are higher in women than men, previous work on sex-differences in self-reported symptoms of depression have shown that women selfreport more mild symptoms while men report more severe symptoms (Newmann 1984). These findings could help to explain why there is a stronger risk of dementia among depressed men compared to depressed women. Age Differences There is little research that explores age differences among adults in the relationship between depression and dementia. However, Boland (2000) summarized published findings that together make a case for pathological brain changes underlying depression in the context of dementia among older but not younger adults. Boland argues that through various mechanisms, such as white-matter changes, the structural deficits found on older but not younger patients may be important in understanding how late-life depression is related to dementia. Further, in a cross-sectional community-based study, Chen and colleagues reported that in both a Chinese and British sample the relationship between syndrome levels of depression and dementia were stronger in older (85+ years old) than younger (65–75 years old) participants (Chen et al. 2009). These findings are far from consistent and most previous theories explaining age differences in the association between depression and dementia have failed to find support from future studies. For example, as Boland (2000) reports in a summary of the research on depression in AD and other dementias, early studies found that cerebral atrophy played a role in the pathogenesis of late-life depression, while other studies have not supported this relationship (Palsson, Aevarsson, and Skoog 1999). The exception is that of vascular depression where the evidence points to the importance of vascular dementia in late-life depression (Copeland et al. 2003). Education Differences There is an abundance of literature that reports on the modifying influence of education on depression and dementia as individual constructs. Overwhelmingly, the evidence indicates that lower education is associated with greater risk of cognitive decline including dementia and AD
184
Dementia
(Ganguli et al. 2000; Evans et al. 1997). Similarly, lower rates of education are associated with increases in the prevalence of depression (Miech and Shanahan 2000). However, there is a dearth of published literature that explores the impact of education on the relationship between depression and risk of dementia. In the two related studies reported by Geerlings and colleagues (Geerlings, Schoevers, et al. 2000), education was found to modify the relationship between depression and incident dementia. Independent samples of older adults from the Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL) and the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam (LASA) were included in these prospective community-based studies. The results of both studies revealed that depression increased the risk of incident cognitive decline and AD over a three- to four-year follow-up period, but only among participants with higher levels of education (greater than eight years) (Geerlings, Schoevers, et al. 2000; Geerlings, Schmand, et al. 2000). These findings conflict with those that explore the independent relationship between education and depression and dementia where lower education was found to increase the rate of both conditions. However, findings for an increased risk of cognitive decline and AD only among adults with higher levels of education supports the notion of depression as a prodrome of dementia by suggesting that depression may be an early expression of AD prior to the presentation of cognitive symptoms. RISK FACTORS FOR DEPRESSION IN DEMENTIA Given that the diagnosis of depression in patients with dementia is so challenging and the consequences of comorbid depression and dementia so great, understanding the risk factors associated with developing depression in persons with dementia is important for practicing clinicians. In addition to demographic characteristics, which have been previously discussed, several risk factors for depression in dementia should be considered. The most consistent of these risk factors are family history of depression, personal history of depression, and early age of dementia onset. These risk factors are briefly described below. Perhaps the most widely studied and most consistent risk factors for depression in dementia are family and personal history of depression or mood disorders (Migliorelli et al. 1995; Strauss and Ogrocki 1996; Butt and Strauss 2001; Harwood et al. 1999). Two similar studies of patients with AD compared family histories of depression among patients with and without depression occurring in the context of AD (Strauss and Ogrocki 1996; Pearlson et al. 1990). They found that significantly more families of depressed dementia patients had first-degree relatives with a history of
Depression and Dementia
185
depression than did families of the nondepressed patients. Harwood and colleagues examined personal history of depression in relation to depression in the context of dementia among 243 AD outpatients (Harwood et al. 1999). They found that AD patients who were currently depressed were significantly more likely to have a history of depression compared to AD patients who were not depressed, even after adjustment for demographic factors and level of cognitive impairment. Despite the abundance of evidence for an association between family and personal history of depression and depression in dementia, the association is rather complex. Butt and Strauss (2001) examined family and personal history of depression separately and simultaneously in 161 patients with AD. When family and personal history were examined separately, both were associated with an increased risk for depression in dementia. When examined simultaneously, neither was significantly associated with depression, suggesting that these factors may represent a similar construct. In addition, a number of studies have found no association between family and personal history of depression and depression in the setting of dementia (Lyketsos et al. 1996; Migliorelli et al. 1995), or that the association was limited to subgroups of the population (Lyketsos et al. 1996). For example, Lyketsos and colleagues (1996) found that a family history of mood disorders was associated with an increased risk for developing a major depressive episode in the context of AD, but only in women. Conflicting findings suggest that this relationship may warrant further investigation. Also of note, a majority of the studies of family and personal history have been in patients with AD, thus examination of this relationship in other dementias is lacking, but important to explore in order to more fully understanding the association. Age of onset of dementia also may be associated with the risk of developing concurrent depression. Zubenko and colleagues (2003) conducted structured interviews of 243 patients with AD to assess clinical features of major depressive disorder. They found that the age of onset of AD was significantly younger in depressed as compared to nondepressed patients. In contrast, Lyketsos found no relationship between age of onset of AD and major depression (Lyketsos et al. 1996), suggesting that this potential modifier of association requires further examination. DEPRESSION AS A RISK FACTOR OR PRODROME OF DEMENTIA The frequent coexistence of depression and dementia has sparked interest in understanding the complex association between the two conditions (Alexopoulos et al. 1993; Rovner et al. 1989). It is unclear whether
186
Dementia
depression is a risk factor for dementia or whether depressive symptoms are an early sign of dementia pathology. Because dementia is a progressive disease with a long prodromal phase, there is uncertainty regarding causality in studies that measure depression close to the time dementia is diagnosed. Often, a five-year interval is used as a benchmark in longitudinal studies examining risk factors for dementia. Although the prodromal phase of dementia may be much longer than five years, risk factors measured at least five years prior to a dementia diagnosis are considered risk factors rather than symptoms of prodromal disease. In epidemiologic studies, depression has been reported to be associated with an increased risk for dementia up to 17 years later (Saczynski et al. 2010; Jorm 2001). These studies with longitudinal follow-up provide support for depression as a risk factor rather than a sign of prodromal disease. Other study designs support depression as an early symptom of dementia, including studies that examine the role of depression in patients who are likely to develop dementia, and studies that examine the concurrent and longitudinal association between depression and dementia. Both study designs are described below. MCI is associated with a high rate of conversion to dementia; conversion rates of nearly 100% over 10 years have been reported, thus MCI has been described as a precursor to dementia (Morris et al. 2001; Bennett et al. 2002). Several studies have examined whether the presence of depression in patients with MCI is associated with the development of dementia. The hypothesis tested in these studies is that depression may increase the risk of conversion from MCI to dementia or may decrease the time it takes to convert from MCI to dementia. Since some have suggested that all patients with MCI will eventually convert to dementia (Morris et al. 2001), this design examines whether or not depression is a sign of prodromal dementia. A recent prospective cohort study found that depressed persons with MCI were two and a half times as likely to develop dementia of the Alzheimer ’s type over a three-year follow-up period compared to nondepressed persons (Modrego and Ferrandez 2004). Depressed subjects also developed dementia earlier than nondepressed subjects. A smaller cohort study found similar results: depressed participants were significantly more likely to develop dementia over three years; however, rates of cognitive decline did not differ in depressed and nondepressed subjects (Copeland et al. 2003). A cohort study of nearly 1,000 subjects examined depression prior to dementia diagnosis (as a risk factor for dementia) as well as following the onset of dementia (as an early manifestation of the disease) (Chen et al. 1999). The authors found that depression assessed prior to the diagnosis of dementia was not associated with dementia risk; however, depression
Depression and Dementia
187
was more likely to develop in patients who had been diagnosed with dementia. That is, the longitudinal association between depression and dementia was not significant but the concurrent relationship was significant, suggesting that depression is a consequence or marker of depression rather than a risk factor for the disease. Similarly, a study of depression and dementia conducted in China and the United Kingdom found a strong cross-sectional association between depression and dementia but the association was attenuated when examined longitudinally over twoand four-year follow-up periods (Chen et al. 2009). Findings from studies such as these support the theory of depression as a symptom of prodromal dementia rather than as a risk factor for dementia. Overall, causality is difficult to resolve and many studies on the association between late-life depression and dementia simply control for prodromal dementia in either their study design or analysis. The methods by which reverse causality is examined varies among previously published studies. A number of studies have excluded subjects with MCI, based on global cognitive test scores such as the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE), to account for depression as a prodromal sign of dementia. Other studies address causality through statistical modeling (e.g., controlling for baseline cognitive performance). Optimally, more study designs such as the one employed by Chen and colleagues (1999) that measure depression many years before dementia onset and compare these results to depression measured concurrently or after dementia diagnosis are needed to further examine whether dementia is a risk factor or early symptom of dementia. DEPRESSION AS A RISK FACTOR FOR AD AS COMPARED TO VASCULAR DEMENTIA While there are a number of studies on the association between depression and dementia, little work has been done to examine whether depression is a risk factor for certain dementia subtypes and not others. Although both neurodegenerative and vascular factors have been found to be involved in AD and vascular dementia, and it is often difficult to identify “pure” AD or vascular dementia, understanding the association between depression and subtypes of dementia may provide insight into the mechanisms behind the relationship. A large majority of the studies on depression and the risk of dementia examine dementia overall and do not look at cases split out into specific subtypes. Despite the importance of the implications for understanding this association at the level of dementia subtype, few studies have utilized a designed that would allow for these questions to be systematically examined. Assessment of dementia subtype
188
Dementia
is difficult and it requires a large sample size to have sufficient statistical power to examine differences by subtype. The few studies that do examine subtypes typically examine AD, since it is the most common subtype of dementia. A majority of these studies find that the strength of the association between depression and overall dementia and depression and AD is similar or that the results are slightly stronger for AD (Saczynski et al. 2010; Fuhrer, Dufouil, and Dartigues 2003; Dal Forno et al. 2005; Geerlings et al. 2008). Several studies, however, have compared the strength of the association between depression and the major subtypes of dementia: AD and vascular dementia. Among men in the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study, depression was associated with a two and half times greater likelihood of developing any dementia over a six-year follow-up period (Irie et al. 2008). When dementia subtypes were examined separately, depressed men were three times as likely to develop pure or mixed AD and only two times as likely to develop vascular dementia. A recent postmortem study found increased AD neuropathologic findings among 10 subjects who were depressed but lacked clinical symptoms of depression (Sweet et al. 2004). In contrast, a smaller clinical sample of 228 outpatients found a higher rate of depression among patients with vascular dementia compared to those with AD (Brodaty and Luscombe 1996). In addition, associations between depression and white-matter lesion pathology, associated with vascular disease, have also been reported (de Groot et al. 2000). The underlying hypothesis for vascular disease, however, remains unclear. In fact, a recent report on 2,220 participants in the Cardiovascular Health Study found that depression increased the risk for MCI over a six-year follow-up period independent of underlying vascular pathology, suggesting that the link between depression and AD may be stronger than the link between depression and vascular dementia. Clearly, much more work is needed to fully understand whether depression is associated with a higher risk of specific subtypes of dementia compared to others. With the growing number of older adults with vascular disease and the increasing number of cases of mixed and vascular dementias, understanding the role that depression plays in subtypes will become increasingly important. POTENTIAL MECHANISMS UNDERLYING THE ASSOCIATION BETWEEN DEPRESSION AND DEMENTIA In addition to the complexity of the relationship between depression and dementia, and the risk factors that may influence this comorbid association,
Depression and Dementia
189
it is important to examine the mechanisms through which depression may impact dementia. To better understand the underlying pathology, studies have explored vascular changes, chronic inflammation, genetic, and lifestyle factors as possible mechanisms through which depression may impact dementia. Each of these potential mechanisms is described below. Vascular Changes One potential mechanism that has been explored is the presence of vascular changes in depression. Depression may be associated with vascular factors as described by the “vascular depression” hypothesis, which postulates that vascular pathology contributes to the pathogenesis of depression in late life (Alexopoulos et al. 1997). Indeed, cerebrovascular disease, observed in neuroimaging studies and controlled for white-matter lesion severity (Steffens et al. 1999), and increased arterial stiffness, assessed using carotid artery distensibility and pulse wave velocity (Tiemeier, va Tuijl, et al. 2002), have been observed in patients with depressive symptoms in large epidemiologic studies. In addition, depression has been associated with changes in cerebral haemodynamics, including reduced blood flow velocity and reduced vasomotor reactivity (Dotson et al. 2009; Tiemeier, Bakker, et al. 2002). Chronic Inflammation Chronic inflammation is now recognized as central to the pathogenesis of major depression, with chronic inflammatory changes as a common feature of depression (Zunszain et al. in press). Since inflammation is also implicated in the pathogenesis of dementia, it follows that the inflammatory changes seen in depression may also predispose depressed patients to the neurodegenerative changes that hallmark dementias such as AD. These chronic inflammatory changes, which occur in depression, potentially as a result of stress, may contribute to the development of dementia and AD (Leonard 2007). In particular, interleukin-6, C-reactive protein, and tumor necrosis factor (TNF) alpha are increased in persons with depression and may also be associated with an increased risk for dementia (Miller et al. 2002; Engelhart et al. 2004). Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), a neurotrophin that may be involved in synaptic plasticity, is decreased in depression and low
190
Dementia
levels of BDNF are also associated with dementia, particularly AD (Zuccato and Cattaneo 2009; Tsai 2003). While BDNF is found throughout the brain, it is particularly abundant in the hippocampus, a structure that has been found to be smaller in depressed patients compared to normal controls, as well as in persons with AD or vascular dementia (Bremner et al. 2000; Sheline, Gado, and Kraemer 2003; van de Pol et al. 2006; Barber et al. 2000). In addition, a recent meta-analysis showed that serum BDNF levels increased following pharmacologic treatment for depression (Sen, Duman, and Sanacora 2008). Genetic Factors There may also be genetic links between depression and dementia. Apolipoprotein ε is a major risk factor for Alzheimer ’s disease and leads to accelerated deposition of amyloid (Cummings 2004). The association between Apoε4, depression, and dementia has been examined in a number of studies, but the findings are inconsistent. A number of studies report that the presence of one or more Apoε4 alleles is associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline and dementia (Devanand et al. 1996; Irie et al. 2008). Among 1,070 community dwelling participants living in North Manhattan, Devanand et al. (1996) found that participants who were depressed at baseline and had the Apoε4 allele were nearly five times more likely to develop incident dementia over a five-year follow-up period compared to participants without the Apoε4 allele. Similarly, a recent report examined the joint effect of Apoε4 and depression on the risk of dementia among 1,932 men who were participants in the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study (Irie et al. 2008). They found that there was a significant interaction between depression and Apoε4 status, such that men who were both depressed and had one or more Apoε4 alleles were more than seven times as likely to develop dementia than nondepressed men without Apoε4 alleles. Depressed men without an Apoε4 allele were only one and a half times as likely to develop dementia than the comparison group, suggesting that the joint effect of depression and Apoε4 was stronger than the effect of either risk factor alone. Further, the interaction between depression and Apoε4 was significant for all dementia and for pure and mixed AD, but not for vascular dementia, suggesting that Apoε4 may modify the association between depression and AD exclusively. Despite the evidence for a role of Apoε4 in the association between depression and dementia, other studies have found that the relationship does not vary according to Apoε4 status (Craig et al. 2005; Scarmeas et al. 2002). For instance, Scarmeas and colleagues (2002) found that although
Depression and Dementia
191
the presence of one or more ε4 alleles was associated with the risk of certain psychopathologic symptoms in patients with AD, such as delusions and hallucinations, ApoE status was not associated with symptoms of depression. Mixed findings suggest that more work is needed to fully understand whether Apoε4 modifies the association between depression and dementia. Since Apoε4 is a risk factor for AD specifically, future studies may focus on examining the role of Apoε4 in the relationship between depression and specific subtypes of dementia. Lifestyle Factors Several lifestyle factors associated with longstanding depression, such as diet, physical activity, and social engagement, may increase the risk for dementia. Population-based epidemiologic studies have shown that vitamin B12 deficiency and high levels of homocysteine are associated with depression in older adults and with an increased risk for dementia and AD (Tiemeier, Bakker, et al. 2002; Sachdev et al. 2005; Penninx, Guralnik, et al. 2000). A bidirectional association between physical activity and depression has been suggested, with some finding that physical activity declines as a result of depression (van Gool et al. 2003; Penninx, Deeg et al. 2000), and others suggesting that change in activity is a potential risk factor for depression (Lampinen and Heikkinen 2003; Lampinen, Heikkinen, and Ruoppila 2000). Social engagement may also be a mechanism by which depression and dementia are linked. Depressed persons may decrease their levels of social engagement, which is also associated with an increased risk for dementia (Saczynski et al. 2006). THE CONSEQUENCES OF DEPRESSION IN DEMENTIA Regardless of the nature through which depression impacts dementia, or the risk factors that contribute to the strength of this complex relationship, there are far-reaching consequences for the patient, their caregivers, and society overall. Among patients with dementia, physical, behavioral, and cognitive outcomes have been shown to differ according to whether or not the patient also has concurrent depression. Geerlings and colleagues (1999) found that among 216 noninstitutionalized persons with dementia, presence of depression was associated with higher rates of mortality. In fact, the detrimental effect of depression was strong enough to negate the protective effect of high education on mortality risk among demented patients in this cohort. That is, high education was associated with lower risk only among nondepressed participants. Further, the
192
Dementia
role of depression on mortality among patients with dementia appears to be independent of dementia severity. In a small sample of 73 patients with mild/moderate dementia (median MMSE = 19), depressed patients had significantly higher rates of 12-month mortality compared to nondepressed patients. In a sample of 178 patients with moderate/severe dementia enrolled through a psychiatric service, presence of depression was associated with significantly higher rates of cumulative three-year mortality (Burns et al. 1991). By contrast, several studies have failed to find an association between depression and rate of mortality in dementia, or found a significant association in only selected subgroups (Moritz et al. 1997; Jagger, Clarke, and Stone 1995). In a small clinical study of 22 patients with AD, Jagger and colleagues found that the presence of depression was not associated with mortality (Jagger, Clarke, and Stone 1995). Moritz et al. (1997) examined psychiatric predictors of mortality over a 2.5-year follow-up period among 936 men and women diagnosed with AD. They found that depression was associated with poorer survival only in men; there was no association between depression and survival in women. In addition to an effect on prognosis, depression status may also influence the physical and cognitive trajectories of demented patients. Surprisingly, results from studies that explored the role of depression on the rate of cognitive impairment among patients with depression show that depressed patients have similar rates of cognitive decline and disease progression compared to nondepressed patients. In a small study comparing 10 demented patients who were concurrently depressed with 10 nondepressed demented patients, Lopez and colleagues (1990) found a comparable rate of cognitive decline in the two groups over a one-year follow-up period (Ferri et al. 2005). In a larger study of 179 patients with mild/ moderate AD, Lopez et al. examined whether or not progression of cognitive impairment, defined as time to MMSE score of ≤9, varied according to the presence of depression (Becker et al. 2009). They found that the rate of cognitive decline over approximately four years did not vary by the presence of depression status. Similarly, a study of 116 patients with probable AD found no differences in decline in cognitive function, based on MMSE scores, among patients with major depression, mild depression and those without depression (Starkstein et al. 1997). While the studies described above found no differences in rates of cognitive decline among demented patients based on presence or absence of depression, Janzing found that patients with mild dementia who reported experiencing depressive symptoms had slower cognitive decline compared to patients who did not report depressive symptoms (Janzing, Naarding,
Depression and Dementia
193
and Eling 2005). These findings are in the same general direction as previous work, but are the first, to our knowledge, to report slower cognitive decline in depressed patients as opposed to similar rates of decline. The authors hypothesize that the report of depressive symptoms by patients with mild dementia may reflect relative intact cognitive functions and awareness. Overall, the findings reported above suggest that comorbid depression in the setting of dementia increases the disease burden for the patient by increasing morbidity and mortality. In addition, the physical and psychological burden on the caregiver is also increased when depression co-occurs with dementia. It is not difficult to speculate that comorbid depression and dementia also increase healthcare utilization and overall healthcare costs. Indeed, among patients with dementia, depression has also been found to be associated with higher healthcare utilization and overall healthcare costs. In a study of more than 7,000 veterans aged 60 and older with diagnoses of major depression, dementia, or both, patients with both depression and dementia were higher users of inpatient services compared to patients with one condition alone (Kales et al. 1999). These findings highlight the financial burden this high-risk patient population exerts on the healthcare system, and the importance of understanding the course and treatment of dementia with co-existing depression. In addition to consequences for the patient, depression in the setting of dementia also has major implications for the caregiver of the demented patient. Among caregivers of 100 patients with AD who lived at home, patient depression was one of the strongest predictors of burden and distress in caregivers (Donaldson, Tarrier, and Burns 1998). Similarly, Brodaty (1998) found that caregivers of depressed patients enrolled through a memory clinic were more likely to be depressed themselves than were caregivers to patients who were not depressed. Although few studies have examined depression in demented patients as a factor in caregiver outcomes, these results suggest that depression status of patients has consequences and implications beyond the patient. TREATING DEPRESSION IN DEMENTIA Treatment of psychiatric and behavioral disturbances, including depression, has been suggested to be an essential part of the complete treatment of dementia (Doody et al. 2001; American Psychiatric Association 1997; Rabins, Lyketsos, and Steele 1999). There are a number of treatment options including pharmacologic treatment, behavioral treatment aimed at the patient, and caregiver interventions. However, there is a lack of evidence
194
Dementia
to support the use of treatments and published studies employ varying definitions for depression and have been conducted in small, often select, samples with a wide range of outcome measures. Due to these factors, it is difficult for the practicing clinician to choose an evidence-based treatment. The limited literature on the treatment of depression in dementia is briefly summarized below. Several classes of antidepressants, including selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI), tricyclic antidepressants (TCA), and monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOI), have been found to have some benefits in patients with dementia (Trivedi et al. 2006; Starkstein, Mizrahi, and Power 2008; Lyketsos and Olin 2002; Katona, Hunter, and Bray 1998). The most consistent and promising findings have been for SSRIs (Lyketsos and Olin 2002). However, a recent double-blind, placebo-control trial, “Depression in Dementia—2,” concluded that SSRIs may have limited effectiveness in treating depression in Alzheimer ’s disease (Rosenberg et al. 2010; Weintraub et al. 2010). The efficacy of antidepressant treatment in dementia may also vary according to severity of depression. A number of studies showing that antidepressants are less effective in demented patients with mild depression compared to those with moderate to severe depression (Magai et al. 2000; Nyth and Gottfries 1990; Petracca, Chemerinski, and Starkstein 2001). Further, with the exception of the SSRI sertraline (Royall et al. 2009), the treatment of depression in dementia is largely limited to Alzheimer ’s disease, with a lack of data in other dementias such as vascular dementia or Lewy bodies. Behavioral interventions with both the demented patient and the caregiver, although less well studied, have also been suggested as a treatment for depression in dementia (Teri 1994). A large randomized trial of 153 community-dwelling patients with AD and their caregivers randomized dyads to a combined exercise and caregiver training program (intervention group) or the control group (Teri et al. 2003). Patients who were in the intervention group received 12 one-hour, in-home training sessions directed at both the caregiver and patient. Caregivers were given specific instructions on how to respond to behavior problems in patients, were educated about dementia and AD, and were taught how to modulate their responses to patient problems. The exercise component of the study, in which the patients participated, involved aerobics, strength training, balance, and flexibility. After three months, participants in the intervention group had lower rates of depression and improved physical function. After two years, participants who were depressed at baseline maintained significant improvements in scores on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale.
Depression and Dementia
195
A smaller study examining the effect of nonpharmacologic treatment of depression in patients with dementia in patient/caregiver dyads focused on altering the aversive events and interactions that maintain patient depression, increasing pleasant events and interactions, maximizing cognitive abilities, and teaching caregivers strategies for behavior change and effective problem solving (Teri 1994; Teri et al. 1997). Among 72 dyads, they found that rates of depression in the patients and caregivers in the intervention group were lower six months following the completion of the intervention (Teri et al. 1997). Overall, the most appropriate and safe way to treat depression in dementia is not well understood. Clearly, behavioral interventions are, in general, safer than pharmacologic treatments, and may be preferable, particularly in patients with mild depression. Behavioral interventions may also result in psychological benefits for the caregiver. The published literature on behavioral interventions, however, is particularly underdeveloped, making it difficult for a practitioner to recommend this course of action to a patient. In addition, to our knowledge, combined pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic interventions have not been examined in demented persons suffering from depression. SUMMARY AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS With the aging of the U.S. population and the increasing number of people surviving to develop dementia and corresponding comorbidities such as depression, the co-occurrence of depression and dementia is an issue of major public health concern. Depression in the setting of dementia results in poorer outcomes in patients, higher stress in caregivers, and greater burden on the healthcare system. Diagnosis is difficult and results for evidence based treatments are mixed. The development of assessment tools that are sensitive and specific to identifying depression in patients with dementia, such as the NIMH-dAD, is an important step for future research. Validation of these assessment tools in representative samples is also important. Further, longitudinal studies of depression in incident cases of dementia will help unravel the question of temporality and reverse causality, still an unclear issue. Longitudinal studies will also provide insight into mechanisms, as many hypothesized pathways have not been examined in cohorts designed to compare demented patients with and without depression. Educating clinicians to identify signs of depression in demented patients and dementia in depressed patients is vitally important for early identification and proper treatment.
196
Dementia
REFERENCES Alexopoulos, G. S., R. C. Abrams, R. C. Young, and C. A. Shamoian. 1988. Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. Biol Psychiatry 23: 271–284. Alexopoulos, G. S., B. S. Meyers, R. C. Young, S. Campbell, D. Silbersweig, and M. Charlson. 1997. “Vascular depression” hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54: 915–922. Alexopoulos, G. S., B. S. Meyers, R. C. Young, S. Mattis, and T. Kakuma. 1993. The course of geriatric depression with “reversible dementia”: A controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 150: 1693–1699. American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. 1997. Practice guidelines for the treatment of dementia and Alzheimer ’s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry 154: 1–39. Amore, M., P. Tagariello, C. Laterza, and E. M. Savoia. 2007. Subtypes of depression in dementia. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 44 (Suppl. 1): 23–33. Artero, S., M. L. Ancelin, F. Portet, et al. 2008. Risk profiles for mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia are gender specific. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 79: 979–984. Backman, L., S. Jones, A. K. Berger, E. J. Laukka, and B. J. Small. 2005. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer ’s disease: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 19: 520–531. Barber, R., C. Ballard, I. G. McKeith, A. Gholkar, and J. T. O’Brien. 2000. MRI volumetric study of dementia with Lewy bodies: A comparison with AD and vascular dementia. Neurology 54: 1304–1309. Bazin, N., P. Perruchet, M. De Bonis, and A. Feline. 1994. The dissociation of explicit and implicit memory in depressed patients. Psychol Med 24: 239–245. Becker, J. T., Y. F. Chang, O. L. Lopez, et al. 2009. Depressed mood is not a risk factor for incident dementia in a community-based cohort. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17: 653–663. Bennett, D. A., R. S. Wilson, J. A. Schneider, et al. 2002. Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in older persons. Neurology 59: 198–205. Berger, A. K., L. Fratiglioni, Y. Forsell, B. Winblad, and L. Backman. 1999. The occurrence of depressive symptoms in the preclinical phase of AD: A population-based study. Neurology 53: 1998–2002. Boland, R. J. 2000. Depression in Alzheimer ’s disease and other dementias. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2: 427–433. Bremner, J. D., M. Narayan, E. R. Anderson, L. H. Staib, H. L. Miller, and D. S. Charney. 2000. Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 157: 115–118. Brodaty, H., and G. Luscombe. 1996. Depression in persons with dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 8: 609–622. Brodaty, H., and G. Luscombe. 1998. Psychological morbidity in caregivers is associated with depression in patients with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 12: 62–70.
Depression and Dementia
197
Buckner, R. L., A. Z. Snyder, B. J. Shannon, et al. 2005. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer ’s disease: Evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J. Neurosci 25: 7709–7717. Burns, A., G. Lewis, R. Jacoby, and R. Levy. 1991. Factors affecting survival in Alzheimer ’s disease. Psychol Med 21: 363–370. Butt, Z. A., and M. E. Strauss. 2001. Relationship of family and personal history to the occurrence of depression in persons with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 9: 249–254. Butters, M. A., W. E. Klunk, C. A. Mathis, et al. 2008. Imaging Alzheimer pathology in late-life depression with PET and Pittsburgh Compound-B. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 22: 261–268. Butters, M. A., E. M. Whyte, R. D. Nebes, et al. 2004. The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61: 587–595. Cervilla, J. A., M. Prince, S. Joels, and A. Mann. 2000. Does depression predict cognitive outcome 9 to 12 years later? Evidence from a prospective study of elderly hypertensives. Psychol Med 30: 1017–1023. Chemerinski, E., G. Petracca, L. Sabe, J. Kremer, and S. E. Starkstein. 2001. The specificity of depressive symptoms in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 158: 68–72. Chen, P., M. Ganguli, B. H. Mulsant, and S. T. DeKosky. 1999. The temporal relationship between depressive symptoms and dementia: A community-based prospective study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56: 261–266. Chen, R., Z. Hu, L. Wei, X. Qin, and J. R. Copeland. 2009. Is the relationship between syndromes of depression and dementia temporal? The MRC-ALPHA and Hefei-China studies. Psychol Med 39: 425–430. Christensen, H., K. Griffiths, A. Mackinnon, and P. Jacomb. 1997. A quantitative review of cognitive deficits in depression and Alzheimer-type dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 3: 631–651. Copeland, M. P., E. Daly, V. Hines, et al. 2003. Psychiatric symptomatology and prodromal Alzheimer ’s disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 17: 1–8. Craig, D., D. J. Hart, S. P. McIlroy, and A. P. Passmore. 2005. Association analysis of apolipoprotein E genotype and risk of depressive symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 19: 154–157. Crowe, S., and K. Hoogenraad. 1999. Differentiation of dementia of the Alzheimer ’s type from depression with cognitive impairment on the basis of a cortical versus subcortical pattern of cognitive deficit. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 15: 9–19. Cummings, J. L. 2004. Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 351: 56–67. Dal Forno, G., M. T. Palermo, J. E. Donohue, H. Karagiozis, A. B. Zonderman, and C. H. Kawas. 2005. Depressive symptoms, sex, and risk for Alzheimer ’s disease. Ann Neurol 57: 381–387. Dannenbaum, S., S. Parkinson, and V. Inman. 1988. Short-term forgetting: Comparisons between patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type, depressed, and normal elderly. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5: 213–233.
198
Dementia
de Groot, J. C., F.-E. de Leeuw, M. Oudkerk, A. Hofman, J. Jolles, and M. M. B. Breteler. 2000. Cerebral white matter lesions and depressive symptoms in elderly adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 1071–1076. Devanand, D. P., M. Sano, M.-X. Tang, et al. 1996. Depressed mood and the incidence of Alzheimer ’s disease in the elderly living in the community. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53: 175–182. Donaldson, C., N. Tarrier, and A. Burns. 1998. Determinants of carer stress in Alzheimer ’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 13: 248–256. Doody, R. S., J. C. Stevens, C. Beck, et al. 2001. Practice parameter: Management of dementia (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 56: 1154–1166. Dotson, V. M., L. Beason-Held, M. A. Kraut, and S. M. Resnick. 2009. Longitudinal study of chronic depressive symptoms and regional cerebral blood flow in older men and women. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 24: 809–819. Dufouil, C., R. Fuhrer, J. F. Dartigues, and A. Alperovitch. 1996. Longitudinal analysis of the association between depressive symptomatology and cognitive deterioration. Am J Epidemiol 144: 634–641. Elderkin-Thompson, V., A. Kumar, J. Mintz, K. Boone, E. Bahng, and H. Lavretsky. 2004. Executive dysfunction and visuospatial ability among depressed elders in a community setting. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19: 597–611. Engelhart, M. J., M. I. Geerlings, J. Meijer, et al. 2004. Inflammatory proteins in plasma and the risk of dementia: The Rotterdam Study. Arch Neurol 61: 668–672. Evans, D. A., L. E. Hebert, L. A. Beckett, et al. 1997. Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older persons. Arch Neurol 54: 1399–1405. Ferri, C. P., M. Prince, C. Brayne, et al. 2005. Global prevalence of dementia: A Delphi consensus study. Lancet 366: 2112–2117. Fuhrer, R., C. Dufouil, and J. F. Dartigues. 2003. Exploring sex differences in the relationship between depressive symptoms and dementia incidence: Prospective results from the PAQUID Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 51: 1055–1063. Ganguli, M., H. H. Dodge, P. Chen, S. Belle, and S. T. DeKosky. 2000. Ten-year incidence of dementia in a rural elderly US community population: The MoVIES Project. Neurology 54: 1109–1116. Geerlings, M. I., T. den Heijer, P. J. Koudstaal, A. Hofman, and M. M. B. Breteler. 2008. History of depression, depressive symptoms, and medial temporal lobe atrophy and the risk of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 70: 1258–1264. Geerlings, M. I., B. Schmand, A. W. Braam, C. Jonker, L. M. Bouter, and W. van Tilburg. 2000. Depressive symptoms and risk of Alzheimer ’s disease in more highly educated older people. J Am Geriatr Soc 48: 1092–1097. Geerlings, M. I., B. Schmand, C. Jonker, J. Lindeboom, and L. M. Bouter. 1999. Education and incident Alzheimer ’s disease: A biased association due to
Depression and Dementia
199
selective attrition and use of a two-step diagnostic procedure? Int J Epidemiol 28: 492–497. Geerlings, M. I., R. A. Schoevers, A. T. Beekman, et al. 2000. Depression and risk of cognitive decline and Alzheimer ’s disease. Results of two prospective community-based studies in The Netherlands. Br J Psychiatry 176: 568–575. Gray, J. W., A. I. Rattan, and R. S. Dean. 1986. Differential diagnosis of dementia and depression in the elderly using neuropsychological methods. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1: 341–349. Green, R. C., L. A. Cupples, A. Kurz, et al. 2003. Depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer disease: The MIRAGE Study. Arch Neurol 60: 753–759. Hart, R. P., J. A. Kwentus, R. M. Hamer, and J. R. Taylor. 1987. Selective reminding procedure in depression and dementia. Psychol Aging 2: 111–115. Harwood, D. G., W. W. Barker, R. L. Ownby, and R. Duara. 1999. Association between premorbid history of depression and current depression in Alzheimer ’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 12: 72–75. Hedlund, J., and B. Viewig. 1979. The Hamilton rating scale for depression: A comprehensive review. J Operat Psychiatry 10: 149–165. Henderson, A. S., A. E. Korten, P. A. Jacomb, et al. 1997. The course of depression in the elderly: A longitudinal community-based study in Australia. Psychol Med 27: 119–129. Herrmann, L. L., G. M. Goodwin, and K. P. Ebmeier. 2007. The cognitive neuropsychology of depression in the elderly. Psychol Med 37: 1693–1702. Hickie, I., E. Scott, K. Wilhelm, and H. Brodaty. 1997. Subcortical hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging in patients with severe depression—A longitudinal evaluation. Biol Psychiatry 42: 367–374. Iidaka, T., T. Nakajima, K. Kawamoto, et al. 1996. Signal hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging in elderly depressed patients. Eur Neurol 36: 293–299. Irie, F., K. H. Masaki, H. Petrovitch, et al. 2008. Apolipoprotein E ε4 allele genotype and the effect of depressive symptoms on the risk of dementia in men: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65: 906–912. Jagger, C., M. Clarke, and A. Stone. 1995. Predictors of survival with Alzheimer ’s disease: A community-based study. Psychol Med 25: 171–177. Janzing, J. G. E., P. Naarding, and P. Eling. 2005. Depressive symptoms predict slow cognitive decline in mild dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 20: 77–81. Jean, L., M. Simard, R. van Reekum, and D. E. Clarke. 2005. Differential cognitive impairment in subjects with geriatric depression who will develop Alzheimer ’s disease and other dementias: A retrospective study. Int Psychogeriatr 17: 289–301. Jorm, A. F. 2000. Is depression a risk factor for dementia or cognitive decline? A review. Gerontology 46: 219–227. Jorm, A. F. 2001. History of depression as a risk factor for dementia: An updated review. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 35: 776–781.
200
Dementia
Kales, H. C., F. C. Blow, L. A. Copeland, R. C. Bingham, E. E. Kammerer, and A. M. Mellow. 1999. Health care utilization by older patients with coexisting dementia and depression. Am J Psychiatry 156: 550–556. Katona, C., B. Hunter, and J. Bray. 1998. A double-blind comparison of the efficacy and safety of paroxetine and imipramine in the treatment of depression with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 13: 100–108. Kessing, L. V., and P. K. Andersen. 2004. Does the risk of developing dementia increase with the number of episodes in patients with depressive disorder and in patients with bipolar disorder? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75: 1662–1666. King. D., E. Caine, Y. Conwell, and C. Cox. 1990. The neuropsychology of depression in the elderly: A comparative study of normal aging and Alzheimer ’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry 3: 163–168. Korner, A., L. Lauritzen, K. Abelskov, et al. 2006. The Geriatric Depression Scale and the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. A validity study. Nord J Psychiatry 60: 360–364. La Rue, A., L. F. D’Elia, E. O. Clark, J. E. Spar, and L. F. Jarvik. 1986. Clinical tests of memory in dementia, depression, and healthy aging. Psychol Aging 1: 69–77. Lampinen, P., and E. Heikkinen. 2003. Reduced mobility and physical activity as predictors of depressive symptoms among community-dwelling older adults: An eight-year follow-up study. Aging Clin Exp Res 15: 205–211. Lampinen, P., R.-L. Heikkinen, and I. Ruoppila. 2000. Changes in intensity of physical exercise as predictors of depressive symptoms among older adults: An eight-year follow-up. Preventive Medicine 30: 371–380. Lee, H. B., and C. G. Lyketsos. 2003. Depression in Alzheimer ’s disease: Heterogeneity and related issues. Biological Psychiatry 54: 353–362. Leonard, B. E. 2007. Inflammation, depression and dementia: Are they connected? Neurochem Res 32: 1749–1756. Lockwood, K. A., G. S. Alexopoulos, T. Kakuma, and W. G. Van Gorp. 2000. Subtypes of cognitive impairment in depressed older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 8: 201–208. Looi, J. C. L., and P. S. Sachdev. 1999. Differentiation of vascular dementia from AD on neuropsychological tests. Neurology 53: 670–678. Lopez, O. L., F. Boller, J. T. Becker, M. Miller, and C. F. Reynolds. 1990. Alzheimer ’s disease and depression: Neuropsychological impairment and progression of the illness. Am J Psychiatry 147: 855–860. Lyketsos, C. G., and J. Olin. 2002. Depression in Alzheimer ’s disease: Overview and treatment. Biological Psychiatry 52: 243–252. Lyketsos, C. G., L. E. Tune, G. Pearlson, and C. Steele. 1996. Major depression in Alzheimer ’s disease. An interaction between gender and family history. Psychosomatics 37: 380–384. Mackenzie, T. B., W. N. Robiner, and D. S. Knopman. 1989. Differences between patient and family assessments of depression in Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 146: 1174–1178.
Depression and Dementia
201
Magai, C., G. Kennedy, C. I. Cohen, and D. Gomberg. 2000. A controlled clinical trial of sertraline in the treatment of depression in nursing home patients with late-stage Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 8: 66–74. Miech, R., and M. Shanahan. 2000. Socioeconomic status and depression over the life course. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 41: 162–167. Migliorelli, R., A. Teson, L. Sabe, M. Petracchi, R. Leiguarda, and S. E. Starkstein. 1995. Prevalence and correlates of dysthymia and major depression among patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 152: 37–44. Miller, G. E., C. A. Stetler, R. M. Carney, K. E. Freedland, and W. A. Banks. 2002. Clinical depression and inflammatory risk markers for coronary heart disease. American Journal of Cardiology 90: 1279–1283. Modrego, P. J., and J. Ferrandez. 2004. Depression in patients with mild cognitive impairment increases the risk of developing dementia of Alzheimer type: A prospective cohort study. Arch Neurol 61: 1290–1293. Moritz, D. J., P. J. Fox, F. A. Luscombe, and H. C. Kraemer. 1997. Neurological and psychiatric predictors of mortality in patients with Alzheimer disease in California. Arch Neurol 54: 878–885. Morris, J. C., M. Storandt, J. P. Miller, et al. 2001. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 58: 397–405. Moye, J., W. N. Robiner, and T. B. Mackenzie. 1993. Depression in Alzheimer patients: Discrepancies between patient and caregiver reports. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 7: 187–201. Mulsant, B. H., and M. Ganguli. 1999. Epidemiology and diagnosis of depression in late life. J Clin Psychiatry 60 (Suppl. 20): 9–15. Nebes, R. D., M. A. Butters, B. H. Mulsant, et al. 2000. Decreased working memory and processing speed mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Psychol Med 30: 679–691. Nelson, D. V., R. G. Harper, D. Kotik-Harper, and H. B. Kirby. 1993. Brief neuropsychologic differentiation of demented versus depressed elderly inpatients. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 15: 409–416. Newmann, J. P. 1984. Sex differences in symptoms of depression: Clinical disorder or normal distress? J Health Soc Behav 25: 136–159. Nyth, A. L., and C. G. Gottfries. 1990. The clinical efficacy of citalopram in treatment of emotional disturbances in dementia disorders. A Nordic multicentre study. Br J Psychiatry 157: 894–901. O’Brien, H. J. T., D. Ames, and I. Schweitzer. 1996. White matter changes in depression and Alzheimer ’s disease: A review of magnetic resonance imaging studies. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 11: 681–694. O’Carroll, R. E., S. M. Curran, M. Ross, et al. 1994. The differentiation of major depression from dementia of the Alzheimer type using within-subject neuropsychological discrepancy analysis. Br J Clin Psychol 33 (pt. 1): 23–32. Olin, J. T., I. R. Katz, B. S. Meyers, L. S. Schneider, and B. D. Lebowitz. 2002. Provisional diagnostic criteria for depression of Alzheimer disease: Rationale and background. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 10: 129–141.
202
Dementia
Olin, J. T., L. S. Schneider, I. R. Katz, et al. 2002. Provisional diagnostic criteria for depression of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 10: 125–128. Palsson, S., O. Aevarsson, and I. Skoog. 1999. Depression, cerebral atrophy, cognitive performance and incidence of dementia. Population study of 85-yearolds. Br J Psychiatry 174: 249–253. Palsson, S., B. Johansson, S. Berg, and I. Skoog. 2000. A population study on the influence of depression on neuropsychological functioning in 85-year-olds. Acta Psychiatr Scand 101: 185–193. Parekh, A. K., and M. B. Barton. 2010. The challenge of multiple comorbidity for the US health care system. JAMA 303: 1303–1304. Pearlson, G. D., C. A. Ross, W. D. Lohr, B. W. Rovner, G. A. Chase, and M. F. Folstein. 1990. Association between family history of affective disorder and the depressive syndrome of Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 147: 452–456. Penninx, B. W., D. J. Deeg, J. T. van Eijk, A. T. Beekman, and J. M. Guralnik. 2000. Changes in depression and physical decline in older adults: A longitudinal perspective. J Affect Disord 61: 1–12. Penninx, B. W., J. M. Guralnik, L. Ferrucci, L. P. Fried, R. H. Allen, and S. P. Stabler. 2000. Vitamin B(12) deficiency and depression in physically disabled older women: Epidemiologic evidence from the Women’s Health and Aging Study. Am J Psychiatry 157: 715–721. Petracca, G. M., E. Chemerinski, and S. E. Starkstein. 2001. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of fluoxetine in depressed patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr 13: 233–240. Rabins, P., C. Lyketsos, and C. Steele. 1999. Practical dementia care. New York: Oxford University Press. Rapp, M. A., K. Dahlman, M. Sano, H. T. Grossman, V. Haroutunian, and J. M. Gorman. 2005. Neuropsychological differences between late-onset and recurrent geriatric major depression. Am J Psychiatry 162: 691–698. Rapp, M. A., M. Schnaider-Beeri, H. T. Grossman, et al. 2006. Increased hippocampal plaques and tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease with a lifetime history of major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 161–167. Rapp, M. A., M. Schnaider-Beeri, D. P. Purohit, D. P. Perl, V. Haroutunian, and M. Sano. 2008. Increased neurofibrillary tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease with comorbid depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 16: 168–174. Rosenberg, P. B., L. T. Drye, B. K. Martin, et al. 2010. Sertraline for the treatment of depression in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 18: 136–145. Rovner, B. W., J. Broadhead, M. Spencer, K. Carson, M. F. Folstein. 1989. Depression and Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 146: 350–353. Royall, D. R., J. A. Cordes, G. Roman, et al. 2009. Sertraline improves executive function in patients with vascular cognitive impairment. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 21: 445–454. Roy-Byrne, P. P., H. Weingartner, L. M. Bierer, K. Thompson, and R. M. Post. 1986. Effortful and automatic cognitive processes in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43: 265–267.
Depression and Dementia
203
Sachdev, P. S., R. A. Parslow, O. Lux, et al. 2005. Relationship of homocysteine, folic acid and vitamin B12 with depression in a middle-aged community sample. Psychol Med 35: 529–538. Saczynski, J. S., A. Beiser, S. Seshadri, S. Auerbach, P. A. Wolf, and R. Au. 2010. Depressive symptoms and risk of dementia: The Framingham Heart Study. Neurology 75: 35–41. Saczynski, J. S., L. A. Pfeifer, K. Masaki, et al. 2006. The effect of social engagement on incident dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Am J Epidemiol 163: 433–440. Scarmeas, N., J. Brandt, M. Albert, et al. 2002. Association between the APOE genotype and psychopathologic symptoms in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 58: 1182–1188. Sen, S., R. Duman, and G. Sanacora. 2008. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor, depression, and antidepressant medications: Meta-analyses and implications. Biological Psychiatry 64: 527–532. Sheikh, J., and J. Yesavage. 1986. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist 5: 165–173. Sheline, Y. I., M. H. Gado, and H. C. Kraemer. 2003. Untreated depression and hippocampal volume loss. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1516–1518. Small, G. W. 2009. Differential diagnoses and assessment of depression in elderly patients. J Clin Psychiatry 70: e47. Smith G, S., E. Kramer, Y. Ma, et al. 2009. The functional neuroanatomy of geriatric depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24: 798–808. Starkstein, S. E., E. Chemerinski, L. Sabe, et al. 1997. Prospective longitudinal study of depression and anosognosia in Alzheimer ’s disease. Br J Psychiatry 171: 47–52. Starkstein, S. E., R. Mizrahi, and B. D. Power. 2008. Depression in Alzheimer ’s disease: Phenomenology, clinical correlates and treatment. International Review of Psychiatry 20: 382–388. Steffens, D. C., M. J. Helms, K. R. Krishnan, and G. L. Burke. 1999. Cerebrovascular disease and depression symptoms in the cardiovascular health study. Stroke 30: 2159–2166. Strauss, M. E., and P. K. Ogrocki. 1996. Confirmation of an association between family history of affective disorder and the depressive syndrome in Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 153: 1340–1342. Sweet, R. A., R. L. Hamilton, M. A. Butters, et al. 2004. Neuropathologic correlates of late-onset major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 2242–2250. Taylor, W. D., J. R. MacFall, M. E. Payne, et al. 2005. Greater MRI lesion volumes in elderly depressed subjects than in control subjects. Psychiatry Res 139: 1–7. Teng, E., J. M. Ringman, L. K. Ross, et al. 2008. Diagnosing depression in Alzheimer disease with the National Institute of Mental Health provisional criteria. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 16: 469–477. Teri, L., L. E. Gibbons, S. M. McCurry, et al. 2003. Exercise plus behavioral management in patients with Alzheimer disease: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 290: 2015–2022.
204
Dementia
Teri, L., R. G. Logsdon, J. Uomoto, and S. M. McCurry. 1997. Behavioral treatment of depression in dementia patients: A controlled clinical trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 52: P159–66. Teri, L., and A. W. Wagner. 1991. Assessment of depression in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease: Concordance among informants. Psychol Aging 6: 280–285. Teri, L. 1994. Behavioral treatment of depression in patients with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 8 (Suppl. 3): 66–74. Tiemeier, H., S. L. Bakker, A. Hofman, P. J. Koudstaal, and M. M. Breteler. 2002. Cerebral haemodynamics and depression in the elderly. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73: 34–39. Tiemeier, H., H. R. van Tuijl, A. Hofman, J. Meijer, A. J. Kiliaan, and M. M. B. Breteler. 2002. Vitamin B12, folate, and homocysteine in Depression: The Rotterdam Study. Am J Psychiatry 159: 2099–2101. Trivedi, M. H., A. J. Rush, S. R. Wisniewski, et al. 2006. Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR*D: Implications for clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry 163: 28–40. Tsai, S.-J. 2003. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: A bridge between major depression and Alzheimer ’s disease? Medical Hypotheses 61: 110–113. van de Pol, L. A., A. Hensel, F. Barkhof, H. J. Gertz, P. Scheltens, and W. M. van der Flier. 2006. Hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer disease: Age matters. Neurology 66: 236–238. van Gool, C. H., G. I. Kempen, B. W. Penninx, D. J. Deeg, A. T. Beekman, and J. T. van Eijk. 2003. Relationship between changes in depressive symptoms and unhealthy lifestyles in late middle aged and older persons: Results from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. Age Ageing 32: 81–87. Weintraub, D., P. B. Rosenberg, L. T. Drye, et al. 2010. Sertraline for the treatment of depression in Alzheimer disease: Week-24 outcomes. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 18: 332–340. World Health Organization. 2003. World health report 2003: Shaping the future. Geneva: WHO. Wright, S. L., and C. Persad. 2007. Distinguishing between depression and dementia in older persons: Neuropsychological and neuropathological correlates. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 20: 189–198. Zubenko, G. S., W. N. Zubenko, S. McPherson, et al. 2003. A collaborative study of the emergence and clinical features of the major depressive syndrome of Alzheimer ’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 160: 857–866. Zuccato, C., and E. Cattaneo. 2009. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in neurodegenerative diseases. Nat Rev Neurol 5: 311–322. Zunszain, P. A., C. Anacker, A. Cattaneo, L. A. Carvalho, and C. M. Pariante. In press. Glucocorticoids, cytokines and brain abnormalities in depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
Chapter 10
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease Frédéric Assal
The diagnosis of Alzheimer ’s disease (AD) requires a deficit in episodic memory and a second cognitive domain (e.g., language, visuospatial/ visuoperceptual abilities, limb praxis, or executive functions), as well as a significant functional decline assessed with measures of instrumental and basic activities of daily living (American Psychiatric Association 2000). Tools currently available for the diagnosis and/or the follow-up of patients on medication include brief bedside tests for cognitive functioning, such as the MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh 1975), many neuropsychological batteries, various structural and functional neuroimaging techniques, and cerebrospinal fluid markers. In order to demonstrate the clinical relevance of a drug-placebo difference in randomized clinical trials (RCTs), two outcome criteria have to be met, namely improvement of the core cognitive deficit—usually assessed with the ADAS-cog (Mohs, Rosen, and Davis 1983)—and improvement of a global or functional measure such as the Clinical Global Impressions of Change (CGIC) (Schneider and Olin 1996). In the last 15 years, behavioral and psychological symptoms in dementia (BPSD) have been increasingly recognized as part of the disease, which has led to the development of diagnostic instruments such as the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (Cummings et al. 1994) or the Behave-AD (Reisberg, Auer, and Monteiro 1996). BPSD, such as depression, anxiety, apathy, hallucinations, delusions, agitation, aggression, irritability, wandering, and food preference modification, are common in AD, frequently occur in combination, are demanding for caregivers, and lead to nursing
206
Dementia
home placement (Mega et al. 1996; Assal and Cummings 2002a). The presence and severity of BPSD depend on many medical and environmental factors. These behavioral and psychological symptoms associated with AD do not meet the criteria for diagnosis of axis I psychiatric diseases (i.e., major depression) as is the case for the same phenomenological symptoms outside a context of AD. As a result, many psychotropic drugs approved for psychiatric diseases have not been tested and are not approved for BPSD in AD. This chapter will review systematic meta-analyses, RCTs, and expert opinions on current neuropsychiatric treatments and will propose some practical recommendations from a clinical perspective. PRACTICAL ISSUES When and how to refer patients with dementia and/or BPSD to a specialist is an important issue and depends on the expertise of the physician, availability of specialists, and federal regulations (i.e., some specifically require certain treatments to be prescribed by specialists, whereas others allow prescription by primary care physicians). Referring all patients to specialists would not be cost-effective and specialists might get overbooked quickly. Principal reasons for referral are young-onset dementia/ predementia, especially in professionally active persons, rapid progression of symptoms, presence of family history, particularly difficult BPSD (i.e., depression, disinhibition, profound apathy, hallucinations, delusions), additional neurological signs (parkinsonism, cerebellar signs, gait problems, focal deficits), or abnormal neuroimaging findings (significant T2 hyperintensities, a significant number of microbleeds, focal lesions or atrophy, dilated ventricles). Before treating any BPSD, physicians must rule out delirium (i.e., in agitated patients) and frequently associated secondary conditions (e.g., urinary tract infection, pneumonia, stroke, subdural hematoma, hip fracture, dehydration, drug intoxication), they must try to eliminate environmental triggers (e.g., abnormal light, noise, or a sudden change of nursing staff), and finally they must train caregivers and/or nursing staff to recognize side effects in order to prevent increased morbidity or mortality (e.g., falls and bone fractures due to drowsiness or hypotension). When initiating BPSD treatment, target symptoms (e.g., agitation in the setting of depression or psychosis) should be identified together with the patient, the caregiver or the nursing home team in order to monitor treatment effect. When commencing a medication, medical charts should be studied carefully, whereby it is important to ensure that current and
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
207
past medications will not interact with the new treatment, causing any adverse events. The consistent monitoring of side effects is essential. In general, slow titration is the rule. When no response is noted, the target symptom and objectives should be reassessed with the patient and/or the caregiver. When the target symptom is significantly improved, treatment should be continued at the lowest possible dose, then slowly tapered after six to eight weeks. This rule does not apply to cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine, which should be prescribed for at least six months, unless side effects occur (see below). See Table 10.1 for main neuropsychiatric treatments in AD. CHOLINESTERASE INHIBITORS Besides neurofibrillary tangles, senile plaque formation, and neuronal and synaptic loss, AD is characterized by a reduced cholinergic neurotransmission, secondary to neuronal loss in the nucleus basalis of Meynert in the basal forebrain. Cholinesterase inhibitors (ChEIns) were developed in an attempt to enhance cholinergic neurotransmission in the cerebral cortex. Three ChEIns are currently approved in the US and Europe, but these have not yet been adequately compared in head-to-head trials (Trinh et al. 2003). In general, ChEIns yield a modest improvement in cognition for at least six months, with a 0.8 point increase in MMSE score (Courtney et al. 2004), a 0.1 SD increase on the activities of daily living (ADL) scales, and a 0.09 SD increase on instrumental ADL (IADL) scales (Trinh et al. 2003). In a naturalistic study over a 36-week period, no significant difference between the three molecules was noted with respect to their effect on primary outcomes (MMSE, ADAS-Cog). In a prospective, observational study representative of the real world clinical practice with 938 AD patients treated with all three ChEIns, patients on galantamine significantly worsened on the NPI and IADL compared to patients on donepezil (Santoro et al. 2010). An important limitation of ChEIns is that only 30–50% of patients respond to the treatment (Cummings 2003). There is thus an urgent need for techniques that can help us to predict which patients are likely to respond, in order to prevent treatment in those who will not respond. Long-term positive effects of ChEIns, such as a significantly reduced rate of nursing home placement or a prolongation of normal activities of daily living and behavior, are questionable and the results from trials reported in a meta-analysis conflicting (Trinh et al. 2003). ChEIns were cost effective, decreased the need for home care, and reduced nursing home placement by approximately 30% for each year of treatment (Feldman et al. 2009, Neumann et al. 1999). However, the only nonsponsored study did
AntiNMDA agents
ChEIns
Memantine
Moderate to severe stage
10 mg
Patch5-10 (20 in the U.S.)
1.5 mg bid (with meals)
Rivastigmine (R)
5 mg qs
8 mg qs (with meals)
Mild to moderate stage
Starting dose
Galantamine (G)
Donepezil (D)
Indication
Table 10.1 Current Neuropsychiatric Treatments Used in AD
4-week titration 10 mg bid
Increase in 2 week 3 mg daily increment Low dosage for 4 weeks, then increase
16 (24) mg qs after 4 (8) weeks
10 mg qs (increased after 4 weeks)
Maintenance dose (daily)
Dizziness, confusion, drowsiness, hallucinations
Easier titration (two steps) and oncedaily possible Rapid switch from ChEIns possible Might improve agitation
MCI: no consensus but a trial of D can be suggested
Same as above Same as above, GI SE more marked Less GI SE than with capsule
Head-to-head comparisons
D, G, R have similar efficacy
Gastrointestinal (GI) Sinus bradycardia, AV block
Comparisons with ChEIns
Better define responders vs. nonresponders
To be done
Practical comment
Side effects (SE)
Antipsychotics
12.5–25 mg qs 12.5 mg qs
Quietapine (Q)
Clozapine (C)
25–100 mg
25–150 mg
0.5–1 mg
2.5–5 mg
2.5 mg
0.5 mg
Delusions Agitation
Olanzapine (O)
0.5–1 mg (try to avoid)
0.5 mg im
Risperidone (R)
Extreme agitation, Acute psychosis
Conventional: Haloperidol (H), thioridazine, chlorpromazine
C: agranulocytosis
Same as above Fewer extrapyramidal SE (especially for Q and C) Weight gain
C: monitor leukocytes
In case of modest Sedation BPSD (psychoExtrapysis, agitation), ramidal try ChEIns or SE QT Memantine first H prolongation im if extreme agitation and for very short periods Q or C if extrapyramidal signs before treatment
New agents with better SE profile
More headto-head comparisons
More comparisons with atypical antipsychotics.
Try to avoid
Sedation, memory loss, falls, paradoxical reaction, dependence
Need for RCT
1-2.5 mg
No dependence
Acute stressful condition
Sedation
Benzodiazepine i.e,. lorazepam
300–2400 mg
100 mg
Agitation, anxiety (?)
Gabapentin
Agitation, tremor, anorexia, nausea, drowsiness More rarely serotoninergic syndrome
No evidence based recommendations, should not be given, cave confusion and hyponatremia with carbamazepin, confusion/encephalopathy, ataxia, tremor with valproate
50–200 mg in one of 2 doses
50 (100) mg bid
20–40 mg qs
Carbamazepine Valproate acid
25–50 mg
Particularly if insomnia
Trazodone
10–20 mg
50 mg
Depression Agitation
Sertraline
Citalopram
(Continued)
Note: BPSD = behavioral and psychological symptoms in dementia; ChEIns = cholinesterase inhibitors; MCI = mild cognitive impairment; RCT = randomized clinical trial.
Others
Antidepressants
Table 10.1
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
211
not find any nursing home placement benefit in treated patients compared to placebo (Courtney et al. 2004). Recent population cohort studies of more than 10,000 participants confirmed preliminary case reports showing a greater risk of bradycardia or syncope in patients on ChEIns compared to the no-treatment group. Patients suffering from bradycardia and/or syncope were more likely to fall, experience hip fracture, or need a pacemaker implantation (Gill et al. 2009; Hernandez et al. 2009). Donepezil has minimal peripheral anticholinesterase activity and is therefore generally well tolerated by patients. It is the most popular ChEInh on the market because of its easy once-daily titration. It has been demonstrated to significantly improve cognitive and global rating scales, such as the ADAS-cog, the MMSE, as well as the clinicians’ global ratings, in patients with mild to moderate AD, but it did not significantly improve quality-of-life measures (Rogers et al. 1998). Extended treatment over six months was safe and effective in mild to moderate stages (Winblad et al. 2001) as well as in an advanced stage (Winblad et al. 2006). A nonsponsored RCT showed that patients in mild to moderate stage of the disease demonstrated a small but significant positive effect of the medication (0.8-point increase in MMSE score over two years compared to placebo) but there was no delay in the need for institutional care of patients (Courtney et al. 2004). In this study, a return to baseline cognition level after a sixweek washout period strongly suggests an absence of disease-modifying activity of donepezil. BPSD may be modestly improved by donepezil (Sink, Holden, and Yaffe 2005). In mild cognitive impairment (MCI), it was the only cholinesterase inhibitor that significantly improved cognitive outcome (MMSE, ADAS-Cog, CDR) after a six-month follow-up, but it had no effect on the rate of conversion to dementia after three years (Petersen et al. 2005). Galantamine has slightly more gastrointestinal side effects (nausea, vomiting, anorexia, and weight loss), which can be attenuated by using a once-daily prescription. Overall, its clinical efficacy is similar to donepezil. RCTs and post-hoc analysis of RCTs demonstrated a significant positive effect of galantamine (16 or 24 mg per day) on cognition and ADL in the treated group over the placebo group (Aronson et al. 2009; Wilcock, Lilienfeld, and Gaens 2000). Positive effects on cognition were sustained for up to 36 months (Raskind et al. 2004) and were even significant in patients with severe AD, and without causing an increase in mortality (Burns et al. 2009). Galantamine, unlike donepezil, is not recommended for patients with MCI, due to a significant increased mortality (Mayor 2005).
212
Dementia
Rivastigmine is the cholinesterase inhibitor with the most peripheral cholinergic side effects. It is recommended to be taken with food and following a very slow titration schedule. Its advantages over other ChEIns are fewer interactions with other medications and less liver metabolism, with a similar clinical efficacy (Rosler et al. 1999). A once-daily transdermal patch is also available for patients in the mild-to-moderate stage of the disease. The patch may cause application-site skin reaction, but it has a much better gastrointestinal profile and it is better accepted than capsules by the caregiver because of its easier administration. These factors make rivastigmine as attractive as the other two ChEIns (Winblad, Grosberg, et al. 2007). Pooled data from two RCTs with oral rivastigmine demonstrated that occurrence of hallucinations in patients with AD was predictive of a better response than in patients without hallucinations (Cummings et al. 2010). ChEIns and BPSD: In a meta-analysis, ChEIns offered a small but significant alleviation of BPSD with a 1.72-point improvement on the NPI (95% confidence interval 0.87–2.57 points), and 0.03-point improvement on the ADAS-noncog (95% CI, 0.00–0.05 points) (Trinh et al. 2003). Other meta-analyses came to the conclusion of similar significant but modest (Sink, Holden, and Yaffe 2005) or limited efficacy (Rodda, Morgan, and Walker 2009). These findings remain debatable because most patients presented with mild symptoms only, and subsequent studies of rivastigmine and donepezil did not replicate the benefits in patients with severe symptoms (Howard et al. 2007). In an analysis of 271 questionnaires sent out to primary care physicians, neurologists, geriatricians, and psychiatrists dealing with patients with AD, ChEIns were rated to improve cognitive functions, attention, and initiative (Rockwood et al. 2004). It is necessary to develop tools to better capture these subjectively observed effects. In patients in a more severe stage of the disease, with average NPI scores of 30, donepezil significantly reduced BPSD, in particular mood and psychosis (Cummings, McRae, and Zhang 2006). MEMANTINE N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonists, which presumably reduce abnormal excitotoxicity in the brain, constitute the second class of medication accepted by federal regulations for symptomatic treatment of AD. Twenty mg of memantine daily in two doses significantly stabilizes or even improves cognition and/or IADL, as demonstrated by several 28-week randomized trials in patients with moderate to severe AD (Reisberg et al. 2003; Tariot et al. 2004; Winblad and Poritis 1999). There
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
213
were no more adverse effects in the treatment group than in the placebo group, suggesting a better side-effect profile than cholinesterase inhibitors. Dizziness, confusion, and hallucinations were the most frequent side effects. Two meta-analyses of published studies (in moderate to severe AD patients and in mild to moderate AD patients) found a significant but very modest benefit of memantine on cognition and global changes (Qaseem et al. 2008; Winblad, Jones, et al. 2007). Administration of the medication on a once-daily schedule appeared to be similar in terms of efficacy and tolerability (Jones et al. 2007). A post-hoc analysis of four clinical trials confirmed that moderate to severe AD patients treated with memantine benefited from treatment, with a more pronounced positive effect in those patients with substantial language impairment (Ferris et al. 2009). Memantine might be cost effective in AD, but more research is required to confirm this (Antonanzas et al. 2006). A post-hoc analysis of six pooled RCTs demonstrated diminished BPSD in the memantine treatment group compared to the placebo group, particularly for psychosis and agitation/aggression (Gauthier, Loft, and Cummings 2008). Another meta-analysis including the same RCTs replicated the conclusion of a significant improvement of BPSD, but questioned whether there was a clinical benefit of the medication, mainly because of the small effect size (Maidment et al. 2008). In clinical practice, a rapid switch from donepezil to memantine was tolerated as well as a stepwise one (Waldemar et al. 2008) offering a possibility for patients with contraindications, side effects, or deterioration on ChEIns. Currently, there are no powerful head-to-head trials directly comparing the efficacies of memantine and ChEIns, but these are being planned (Jones et al. 2009). One preliminary comparative double-blind randomized trial with a small number of patients did not find any significant difference between memantine and donepezil (Modrego et al. 2009). To date, long-term effects of memantine in patients with mild to moderate AD have not been published. There is an urgent need to study the possible neuroprotective effects of memantine, and to study side effects in large clinical samples. MEMANTINE PLUS CHEINS In one RCT, the combination of memantine and donepezil was found to be more effective than donepezil alone in AD patients in a moderate to severe stage, in terms of cognition, ADLs, global functioning, and behavior (Tariot et al. 2004). These results were extended for the same dataset using a responder analysis (van Dyck, Schmitt, and Olin 2006). However,
214
Dementia
another trial with stable patients studying the efficacy of the combination of memantine with ChEIns (donepezil, rivastigmine, or galantamine) did not replicate this advantage over the combination of placebo and ChEIns (Porsteinsson et al. 2008). Neither of these trials found any significant differences in side effects, suggesting that the combination was well tolerated. Because of these inconsistencies reported on efficacy of the combination of memantine plus ChEIns, several medication agencies have not authorized reimbursement of the combined treatment. OTHER MEDICATIONS FOR DEMENTIA It is beyond the scope of this chapter to discuss all medications that have been developed and tested in AD. Below is a selection of the most interesting molecules. Treatment with Vitamin E (alpha tocopherol), an antioxidant with possible neuroprotective effects, has been demonstrated in one RCT (Sano et al. 1997) to result in a significant delay of institutionalization, but there was no effect on other primary or secondary outcomes. Since these results have not been replicated, vitamin E is usually not recommended (Isaac, Quinn, and Tabet 2008) and no longer prescribed for AD. Ginkgo biloba is another agent with antioxidant properties, which has been commercialized in Europe for more than two decades, but has been shown to result in no significant benefits in a large RCT (DeKosky et al. 2008). Its use is therefore not recommended in AD. Estrogen replacement therapy was considered as a promising treatment because of its role in cerebral blood flow enhancement, cholinergic neuronal death prevention, and nerve growth factor modulation in animals and in vitro. Unfortunately, large RCTs did not observe a modification of conversion rates to dementia, and even showed that estrogen replacement was associated with an increased risk of developing dementia and increased morbidity and mortality rates (Rapp et al. 2003). Anti-inflammatory drugs—traditional nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs or selective COX-2 inhibitors were tested in AD or in AD prevention, given the involvement of inflammatory mechanisms (i.e., activated microglia or cytokine release) in AD neuropathology (for a review see McGeer and McGeer 2007). Several pilot trials using therapeutically established doses of indomethacin and diclofenac/misoprostol yielded promising results but large RCTs using COX-2-inhibitors, ibuprofen, or aspirin did not demonstrate any significant benefit (de Jong et al. 2008; Pasqualetti et al. 2009), reported increased bleeding (Bentham et al. 2008), or were even suspended due to an increased rate of cardiovascular events (McGeer and
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
215
McGeer 2007; Meinert, McCaffrey, and Breitner 2009). Anti-inflammatory drugs might be ineffective once AD lesions have become too important, or when the drug is administered too briefly to have any measurable effect. The use of anti-inflammatory drugs in prevention or treatment of AD is not advocated. Vascular risk factor management: strokes aggravate AD pathology (Snowdon et al. 1997) and main vascular risk factors (i.e., diabetes or impaired insulin secretion, hypertension, high cholesterol, obesity) accelerate the amyloid cascade in vitro, or clinical signs of dementia in vivo, as demonstrated by epidemiological studies (Hayden et al. 2006). Strict management of high blood pressure, diabetes, and cholesterol levels are strongly recommended and supported by recent studies in AD patients with (Richard et al. 2010) or without cerebrovascular disease (Deschaintre et al. 2009) and no history of cerebrovascular disease (McGuinness et al. 2006). However, cholesterol-lowering agents (Zhou, Teramukai, and Fukushima 2007) or antihypertensive drugs did not modify the rate of progression to dementia in patients with high cholesterol or hypertension respectively. Lack of an adequately long follow-up period or an intervention that is too late could explain these negative results, given that midlife risk factors were associated with occurrence of dementia decades later (Fitzpatrick et al. 2009; Kivipelto et al. 2001). There is insufficient evidence to support the effectiveness of melatonin (Jansen et al. 2006), folic acid with or without vitamin B12 (Malouf and Grimley Evans 2008), multivitamin supplement containing vitamin B12 and folic acid (Sun et al. 2007), lecithin (Higgins and Flicker 2003), or piracetam (Flicker and Grimley Evans 2001) for managing cognitive impairment or BPSD in AD. ANTIPSYCHOTIC AGENTS Conventional antipsychotics such as haloperidol or thioridazine were used for many years to treat BPSD, but their effects have only been studied in small and often poorly controlled trials 25 years ago (Devanand et al. 1989; Risse, Lampe, and Cubberley 1987; Steele, Lucas, and Tune 1986). Target symptoms including psychosis, hostility, uncooperativeness, bothersomeness, emotional lability, and irritability generally improved, but patients could not be maintained on high doses (i.e., more than 4 mg of haloperidol daily) due to severe parkinsonism, and frequent worsening of cognition. Moreover, haloperidol might be neurotoxic inducing apoptosis (Wei et al. 2006). In the last decade, atypical antipsychotic agents have been widely used against agitation, aggression, and delusions because of
216
Dementia
a better extrapyramidal profile than conventional medication. However, atypical antipsychotics have been associated with increased mortality and cardiovascular morbidity, and are still not approved for treatment of AD by federal regulations in most European countries and the United States. There have been very few trials that have compared the efficacy of different atypical antipsychotic agents. A systematic review including 12 trials plus two additional studies of haloperidol, thioridazine, thioxanthene, chlorpromazine, and acetophenazine, did not demonstrate any benefit for patients (Sink, Holden, Yaffe, et al. 2005). Two other systematic reviews concluded that atypical antipsychotics such as clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone, and quietapine had only modest efficacy, and one RCT even claimed that these agents (clozapine not included) had no effect on psychosis, aggression, or agitation compared to a placebo (Schneider, Dagerman, and Insel 2005; Schneider et al. 2006). In addition, second-generation antipsychotics were associated with weight gain in women, olanzapine and quietapine in particular, and with decreases in HDL cholesterol and girth, particularly with olanzapine (Zheng et al. 2009). Aripiprazole did not have any benefit on psychosis but agitation, anxiety, and depression were mildly improved in institutionalized AD patients (Streim et al. 2008). Atypical antipsychotics were also found to elicit side effects, including extrapyramidal symptoms and sedation (Schneider et al. 2006). Increased mortality and stroke risk, although debated due to conflicting results, has been another concern recently (Schneider, Dagerman, and Insel 2005). Withdrawal of risperidone (or conventional antipsychotics such as thioridazine, chlorpromazine, haloperidol, or trifluoperazine) had no overall detrimental effect on functional and cognitive status in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled parallel two-group treatment discontinuation trial (Ballard et al. 2008). Finally, atypical antipsychotics were not cost-effective compared to placebo (Rosenheck et al. 2007). For these reasons, atypical antipsychotic agents should not be routinely used against agitation or psychosis in AD in the community. Despite all these limitations, an evaluation of antipsychotic prescribing practice for inpatients in the U.K. found 56% of patients were receiving antipsychotics. These patients were the ones with severe BPSD, particularly aggressivity or agitation (Haw, Stubbs, and Yorston 2008). ANTIDEPRESSANTS Many recent studies have focused on the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), driven by the finding that serotonin and noradrenergic neurotransmission is disturbed in agitation/aggression and
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
217
depression in AD, and that these symptoms are common in the course of the disease (Assal et al. 2004; Assal and Cummings 2002b; Mega et al. 1996). Several trials have demonstrated that citalopram (Nyth and Gottfries 1990) and sertraline (Lyketsos et al. 2000) have a beneficial effect in depression in AD. However, diarrhea, dizziness, dry mouth, and serious pulmonary adverse events were reported in a recent RCT with sertraline (Weintraub et al. 2010). Overall, because of small sample sizes, insufficient data, absence of significant results and adverse events, a meta-analysis did not support the administration of SSRIs and recommended extreme caution (Bains, Birks, and Dening 2002). Tricyclic antidepressants (i.e., amitriptyline or nortriptyline) should not be prescribed anymore because of their anticholinergic side effects, potentially aggravating memory problems and resulting in dry mouth, confusion, and hypotension (Taragano et al. 1997). Results of RCTs with other new generation antidepressants like venlafaxine, mirtazapine, and bupropion have yet to be reported. ANTI-EPILEPTIC DRUGS There is no evidence from systematic reviews or meta-analyses that carbamazepine or valproate alleviate BPSD in AD (Lonergan and Luxenberg 2009; Sink, Holden, and Yaffe 2005). Valproate did not improve agitation/ aggressivity and was poorly tolerated in moderate to severe stages of the disease (Herrmann et al. 2007). Oxcarbazepine did not reduce agitation either (Sommer et al. 2009). Topiramate in small doses (25–50 mg) had the same efficacy as risperidone (0.5–2 mg), as demonstrated in a small RCT (Mowla and Pani 2010). There is not sufficient evidence to propose gabapentin as treatment in AD (Kim, Wilkins, and Tampi 2008). RCT with lamotrigine, gabapentin, or levetiracetam, which are increasingly used in clinical settings, are expected. Benzodiazepines should only be used in the case of brief stressful episodes, with extreme caution and in small doses considering their side effects, such as a decline of memory function, gait disturbances, dependence, and possible paradoxical reactions. OTHER COMPOUNDS USED IN VARIOUS BPSD Trazodone (triazolopyridine-derivative phenylpiperazine with combined properties such as serotonergic antidepressant and alpha 2-adrenergic blocking activity and no anticholinergic effects) at 200 mg per day modestly reduced agitation in AD, but was nevertheless not superior to
218
Dementia
behavior management technique (Teri et al. 2000). In a six-month naturalistic study, trazodone was found to stabilize BPSD (and particularly irritability) and reduce caregiver burden compared to the untreated group, probably because of its sedative effect (Lopez-Pousa et al. 2008). In a small non-RCT study of only six patients with severe dementia (mainly AD), repetitive and stereotypic behaviors (mainly skin picking or scratching, trichophagia) were successfully treated with a daily dose of 5–20 mg of buspirone (Helvink and Holroyd 2006). Propranol (80–560 mg per day) improved disruptive behavior as demonstrated by case series (Shankle, Nielson, and Cotman 1995; Weiler, Mungas, and Bernick 1988) and one small RCT (Peskind et al. 2005), but its usefulness was very limited by the high frequency of contraindications. Zolpidem, a nonbenzodiazepine hypnotic drug with proven safety and efficacy in older patients with insomnia, was well tolerated and improved sleep patterns in two patients with dementia and severe nighttime wandering at the dose of 10–15 mg qs (Shelton and Hocking 1997). In practice, one should first consider sleep-hygiene modification, exercise, or light therapy in treating insomnia in persons with dementia. Lithium has not been studied as a mood stabilizer in any large RCT of patients with AD. Its mechanism is still obscure. It possibly has a protective effect against dementia, suggested by the fact that its prescription was associated with a reduced rate of dementia in patients with bipolar disorders compared to other treatments (antidepressants, antipsychotics, antiepileptics) in a retrospective study (Kessing, Forman, and Andersen 2010). In vitro and animal studies have shown that lithium has interesting anti-amyloïd and anti-tau properties, such as a decrease of amyloid beta (Aβ) peptide production and inhibition of the activity of glycogen synthase kinase-3, which induces aggregation of tau protein into tangles, and tau hyperphosphorylation. However, a small RCT did not demonstrate any modification of cognition, depression and tau cerebrospinal fluid levels in AD patients treated with lithium compared to placebo (Hampel et al. 2009). Treatment of patients with abnormal vocalizations (i.e., noise-making, screaming, perseverative vocalizations, continuous chattering, muttering, singing or humming, swearing, grunting and bizarre sound-making) remains a challenge (Nagaratnam, Patel, and Whelan 2003). Depression should be ruled out since abnormal noise-making and especially headbanging can be associated with this condition (Greenwald, Marin, and Silverman 1986). Changing environmental factors and behavioral therapies should be considered before pharmacotherapy (Barton, Findlay, and Blake 2005).
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
219
There is no approved treatment for Capgras syndrome (a misidentification syndrome where the patient sees his or her spouse as a visually identical imposter). In one case study, a patient with Capgras, dementia, and parkinsonism was successfully treated with repetitive electroconvulsive therapy (Chiu 2009) and another one with mirtazapine (Khouzam 2002). At the present institution, all cases with AD and Capgras syndrome were resistant to any medication (antiepileptics, antipsychotics); several cases of Capgras syndrome were successfully improved with ChEIns but these patients presented with parkinsonism and other criteria for dementia with Lewy bodies. In all cases the syndrome reappeared after two to three months. There are no guidelines available for the treatment of abnormal sexual behaviors in AD and in dementia in general (Guay 2008). This particular BPSD is complex and not infrequent, but poorly studied and rarely reported. There are no consistent data regarding the treatment for females. In clinical practice, before initiating treatment, one should first differentiate issues with intimacy, libido problems, sexual obsessive-compulsive disorder, or real sexual disinhibition. Haloperidol (Rosenthal et al. 2003) at small doses, olanzapine (Dhikav, Anand, and Aggarwal 2007), or citalopram (Tosto et al. 2008) successfully reduced symptoms as reported in case reports of AD patients with hypersexual behavior. Other compounds that may be tried in males include tricyclic antidepressants, estrogens (oral or transdermal), antiandrogens (cyproterone acetate, medroxyprogesterone acetate), and the LHRH agonists (e.g., leuprolide, triptorelin) (Guay 2008). Abnormal wandering in AD and dementia is common at late stages and must first be differentiated from akathisia (acute or tardive), restless leg syndrome, and agitation in delirium. There is no pharmacological treatment for that particular BPSD. UNRESOLVED ISSUES There are no approved guidelines or consensus on the duration of therapy with ChEIns and memantine. We therefore suggest introducing the treatment for at least six months, after which the patient’s response should be reviewed and a brief cognitive and neuropsychiatric examination should be performed. Treatment should be continued if improvement or stabilization is noted on MMSE, IADL, and family or caregivers’ impression. Treatment should be discontinued if major side effects occur (see Table 10.1) or if patients significantly deteriorate. However, if patients drastically worsen after discontinuation, we suggest to reintroduce the
220
Dementia
treatment, in the absence of major contra-indications. In general, ChEIns can be continued for at least two years, until a moderate to severe stage, when memantine can be introduced. When to stop memantine is not known. We advise discontinuation when the disease is very advanced (MMSE <7) and/or if no benefits are observed, or in the case of adverse effects. The development of valid clinical markers of efficacy that could be translated to clinical practice is essential. FUTURE DIRECTIONS AND CONCLUSIONS There is still a long way to go in the development of neuropsychiatric treatments of AD. More fundamental research is necessary to allow for an earlier and more accurate diagnosis and follow-up of the progression of the disease (e.g., by identifying biomarkers). Also the identification of the various subtypes of AD is important, given that AD is not a single entity but a complex syndrome exhibiting a diversity of clinical phenotypes, from a cognitive and behavioral point of view. An earlier intervention aided by earlier and more accurate diagnosis could lead to slowing down neuronal damage and blocking amyloid deposition and neurofibrillary tangle formation. RCT of Aβ immunization are promising, with one phase IIa trial showing significant clinical benefit in antibody responders (Gilman et al. 2005) that was still measurable 4.6 years after immunization (Vellas et al. 2009), even though this trial was interrupted because of meningo-encephalitis in 6% of patients (Orgogozo et al. 2003). New RCTs mostly with passive immunization against abeta are currently running and already showed promising results (Lemere and Masliah 2010). Other anticipated compounds are neuroprotective agents, such as antioxidants and anti-inflammatory agents, which are expected to reduce the damage caused by amyloid proteins. Neurorestorative approaches such as neurotrophic and nerve growth factors, transplantations, and stem-cell related interventions are currently being developed. A better identification of behavioral and psychological symptoms in AD is necessary, which is difficult given their multidimensional and nonspecific character. Like Alzheimer ’s disease itself, most BPSD belong to various syndromes. For example, depression encompasses a large variety of emotional symptoms, but specifically associated symptoms are different from patient to patient. Some AD patients with depression present with anxiety, accompanying neurovegetative symptoms, diminished sleep or appetite, and apathy. Other patients present with delusions, which clearly aggravate their cognitive symptoms. Causes of depression in AD are not only secondary to abnormal serotoninergic and noradrenergic neurotransmission.
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
221
Genetic background factors such as single nucleotide polymorphisms may play an important role (Assal et al. 2004) as well as medical/neurological (i.e., illnesses, medications, cerebral vascular ischemia), psychological (i.e., reaction to loss of functioning, decreasing health, general effects of aging), and other social or cultural environmental influences. Importantly, there is still a lack of consensus about how to treat most BPSD. There are few systematic studies and a lack of empirical data supporting the effectiveness of specific treatments. Unfortunately, all compounds that have been demonstrated to have a significant positive effect on various measures of cognitive, behavioral or more global functions, yielded little if any clinically meaningful benefit (Qaseem et al. 2008; Schneider et al. 2006). More pharmacodynamic research is needed in AD to better understand the physiological effects on the body and the mechanisms of the action of current neuropsychiatric treatments. Low albumin level in patients was associated with greater cognitive response to donepezil at three months as measured with the ADAS-cog (Rozzini et al. 2008). Patients on donepezil with high cerebrospinal fluid acetylcholine esterase inhibition showed more stable MMSE scores during periods up to two years, than those with lower levels (Darreh-Shori et al. 2006). Haloperidol plasmatic level appeared to be a better predictor of efficacy than haloperidol dosage, as measured by the reduction in various BPSD measurements (Pelton et al. 2003). More nonsponsored or independent RCTs should be carried out and head-to-head trials are needed, which compare the different medications that are currently available (e.g., the three available ChEIns, ChEIns and memantine, the different antidepressants, atypical and classical antipsychotics, antiepileptics, and so on). We also urgently need to reassess our clinical scales in order to better capture clinical modification, to ensure accurate clinical monitoring of patients under treatment of symptomatic agents, or of possible future disease-modifying agents. Sensitive clinical scales are particularly important in this latter case, because the most likely candidates to be treated with future disease-modifying agents are early AD patients, in whom decline is slow, and clinical changes are variable and subtle. At the population level, there is a need for clinical markers that are sensitive, widely available, and cost-effective, as society will not be able to afford expensive and repetitive biomarker assessment for every single patient or subject. In that respect, gait analysis using dual-task related paradigms could be an interesting candidate, because it is easy and inexpensive to administer, applicable to patients with a wide range of levels of functioning and gait and cognition
222
Dementia
are closely linked. A small open-label pilot study already demonstrated an improvement of gait parameters in patients with AD, treated with galantamine (Assal et al. 2008). Better informed guidelines for appropriate commencing and discontinuation time points for medication treatment are also needed, in order to prevent unnecessary treatment when no efficacy is proven. Explanations on medication related issues (i.e., benefits that can be expected) to our patients and families have to be improved. A combination of educational interventions targeting medication management capacity of the patient and caregiver assistance will ameliorate adherence to neuropsychiatric treatments (Arlt et al. 2008). Finally, a better understanding of BPSD from a cognitive, psychological and social perspective can aid the development of additional nonpharmacological therapies including patient and caregiver education. Together, these strategies will contribute to improve patient care and reduce the emotional and financial burden for families and society.
REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. 2000. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. Antonanzas, F., B. Rive, J. M. Badenas, S. Gomez-Lus, and C. Guilhaume. 2006. Cost-effectiveness of memantine in community-based Alzheimer ’s disease patients: An adaptation in Spain. Eur J Health Econ 7 (2): 137–144. Arlt, S., R. Lindner, A. Rosler, and W. von Renteln-Kruse. 2008. Adherence to medication in patients with dementia: Predictors and strategies for improvement. Drugs Aging 25 (12): 1033–1047. Aronson, S., B. Van Baelen, S. Kavanagh, and S. Schwalen. 2009. Optimal dosing of galantamine in patients with mild or moderate Alzheimer ’s disease: Post hoc analysis of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Drugs Aging 26 (3): 231–239. Assal, F., M. Alarcon, E. C. Solomon, D. Masterman, D. H. Geschwind, and J. L. Cummings. 2004. Association of the serotonin transporter and receptor gene polymorphisms in neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 61 (8): 1249–1253. Assal, F., G. Allali, R. W. Kressig, F. R. Herrmann, and O. Beauchet. 2008. Galantamine improves gait performance in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 56 (5): 946–947. Assal, F., and J. L. Cummings. 2002a. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in the dementias. Curr Opin Neurol 15 (4): 445–450. Assal, F., and J. L. Cummings. 2002b. Neuropathological changes underlying neuropsychiatric symptoms on Alzheimer ’s disease. In Alzheimer ’s disease and
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
223
related disorders annual 2002, ed. S. Gauthier and J. L. Cummings, pp. 199–215. London: Martin Dunitz. Bains, J., J. S. Birks, and T. R. Dening. 2002. The efficacy of antidepressants in the treatment of depression in dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (4): CD003944. Ballard, C., M. M. Lana, M. Theodoulou, S. Douglas, R. McShane, R. Jacoby, K. Kossakowski, L. M. Yu, and E. Juszczak. 2008. A randomised, blinded, placebo-controlled trial in dementia patients continuing or stopping neuroleptics (the Dart-AD Trial). PLoS Med 5 (4): e76. Barton, S., D. Findlay, and R. A. Blake. 2005. The management of inappropriate vocalisation in dementia: A hierarchical approach. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 20 (12): 1180–1186. Bentham, P., R. Gray, E. Sellwood, R. Hills, P. Crome, and J. Raftery. 2008. Aspirin in Alzheimer ’s disease (AD2000): A randomised open-label trial. Lancet Neurol 7 (1): 41–49. Burns, A., R. Bernabei, R. Bullock, A. J. Cruz Jentoft, L. Frolich, C. Hock, M. Raivio, et al. 2009. Safety and efficacy of galantamine (reminyl) in severe Alzheimer ’s disease (the Serad Study): A randomised, placebo-controlled, doubleblind trial. Lancet Neurol 8 (1): 39–47. Chiu, N. M. 2009. Repeated electroconvulsive therapy for a patient with Capgras syndrome and Parkinsonism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33 (6): 1084–1085. Courtney, C., D. Farrell, R. Gray, R. Hills, L. Lynch, E. Sellwood, S. Edwards, et al. 2004. Long-term donepezil treatment in 565 patients with Alzheimer ’s disease (AD2000): Randomised double-blind trial. Lancet 363 (9427): 2105–2115. Cummings, J. L. 2003. Use of cholinesterase inhibitors in clinical practice: Evidence-based recommendations. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 11 (2): 131–145. Cummings, J., M. Emre, D. Aarsland, S. Tekin, N. Dronamraju, and R. Lane. 2010. Effects of rivastigmine in Alzheimer ’s disease patients with and without hallucinations. J Alzheimers Dis 20 (1): 301–311. Cummings, J. L., T. McRae, and R. Zhang. 2006. Effects of donepezil on neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with dementia and severe behavioral disorders. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 14 (7): 605–612. Cummings, J. L., M. Mega, K. Gray, S. Rosenberg-Thompson, D. A. Carusi, and J. Gornbein. 1994. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 44 (12): 2308–2314. Darreh-Shori, T., L. Meurling, T. Pettersson, K. Hugosson, E. Hellstrom-Lindahl, N. Andreasen, L. Minthon, and A. Nordberg. 2006. Changes in the activity and protein levels of CSF acetylcholinesterases in relation to cognitive function of patients with mild Alzheimer ’s disease following chronic donepezil treatment. J Neural Transm 113 (11): 1791–1801. de Jong, D., R. Jansen, W. Hoefnagels, M. Jellesma-Eggenkamp, M. Verbeek, G. Borm, and B. Kremer. 2008. No effect of one-year treatment with indomethacin on
224
Dementia
Alzheimer ’s disease progression: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 3 (1): e1475. DeKosky, S. T., J. D. Williamson, A. L. Fitzpatrick, R. A. Kronmal, D. G. Ives, J. A. Saxton, O. L. Lopez, et al. 2008. Ginkgo biloba for prevention of dementia: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 300 (19): 2253–2262. Deschaintre, Y., F. Richard, D. Leys, and F. Pasquier. 2009. Treatment of vascular risk factors is associated with slower decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 73 (9): 674–680. Devanand, D. P., H. A. Sackeim, R. P. Brown, and R. Mayeux. 1989. A pilot study of haloperidol treatment of psychosis and behavioral disturbance in Alzheimer ’s disease. Arch Neurol 46 (8): 854–857. Dhikav, V., K. Anand, and N. Aggarwal. 2007. Grossly disinhibited sexual behavior in dementia of Alzheimer ’s type. Arch Sex Behav 36 (2): 133–134. Feldman, H. H., T. Pirttila, J. F. Dartigues, B. Everitt, B. Van Baelen, S. Schwalen, and S. Kavanagh. 2009. Treatment with galantamine and time to nursing home placement in Alzheimer ’s disease patients with and without cerebrovascular disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 24 (5): 479–488. Ferris, S., R. Ihl, P. Robert, B. Winblad, G. Gatz, F. Tennigkeit, and S. Gauthier. 2009. Treatment effects of memantine on language in moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease patients. Alzheimers Dement 5 (5): 369–374. Fitzpatrick, A. L., L. H. Kuller, O. L. Lopez, P. Diehr, E. S. O’Meara, W. T. Longstreth, Jr., and J. A. Luchsinger. 2009. Midlife and late-life obesity and the risk of dementia: Cardiovascular health study. Arch Neurol 66 (3): 336–342. Flicker, L., and G. Grimley Evans. 2001. Piracetam for dementia or cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2): CD001011. Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh. 1975. Mini-Mental State. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12 (3): 189–198. Gauthier, S., H. Loft, and J. Cummings. 2008. Improvement in behavioural symptoms in patients with moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease by memantine: A pooled data analysis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 23 (5): 537–545. Gill, S. S., G. M. Anderson, H. D. Fischer, C. M. Bell, P. Li, S. L. Normand, and P. A. Rochon. 2009. Syncope and its consequences in patients with dementia receiving cholinesterase inhibitors: A population-based cohort study. Arch Intern Med 169 (9): 867–873. Gilman, S., M. Koller, R. S. Black, L. Jenkins, S. G. Griffith, N. C. Fox, L. Eisner, et al. 2005. Clinical effects of Abeta immunization (An1792) in patients with AD in an interrupted trial. Neurology 64 (9): 1553–1562. Greenwald, B. S., D. B. Marin, and S. M. Silverman. 1986. Serotoninergic treatment of screaming and banging in dementia. Lancet 2 (8521–22): 1464–1465. Guay, D. R. 2008. Inappropriate sexual behaviors in cognitively impaired older individuals. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother 6 (5): 269–288. Hampel, H., M. Ewers, K. Burger, P. Annas, A. Mortberg, A. Bogstedt, L. Frolich, et al. 2009. Lithium trial in Alzheimer ’s disease: A randomized, single-blind,
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
225
placebo-controlled, multicenter 10-week study. J Clin Psychiatry 70 (6): 922–931. Haw, C., J. Stubbs, and G. Yorston. 2008. Antipsychotics for Bpsd: An audit of prescribing practice in a specialist psychiatric inpatient unit. Int Psychogeriatr 20 (4): 790–799. Hayden, K. M., P. P. Zandi, C. G. Lyketsos, A. S. Khachaturian, L. A. Bastian, G. Charoonruk, J. T. Tschanz, et al. 2006. Vascular risk factors for incident Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia: The Cache County Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 20 (2): 93–100. Helvink, B., and S. Holroyd. 2006. Buspirone for stereotypic movements in elderly with cognitive impairment. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 18 (2): 242–244. Hernandez, R. K., W. Farwell, M. D. Cantor, and E. V. Lawler. 2009. Cholinesterase inhibitors and incidence of bradycardia in patients with dementia in the Veterans Affairs New England Healthcare System. J Am Geriatr Soc 57 (11): 1997–2003. Herrmann, N., K. L. Lanctot, L. S. Rothenburg, and G. Eryavec. 2007. A placebocontrolled trial of valproate for agitation and aggression in Alzheimer ’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 23 (2): 116–119. Higgins, J. P., and L. Flicker. 2003. Lecithin for dementia and cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (3): CD001015. Howard, R. J., E. Juszczak, C. G. Ballard, P. Bentham, R. G. Brown, R. Bullock, A. S. Burns, et al. 2007. Donepezil for the treatment of agitation in Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 357 (14): 1382–1392. Isaac, M. G., R. Quinn, and N. Tabet. 2008. Vitamin E for Alzheimer ’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (3): CD002854. Jansen, S. L., D. A. Forbes, V. Duncan, and D. G. Morgan. 2006. Melatonin for cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (1): CD003802. Jones, R. W., A. Bayer, F. Inglis, A. Barker, and R. Phul. 2007. Safety and tolerability of once-daily versus twice-daily memantine: A randomised, double-blind study in moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 22 (3): 258–262. Jones, R., B. Sheehan, P. Phillips, E. Juszczak, J. Adams, A. Baldwin, C. Ballard, et al. 2009. Domino-AD protocol: Donepezil and memantine in moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease—a multicentre RCT. Trials 10: 57. Kessing, L. V., J. L. Forman, and P. K. Andersen. 2010. Does lithium protect against dementia? Bipolar Disord 12 (1): 87–94. Khouzam, H. R. 2002. Capgras syndrome responding to the antidepressant mirtazapine. Compr Ther 28 (3): 238–240. Kim, Y., K. M. Wilkins, and R. R. Tampi. 2008. Use of gabapentin in the treatment of behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia: A review of the evidence. Drugs Aging 25 (3): 187–196. Kivipelto, M., E. L. Helkala, M. P. Laakso, T. Hanninen, M. Hallikainen, K. Alhainen, H. Soininen, J. Tuomilehto, and A. Nissinen. 2001. Midlife vascular risk
226
Dementia
factors and Alzheimer ’s disease in later life: Longitudinal, population based study. BMJ 322 (7300): 1447–1451. Lemere, C. A., and E. Masliah. 2010. Can Alzheimer disease be prevented by amyloid-beta immunotherapy? Nat Rev Neurol 6 (2): 108–119. Lonergan, E., and J. Luxenberg. 2009. Valproate preparations for agitation in dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (3): CD003945. Lopez-Pousa, S., J. Garre-Olmo, J. Vilalta-Franch, A. Turon-Estrada, and I. PericotNierga. 2008. Trazodone for Alzheimer ’s disease: A naturalistic follow-up study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 47 (2): 207–215. Lyketsos, C. G., J. M. Sheppard, C. D. Steele, S. Kopunek, M. Steinberg, A. S. Baker, J. Brandt, and P. V. Rabins. 2000. Randomized, placebo-controlled, doubleblind clinical trial of sertraline in the treatment of depression complicating Alzheimer ’s disease: Initial results from the depression in Alzheimer ’s disease study. Am J Psychiatry 157 (10): 1686–1689. Maidment, I. D., C. G. Fox, M. Boustani, J. Rodriguez, R. C. Brown, and C. L. Katona. 2008. Efficacy of memantine on behavioral and psychological symptoms related to dementia: A systematic meta-analysis. Ann Pharmacother 42 (1): 32–38. Malouf, R., and J. Grimley Evans. 2008. Folic acid with or without vitamin B12 for the prevention and treatment of healthy elderly and demented people. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (4): CD004514. Mayor, S. 2005. Regulatory authorities review use of galantamine in mild cognitive impairment. BMJ 330 (7486): 276. McGeer, P. L., and E. G. McGeer. 2007. Nsaids and Alzheimer disease: Epidemiological, animal model and clinical studies. Neurobiol Aging 28 (5): 639–647. McGuinness, B., S. Todd, P. Passmore, and R. Bullock. 2006. The effects of blood pressure lowering on development of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients without apparent prior cerebrovascular disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2): CD004034. Mega, M. S., J. L. Cummings, T. Fiorello, and J. Gornbein. 1996. The spectrum of behavioral changes in Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 46 (1): 130–135. Meinert, C. L., L. D. McCaffrey, and J. C. Breitner. 2009. Alzheimer ’s disease antiinflammatory prevention trial: Design, methods, and baseline results. Alzheimers Dement 5 (2): 93–104. Modrego, P. J., N. Fayed, J. M. Errea, C. Rios, M. A. Pina, and M. Sarasa. 2009. Memantine versus donepezil in mild to moderate Alzheimer ’s disease: A randomized trial with magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Eur J Neurol 17 (3): 405–412. Mohs, R. C., W. G. Rosen, and K. L. Davis. 1983. The Alzheimer ’s Disease Assessment Scale: An instrument for assessing treatment efficacy. Psychopharmacol Bull 19 (3): 448–450. Mowla, A., and A. Pani. 2010. Comparison of topiramate and risperidone for the treatment of behavioral disturbances of patients with Alzheimer disease: A double-blind, randomized clinical trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol 30 (1): 40–43.
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
227
Nagaratnam, N., I. Patel, and C. Whelan. 2003. Screaming, shrieking and muttering: The noise-makers amongst dementia patients. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 36 (3): 247–258. Neumann, P. J., R. C. Hermann, K. M. Kuntz, S. S. Araki, S. B. Duff, J. Leon, P. A. Berenbaum, P. A. Goldman, L. W. Williams, and M. C. Weinstein. 1999. Cost-effectiveness of donepezil in the treatment of mild or moderate Alzheimer ’s disease. Neurology 52 (6): 1138–1145. Nyth, A. L., and C. G. Gottfries. 1990. The clinical efficacy of citalopram in treatment of emotional disturbances in dementia disorders. A Nordic Multicentre Study. Br J Psychiatry 157: 894–901. Orgogozo, J. M., S. Gilman, J. F. Dartigues, B. Laurent, M. Puel, L. C. Kirby, P. Jouanny, et al. 2003. Subacute meningoencephalitis in a subset of patients with AD after Abeta42 immunization. Neurology 61 (1): 46–54. Pasqualetti, P., C. Bonomini, G. Dal Forno, L. Paulon, E. Sinforiani, C. Marra, O. Zanetti, and P. M. Rossini. 2009. A randomized controlled study on effects of ibuprofen on cognitive progression of Alzheimer ’s disease. Aging Clin Exp Res 21 (2): 102–110. Pelton, G. H., D. P. Devanand, K. Bell, K. Marder, K. Marston, X. Liu, and T. B. Cooper. 2003. Usefulness of plasma haloperidol levels for monitoring clinical efficacy and side effects in Alzheimer patients with psychosis and behavioral dyscontrol. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 11 (2): 186–193. Peskind, E. R., D. W. Tsuang, L. T. Bonner, M. Pascualy, R. G. Riekse, M. B. Snowden, R. Thomas, and M. A. Raskind. 2005. Propranolol for disruptive behaviors in nursing home residents with probable or possible Alzheimer disease: A placebo-controlled study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 19 (1): 23–28. Petersen, R. C., R. G. Thomas, M. Grundman, D. Bennett, R. Doody, S. Ferris, D. Galasko, et al. 2005. Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med 352 (23): 2379–2388. Porsteinsson, A. P., G. T. Grossberg, J. Mintzer, and J. T. Olin. 2008. Memantine treatment in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer ’s disease already receiving a cholinesterase inhibitor: A randomized, double-blind, placebocontrolled trial. Curr Alzheimer Res 5 (1): 83–89. Qaseem, A., V. Snow, J. T. Cross, Jr., M. A. Forciea, R. Hopkins, Jr., P. Shekelle, A. Adelman, et al. 2008. Current pharmacologic treatment of dementia: A clinical practice guideline from the American College of Physicians and the American Academy of Family Physicians. Ann Intern Med 148 (5): 370–378. Rapp, S. R., M. A. Espeland, S. A. Shumaker, V. W. Henderson, R. L. Brunner, J. E. Manson, M. L. Gass, et al. 2003. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: The Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 289 (20): 2663–2672. Raskind, M. A., E. R. Peskind, L. Truyen, P. Kershaw, and C. V. Damaraju. 2004. The cognitive benefits of galantamine are sustained for at least 36 months: A long-term extension trial. Arch Neurol 61 (2): 252–256.
228
Dementia
Reisberg, B., S. R. Auer, and I. M. Monteiro. 1996. Behavioral pathology in Alzheimer ’s Disease (Behave-AD) Rating Scale. Int Psychogeriatr 8 (Suppl. 3): 301–308; discussion 51–54. Reisberg, B., R. Doody, A. Stoffler, F. Schmitt, S. Ferris, and H. J. Mobius. 2003. Memantine in moderate-to-severe Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 348 (14): 1333–1341. Richard, E., A. A. Gouw, P. Scheltens, and W. A. van Gool. 2010. Vascular care in patients with Alzheimer disease with cerebrovascular lesions slows progression of white matter lesions on MRI: The evaluation of vascular care in Alzheimer ’s disease (EVA) Study. Stroke 41 (3): 554–556. Risse, S. C., T. H. Lampe, and L. Cubberley. 1987. Very low-dose neuroleptic treatment in two patients with agitation associated with Alzheimer ’s disease. J Clin Psychiatry 48 (5): 207–208. Rockwood, K., S. E. Black, A. Robillard, and I. Lussier. 2004. Potential treatment effects of donepezil not detected in Alzheimer ’s disease clinical trials: A physician survey. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 19 (10): 954–960. Rodda, J., S. Morgan, and Z. Walker. 2009. Are cholinesterase inhibitors effective in the management of the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in Alzheimer ’s disease? A systematic review of randomized, placebo-controlled trials of donepezil, rivastigmine and galantamine. Int Psychogeriatr 21 (5): 813–824. Rogers, S. L., M. R. Farlow, R. S. Doody, R. Mohs, and L. T. Friedhoff. 1998. A 24-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. Donepezil Study Group. Neurology 50 (1): 136–145. Rosenheck, R. A., D. L. Leslie, J. L. Sindelar, E. A. Miller, P. N. Tariot, K. S. Dagerman, S. M. Davis, et al. 2007. Cost-benefit analysis of second-generation antipsychotics and placebo in a randomized trial of the treatment of psychosis and aggression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64 (11): 1259–1268. Rosenthal, M., P. Berkman, A. Shapira, I. Gil, and J. Abramovitz. 2003. Urethral masturbation and sexual disinhibition in dementia: A case report. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 40 (1): 67–72. Rosler, M., R. Anand, A. Cicin-Sain, S. Gauthier, Y. Agid, P. Dal-Bianco, H. B. Stahelin, R. Hartman, and M. Gharabawi. 1999. Efficacy and safety of rivastigmine in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease: International randomised controlled trial. Bmj 318 (7184): 633–638. Rozzini, L., B. V. Chilovi, E. Bertoletti, D. Ghianda, M. Conti, M. Trabucchi, and A. Padovani. 2008. Serum albumin level interferes with the effect of donepezil in Alzheimer ’s disease. Aging Clin Exp Res 20 (6): 509–512. Sano, M., C. Ernesto, R. G. Thomas, M. R. Klauber, K. Schafer, M. Grundman, P. Woodbury, et al. 1997. A controlled trial of selegiline, alpha-tocopherol, or both as treatment for Alzheimer ’s disease. The Alzheimer ’s Disease Cooperative Study. N Engl J Med 336 (17): 1216–1222.
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
229
Santoro, A., P. Siviero, N. Minicuci, E. Bellavista, M. Mishto, F. Olivieri, F. Marchegiani, et al. 2010. Effects of donepezil, galantamine and rivastigmine in 938 Italian patients with Alzheimer ’s disease: A prospective, observational study. CNS Drugs 24 (2): 163–176. Schneider, L. S., K. S. Dagerman, and P. Insel. 2005. Risk of death with atypical antipsychotic drug treatment for dementia: Meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. JAMA 294 (15): 1934–1943. Schneider, L. S., and J. T. Olin. 1996. Clinical global impressions in Alzheimer ’s clinical trials. Int Psychogeriatr 8 (2): 277–288; discussion 88–90. Schneider, L. S., P. N. Tariot, K. S. Dagerman, S. M. Davis, J. K. Hsiao, M. S. Ismail, B. D. Lebowitz, et al. 2006. Effectiveness of atypical antipsychotic drugs in patients with Alzheimer ’s disease. N Engl J Med 355 (15): 1525–1538. Shankle, W. R., K. A. Nielson, and C. W. Cotman. 1995. Low-dose propranolol reduces aggression and agitation resembling that associated with orbitofrontal dysfunction in elderly demented patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 9 (4): 233–237. Shelton, P. S., and L. B. Hocking. 1997. Zolpidem for dementia-related insomnia and nighttime wandering. Ann Pharmacother 31 (3): 319–322. Sink, K. M., K. F. Holden, and K. Yaffe. 2005. Pharmacological treatment of neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia: A review of the evidence. JAMA 293 (5): 596–608. Snowdon, D. A., L. H. Greiner, J. A. Mortimer, K. P. Riley, P. A. Greiner, and W. R. Markesbery. 1997. Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease. The Nun Study. JAMA 277 (10): 813–817. Sommer, O. H., O. Aga, M. Cvancarova, I. C. Olsen, G. Selbaek, and K. Engedal. 2009. Effect of oxcarbazepine in the treatment of agitation and aggression in severe dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 27 (2): 155–163. Steele, C., M. J. Lucas, and L. Tune. 1986. Haloperidol versus thioridazine in the treatment of behavioral symptoms in senile dementia of the Alzheimer ’s type: Preliminary findings. J Clin Psychiatry 47 (6): 310–312. Streim, J. E., A. P. Porsteinsson, C. D. Breder, R. Swanink, R. Marcus, R. McQuade, and W. H. Carson. 2008. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of aripiprazole for the treatment of psychosis in nursing home patients with Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 16 (7): 537–550. Sun, Y., C. J. Lu, K. L. Chien, S. T. Chen, and R. C. Chen. 2007. Efficacy of multivitamin supplementation containing vitamins B6 and B12 and folic acid as adjunctive treatment with a cholinesterase inhibitor in Alzheimer ’s disease: A 26-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in Taiwanese patients. Clin Ther 29 (10): 2204–2214. Taragano, F. E., C. G. Lyketsos, C. A. Mangone, R. F. Allegri, and E. ComesanaDiaz. 1997. A double-blind, randomized, fixed-dose trial of fluoxetine vs. amitriptyline in the treatment of major depression complicating Alzheimer ’s disease. Psychosomatics 38 (3): 246–252.
230
Dementia
Tariot, P. N., M. R. Farlow, G. T. Grossberg, S. M. Graham, S. McDonald, and I. Gergel. 2004. Memantine treatment in patients with moderate to severe Alzheimer disease already receiving donepezil: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 291 (3): 317–324. Teri, L., R. G. Logsdon, E. Peskind, M. Raskind, M. F. Weiner, R. E. Tractenberg, N. L. Foster, et al. 2000. Treatment of agitation in AD: A randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Neurology 55 (9): 1271–1278. Tosto, G., G. Talarico, G. L. Lenzi, and G. Bruno. 2008. Effect of citalopram in treating hypersexuality in an Alzheimer ’s disease case. Neurol Sci 29 (4): 269–270. Trinh, N. H., J. Hoblyn, S. Mohanty, and K. Yaffe. 2003. Efficacy of cholinesterase inhibitors in the treatment of neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment in Alzheimer disease: A meta-analysis. JAMA 289 (2): 210–216. van Dyck, C. H., F. A. Schmitt, and J. T. Olin. 2006. A responder analysis of memantine treatment in patients with Alzheimer disease maintained on donepezil. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 14 (5): 428–437. Vellas, B., R. Black, L. J. Thal, N. C. Fox, M. Daniels, G. McLennan, C. Tompkins, C. Leibman, M. Pomfret, and M. Grundman. 2009. Long-term follow-up of patients immunized with An1792: Reduced functional decline in antibody responders. Curr Alzheimer Res 6 (2): 144–151. Waldemar, G., M. Hyvarinen, M. K. Josiassen, A. Korner, H. Lehto, and P. Wetterberg. 2008. Tolerability of switching from donepezil to memantine treatment in patients with moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 23 (9): 979–981. Wei, Z., D. D. Mousseau, Y. Dai, X. Cao, and X. M. Li. 2006. Haloperidol induces apoptosis via the Sigma2 receptor system and Bcl-Xs. Pharmacogenomics J 6 (4): 279–288. Weiler, P. G., D. Mungas, and C. Bernick. 1988. Propranolol for the control of disruptive behavior in senile dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1 (4): 226–230. Weintraub, D., P. B. Rosenberg, L. T. Drye, B. K. Martin, C. Frangakis, J. E. Mintzer, A. P. Porsteinsson, et al. 2010. Sertraline for the treatment of depression in Alzheimer disease: Week-24 outcomes. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 18 (4): 332–340. Wilcock, G. K., S. Lilienfeld, and E. Gaens. 2000. Efficacy and safety of galantamine in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer ’s disease: Multicentre randomised controlled trial. Galantamine International-1 Study Group. BMJ 321 (7274): 1445–1449. Winblad, B., K. Engedal, H. Soininen, F. Verhey, G. Waldemar, A. Wimo, A. L. Wetterholm, R. Zhang, A. Haglund, and P. Subbiah. 2001. A 1-year, randomized, placebo-controlled study of donepezil in patients with mild to moderate AD. Neurology 57 (3): 489–495. Winblad, B., G. Grossberg, L. Frolich, M. Farlow, S. Zechner, J. Nagel, and R. Lane. 2007. Ideal: A 6-month, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the first skin patch for Alzheimer disease. Neurology 69 (4 Suppl 1): S14–S22.
Neuropsychiatric Treatments in Alzheimer’s Disease
231
Winblad, B., R. W. Jones, Y. Wirth, A. Stoffler, and H. J. Mobius. 2007. Memantine in moderate to severe Alzheimer ’s disease: A meta-analysis of randomised clinical trials. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 24 (1): 20–27. Winblad, B., L. Kilander, S. Eriksson, L. Minthon, S. Batsman, A. L. Wetterholm, C. Jansson-Blixt, and A. Haglund. 2006. Donepezil in patients with severe Alzheimer ’s disease: Double-blind, parallel-group, placebo-controlled study. Lancet 367 (9516): 1057–1065. Winblad, B., and N. Poritis. 1999. Memantine in severe dementia: Results of the 9m-Best Study (benefit and efficacy in severely demented patients during treatment with memantine). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 14 (2): 135–146. Zheng, L., W. J. Mack, K. S. Dagerman, J. K. Hsiao, B. D. Lebowitz, C. G. Lyketsos, T. S. Stroup, et al. 2009. Metabolic changes associated with second-generation antipsychotic use in Alzheimer ’s disease patients: The Catie-AD study. Am J Psychiatry 166 (5): 583–590. Zhou, B., S. Teramukai, and M. Fukushima. 2007. Prevention and treatment of dementia or Alzheimer ’s disease by statins: A meta-analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 23 (3): 194–201.
About the Contributors
FRÉDÉRIC ASSAL received his MD in 1987, performed basic research from 1988 to 1991, and completed his medical thesis in neurobiology (cortical plasticity in the visual system). He received clinical neurology training including psychiatry, neurosurgery, and internal medicine in Paris and Geneva (1991–1997) and performed a Dementia and Behavioral Neuroscience Fellowship (2000–2002) at UCLA, Los Angeles. He obtained a Privat Docent thesis-academic position in 2007. Dr. Assal is currently an attending neurologist in the Department of Neurology, Geriatrics, and Geropsychiatry and at the Memory Clinic in the Geneva University Hospitals in Geneva, Switzerland. His clinical interests are cognitive and behavioral neurology, neurology of aging, movement disorders, and neuropsychiatry. His major research interests include cognitive and neuropsychiatric aspects of dementias and related disorders, gait and cognitive relationship in dementias, and cognitive and behavioral neurology (case studies). He is currently funded for two three-year projects, one on memory and mild cognitive impairment using functional MRI (as a principal investigator) and the second on electrophysiological markers in Alzheimer ’s disease (as a main investigator). He also engages in teaching activities to medical students, neuropsychologists, and residents. ROSANNA M. BERTRAND, PhD, is a cognitive gerontologist with methodological and analytic experience in long-term care research. Dr. Bertrand’s skills include all aspects of the design, implementation, and analysis of qualitative and quantitative research as well as the development and implementation of evaluation studies. She is currently an associate at Abt Associates, Inc., where her focus has been on studies that assess quality of care and clinical practices. Prior to joining Abt, Dr. Bertrand was on the faculty at Boston
234
About the Contributors
University’s School of Public Health where she was the principal investigator and co-principal investigator on several National Institute on Aging grants that examined the physical and psychological effects of caregiving on older adults including the impact of caregiving stress on cognitive outcomes. JISKA COHEN-MANSFIELD is Professor and Chair of the Department of Health Promotion at the School of Public Health and the Head of the Herczeg Institute on Aging at Tel-Aviv University. She is also Professor of Health Care Sciences and of Prevention and Community Health at the George Washington University Medical Center and School of Public Health. Dr. Cohen-Mansfield’s work focuses on improving quality of life for persons with dementia by understanding the perspective of the person with dementia, on end-of-life decisionmaking and on health and mental health promotion in older persons. She has published over 260 publications in scientific books and journals, for which she is a highly cited researcher as listed by the ISI. She has developed a number of assessment tools and treatment approaches which are used internationally. DOROTHY FORBES, PhD, RN, is an Associate Professor at the Arthur Labatt Family School of Nursing, University of Western Ontario. Her research program focuses on enhancing the quality of life for community-dwelling persons with dementia and their care partners in rural and urban Canada. Currently, Dr. Forbes is disseminating the results of a national study on the Role of Home Care in Dementia Care, conducting four Cochrane Reviews on interventions that manage the symptoms of dementia, and examining the influence of gender on social support among home-care clients using interRAI data. Dr. Forbes is a co-investigator on several projects: the evaluation of the CHSRF EXTRA program (Malcolm Anderson, PI); relationships among home-care providers, persons with dementia, and family caregivers (Cathy Ward-Griffin, PI); healthcare providers’ needs in relation to behavioral issues in long-term care settings (Kristie Clark, PI); enhancing rehabilitation across sectors through better use of health information (Paul Stolee, PI); and the effects and costs of a nurseled interprofessional mental-health promotion intervention for depressed older adults using home-support services (Maureen Markle-Reid, PI). Dr. Forbes is a member of the management committee of the Canadian Dementia and Knowledge Translation Network, previous president of the Canadian Association for Nursing Research, and Adjunct Professor, College of Graduate Studies and Research, University of Saskatchewan. She has published extensively and presented her research at international, national, and local conferences and workshops.
About the Contributors
235
KARAN KVERNO is an Assistant Professor at the University of Maryland where she enjoys teaching in the Psychiatric Mental Health Nurse Practitioner Program. She has a PhD in experimental psychology from the George Washington University and a master ’s degree in psychosocial nursing from the University of Washington. She is dually certified by the American Nurses Credentialing Center as a psychiatric mental health clinical specialist and nurse practitioner and has practiced in psychiatric mental health nursing since 1985. In 2007, Dr. Kverno was awarded the first Blaustein Postdoctoral Fellowship in Psychiatric Nursing Research sponsored by The Johns Hopkins University School of Nursing and Johns Hopkins Medical Institution Department of Psychiatry. Her recent research and publications focus on improving nonpharmacological treatments for the distressing psychiatric and behavioral symptoms associated with dementia. Her interests are in improving the quality of life for persons with dementia, particularly those with advanced illness. PAMELA LINDSEY, DNSc, RN, is Assistant Professor at Illinois State University Mennonite College of Nursing. She was a 2007–2009 Claire Fagin Postdoctoral Fellow (sponsored by the John A. Hartford Foundation and the Atlantic Philanthropies). She earned a BS in psychology from Illinois State University, a diploma in nursing from Mennonite School of Nursing, an MS in psychiatric nursing from University of Illinois at Chicago, and a DNSc at Rush University College of Nursing. Her dissertation was entitled “The Relationship of Organizational Factors and Psychiatric Nurses’ Decision to Restrain.” Her current research focuses on how psychiatric nurses manage behavior in older adults with dementia. She recently completed a pilot study aimed at understanding pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic interventions and the decisionmaking process that psychiatric nurses use in the management of behavioral symptoms exhibited by older adults hospitalized on acute care geropsychiatric units. Dr. Lindsey plans future work aimed at the development and implementation of evidence-based protocols focused on behavioral management of older adults with dementia. ROSA LIPEROTI received her MD in 1998 from Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore (UCSC) in Rome, Italy. In 2002, she specialized in geriatrics at UCSC and received her MPH from Brown University School of Public Health in Providence, RI. She is currently a Professor of Epidemiology and Biostatics at the School of Occupational Therapy, UCSC, Rome. Her field of research is the pharmaco-epidemiology of dementia with a special focus on the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD).
236
About the Contributors
She is the author of numerous papers that have contributed to the current debate on the pharmacological treatment of BPSD. She is a member of the Italian Society of Gerontology and Geriatrics, the European Academy for Medicine of Aging, the European Alzheimer ’s Disease Consortium, the Italian Psychogeriatric Association, and the European Union Geriatric Medicine Society. She is the reviewer for many peer-reviewed journals including Archives of General Psychiatry, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, CNS Drugs, Drug Safety, and Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. DEBRA MORGAN, PhD, RN, is a Professor at Canadian Centre for Health and Safety in Agriculture, University of Saskatchewan, where she also holds a College of Medicine Chair in Rural Health Delivery. Her research program focuses on health-service delivery for rural and northern seniors with dementia and their formal and informal caregivers, from early stage community-based care to later stage facility-based care. She held a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) New Emerging Team grant from 2003 to2009 to lead an interdisciplinary research team in developing strategies to improve the care of persons with dementia in rural and remote areas. The team’s flagship project was the implementation and evaluation of a one-stop interdisciplinary Rural and Remote Memory Clinic that incorporates the use of telehealth videoconferencing for preclinic assessment and follow-up. The clinic continues to serve as the focal point for numerous related projects conducted by co-investigators and graduate students. Dr. Morgan currently holds an Applied Chair in Health Services in Policy Research funded by CIHR and the Saskatchewan Health Research Foundation, titled Healthcare Services Across the Continuum for Rural and Remote Seniors with Dementia (2009–2014). Dr. Morgan is Saskatchewan Provincial Lead for the CIHR-funded Translating Research in Eldercare (TREC) study led by Dr. Carole Estabrooks, Canada Research Chair in Knowledge Translation, University of Alberta. The TREC study is being conducted in the three prairie provinces in Canada and is examining the role of organizational context on the use of best practices in longterm care settings. JANE S. SACZYNSKI is an Assistant Professor of Medicine in the Division of Geriatric Medicine at the University of Massachusetts Medical School and an investigator at the Meyers Primary Care Institute. She is a neuroepidemiologist with research interests in cardiovascular disease and the risk for cognitive decline and dementia in middle aged and older adults. Within large-scale epidemiologic studies, she has looked at how
About the Contributors
237
cardiovascular diseases, such as diabetes and dyslipidemia, are related to cognitive impairment, brain changes, and dementia. PHILIPPE TAUPIN earned his PhD in neurosciences from the Université Pierre et Marie Curie in France. He did his postdoctoral studies in the laboratory of Professor F. H. Gage at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies in San Diego. After completing his postdoctoral studies, he went on to take the position of Head of Laboratory and Associate Professor and to set up his own research program and laboratory in Singapore. Recently, he was appointed as scientific director of a newly created research institute and as a Professor at Dublin City University, Ireland. He is currently editor-in-chief of the Journal of Neurodegeneration and Regeneration. He has been engaged as an entrepreneur in setting up a biotechnology company to bring his expertise and experience to the pharmaceutical and biotechnology sector. EMMELYNE VASSE is a junior researcher and PhD candidate at the Scientific Institute for Quality of Healthcare (IQ Healthcare) and Alzheimer Center at Radboud University, Nijmegen Medical Center in Nijmegen, The Netherlands. IQ Healthcare is one of the leading centers for health services research related to quality improvement in health care in Europe. Her area of expertise is psychosocial interventions for people with dementia and their carers and the development of quality indicators for dementia care. She was involved in the EuroCode project, which was funded by the European Commission. The aim of this project was to develop a European network of all players active in the area of dementia to jointly develop consensual guidelines and indicators for dementia care. At a national level, she was involved in a project for The Netherlands Health Care Inspectorate in which indicators for dementia diagnostics and treatment were developed based on a clinical logic for dementia. MYRRA VERNOOIJ-DASSEN is a medical sociologist at the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre (RUNMC). At RUNMC, she is director of the Alzheimer Centre and affiliate to the Scientific Institute for Quality of Healthcare and the Department of Primary and Community Care: Centre for Family Medicine, Geriatric Care and Public Health. She has a chair in nursing home medicine on psychosocial aspects of care for frail elderly persons, funded by the Kalorama Foundation and RUNMC. She is principal investigator of the Nijmegen Centre for Evidence Based Practice of RUNMC. She is also chair of the European Interdem group. Interdem is
238
About the Contributors
a pan-European research group on detection and timely INTERvention in DEMentia. She is an honorary visiting Professor at Bradford University. In addition, Dr. Vernooij-Dassen has a broad methodological interest ranging from qualitative research to implementation of effective psychosocial interventions. Study subjects are integrated care, especially with regard to dementia and palliative care.
About the Series Editor
PATRICK MCNAMARA, PhD, is Associate Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry at Boston University School of Medicine (BUSM) and is Director of the Evolutionary Neurobehavior Laboratory in the Department of Neurology at the BUSM and the VA New England Healthcare System. Upon graduating from the Behavioral Neuroscience Program at Boston University in 1991, he trained at the Aphasia Research Center at the Boston VA Medical Center in neurolinguistics and brain-cognitive correlation techniques. He then began developing an evolutionary approach to problems of brain and behavior and currently is studying the evolution of the frontal lobes, the evolution of the two mammalian sleep states (REM and NREM) and the evolution of religion in human cultures.
Index
abnormal vocalizations, 218 acetophenazine, 216 acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, 103, 137 activity therapy, 80–81 advanced dementia, 44, 44–45 (table), 46; neuropsychiatric symptoms and, 46, 47 (table), 50–52, 54–56; pain assessment and, 53 (table); quality-of-life and, 48 (table), 49 affective disorders, 77 age, 183, 185 AGE program, 9 aggressive behavior, 2, 52, 55; contributing factors, 96; toward caregivers, 77–78 aging, 24–25 Agitated Behavior Rating scale (ABRS), 30, 31 agitation, 52, 55, 73, 74–75, 78, 103. See also behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) akathisia, 97 Alzheimer, Alois, 93 Alzheimer ’s Disease, 137, 205–206; behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and, 78; depression, comorbidity and, 173, 178, 179, 184–185
(see also depression); functional assessment of, 44 (table); light therapy and, 34, 35, 36; neurogenesis and, 138, 139 (table); pharmaceuticals and, 137–138, 208–210 (table), 211–219, 220–222; quality-oflife and, 48 (table); stem cell treatment and, 144 American Association of Geriatric Psychiatry (AAGP), 50 amisulpride, 99 Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL), 184 animal testing: epilepsy and, 139 (table), 142; lithium and, 218; neurogenesis and, 135, 136, 141–142, 143 antidepressants, 55, 86, 210 (table), 216–217; neurogenesis and, 138, 139 (table), 141, 142 anti-epileptic medication, 217 antihypertensive drugs, 215 anti-inflammatory drugs, 214 antipsychotics: atypical, 83–84, 98–100, 101–102 (table), 103–106, 108, 209 (table), 216; efficacy of, 99–100, 101–102 (table), 103–104; pharmacology of,
242
Index
96, 97 (table), 97–99; safety of, 105–108; side effects, 97–98, 100, 105, 108; typical, 82, 85, 97–98, 99–101, 103–107, 209 (table), 215–216 anxiety, 51, 76 apathy, 32, 51, 76 apolipoprotein ε4 (Apo ε4), 190–191 aripiprazole, 84, 99, 102 (table), 104 aromatherapy, 6, 58–59. See also sensory interventions assessment tools: behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and, 74–75, 78–79; caregivers and, 78–79; depression and, 179; depression vs. dementia and, 175–177. See also rating scale atypical antipsychotics, 83–84, 98–100, 101–102 (table), 103–106, 108, 209 (table), 216 audio recording, 57 autopsy, 142 Balancing Arousal Controls Excesses (BACE) model, 54 behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD), 74; behavioral symptoms of, 74–75; caregiver impact and, 74, 87–89; diagnosis of, 78–79; etiology of, 95–96; management of, 77–86; nonpharmacological treatment of, 80–82, 94; pharmacological treatment of, 83–86, 206–207, 208–210 (table), 211–219 (see also antipsychotics); prevalence of, 77–78; psychological symptoms of, 75–78; psychosocial intervention and, 157–159. See also behavioral symptoms
behavioral interventions, 8, 158, 194. See also nonpharmacological intervention Behavioral Pathologic Rating Scale for Alzheimer ’s disease (BEHAVE-AD), 30, 79 behavioral symptoms, 74–75, 156–159, 163. See also behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (NPSD); inappropriate behavior; specific behaviors behavior-oriented therapy, 57–58 benzamides, 99 benzodiazepines, 86, 210 (table), 217 black box warnings, 83 brain-derived neurotrophic factors (BDNF), 189–190 brain structure, 177 Brief Multidimensional Measure of Religiousness/Spirituality (BMMRS), 128 bright-light therapy, 7, 28–29. See light therapy Brite-Lite box, 26, 35 bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU), 138 Capgras syndrome, 77, 219 carbemazepine, 85, 210 (table) caregiver: accounts in patient diagnosis, 178; behavior assessment tools and, 78–79; behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and, 74, 86–89; elicited response and, 57; family, 159–162; professional, 162–163; psychosocial intervention and, 156, 159–163; techniques, 80. See also caregiver training; nonpharmaceutical interventions caregiver training, 8, 158, 194 care plan, 163–165 catastrophic reactions, 75
Index cerebrovascular events (CVEs), 105–106, 107 ChEIns, 212, 213–214, 219–220 chlorpromazine, 216 Choices, Attitudes, and Strategies for Care of Advanced Dementia at End-of-Life (CASCADE) study, 46 cholinesterase inhibitors, 85, 207, 211–212, 213 chromosomal abnormalities, 95 chronic inflammation, 189 circadian rhythms, 24, 25, 35 citalopram, 86, 210 (table), 217, 219 Clinical Anitpsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness– Alzheimer ’s Disease (CATIE-AD), 104 Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), 44 (table) Clinical Global Impressions of Change (CGIC), 205 clinical guidelines, 82–83, 94, 109 (table), 152–153, 157 clinical studies. See research studies clozapine, 85, 98, 209 (table), 216 cognition-oriented therapy, 56–57 cognitive impairment, 44, 52, 155–156. See also specific dementia forms cognitive interventions, 6, 81 cognitive stimulation therapy (CST), 6 Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (CMAI), 30, 74–75, 78–79 collaborative care model, 162 communication problems, 178 comorbidity, 173, 175, 186, 193 complaining, 75 Comprehensive Process Model of Engagement of Person’s with Dementia (CPME-D), 61–62 Consensus Conference on Behavioral Disturbances of Dementia, 73–74 coping strategies, 156; religious, 120–121, 125, 128; stress and,
243
152. See also decoupling model; psychosocial intervention Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia (CSDD), 32, 179, 180 Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, 78 Dawn-Dusk Simulator, 26 decoupling model, 119–120, 121, 123 (figure), 123–125; Parkinson’s disease and, 126–127 delusions, 76–77, 78 dementia. See advanced dementia; Alzheimer ’s Disease (AD); dementia; frontotemporal dementia; Parkinson’s disease (PD); vascular dementia (VaD) Dementia Care Mapping, 8, 48 (table) dementia guidelines, 152–153, 157. See also clinical guidelines dementia with Lewy bodies, 78, 85 demographic characteristics, 180–184 depression, 51, 187–188, 198, 220– 221; consequences of, 191; dementia comorbidity and, 173, 178, 179, 184–185, 189–191; demographic characteristics, 182–184; diagnosis, 76, 170–180; light therapy and, 31–32; neurogenesis and, 138, 139 (table), 140, 142; nonpharmocological intervention and, 158; pharmaceuticals and, 138–140, 141 (see also antidepressants); prevalence rate, 173–175; as a risk factor for dementia, 185–188; risk factors, 184–185; treatment, 138–140, 141, 193–195 desipramine, 138 dilvalproex, 85 disinhibition, 75 donepezil, 85, 208 (table), 211, 212, 221 dopamine receptor, 95–97, 98–99
244
Index
dysphoric symptoms, 51 dystonia, 97 education, 183–184 emotion-oriented therapy, 57 environmental modifications, 7, 81 epidemiologic studies, 181 epilepsy, 139 (table), 142, 217 estrogen replacement therapy, 214 extrapyramidal symptoms (EPS), 84, 85, 97, 98, 105 family caregivers, 159–162 family history, 184–185 5-hydroxytryptamine. See serotonin fluoxetine, 138, 140, 141 Food and Drug Administration (FDA), 99 Fregoli syndrome, 77 frontal lobes, 124 frontotemporal dementia, 127 function limitations, 27, 28 (figure) Functional Assessment of Alzheimer ’s Disease (FAST), 44 (table) gabapentin, 210 (table), 217 galantamine, 85, 138, 208 (table), 211, 220 gender, 182–183 generative self, 121. See also self-concept genetic polymorphism, 95–96 genetics, 190–191, 221 Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), 32, 179, 180 geriatric-palliative models, 55 ginko biloba, 214 Global Deterioration Scale (GDS), 45 (table) glucocorticoids, 140 Godot syndrome, 76 group cognitive stimulation therapy, 156 hallucinations, 77, 95, 212 haloperidol, 100, 103, 215, 216, 219, 221 Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD), 179
healthcare professionals, 163 hippocampus, 135, 136, 141, 142 Honolulu-Asia Aging Study, 188, 190 Huntington’s chorea, 78 Huntington’s disease, 139 (table), 142–143 ideal self, 122–123, 124, 125 imipramine, 138 inappropriate behavior, 1–2, 4, 7, 8; light therapy and, 30–31; pain and, 54; psychosocial intervention and, 156–159, 163. See also behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (NPSD) nonpharmocological interventions; specific behaviors inhibitory tasks, 124 insomnia, 218 institutionalization, 160–161 Intermetamorphosis, 77 International Psychogeriatric Association (IPA), 73–74 intrusiveness, 75 lifestyle, 191 light therapy, 25–27, 28 (figure), 28–37 lithium, 218 Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam (LASA), 184 long-term care facilities, 83 massage/touch therapy, 5. See also sensory interventions medical interventions, 8–9 melatonergic antagonist, 138, 140 memantine, 86, 138, 208 (table), 212–214, 219–220 memory, 153–154 mentoring, 8 metabolic effects, 84, 85 mild cognitive impairment (MCI), 186 Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), 26–27, 44, 45 (table)
Index mirtazapine, 217 misidentification, 77 Mobilization-Observation-BehaviorIntensity-Dementia Pain Scale (MOBID), 53 (table) monoamine oxidase inhibitors, 138 mood disorders, 77 mood stabilizers, 85 mortality, 191–192 motor symptoms, 126. See also parkinsonism multisensory stimulation (MSS), 6, 60, 62–64, 81 music therapy, 5, 7, 9, 59–60. See also sensory interventions namaste, 63–64 narratives, 122 National Institute of Mental Health diagnosis of depression in Alzheimer ’s disease (NIMHdAD), 179–180 Need-Driven Dementia-Compromised Behavior model, 54 negativism, 75 neural stem cells (NSCs), 135, 136, 143–144 neurogenesis, 135, 136, 137, 139 (table), 140–141, 144–145; Alzheimer ’s disease and, 138, 139 (table); depression and, 138–140; pharmacology of, 137–145; regenerative process and, 141–144. See also regenerative process neuroleptic malignant syndrome, 85 neuronal cells, 136, 140, 141. See also neurogenesis; regenerative process Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), 30, 31, 79, 205 Neuropsychiatric Inventory–Nursing Home (NPI-NH), 31–32
245
neuropsychiatric symptoms, 46, 47 (table), 50 neuropsychological test performance, 175–176 neurotransmitters, 95 N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamate receptor antagonist, 86, 137–138, 212–213 noncognitive changes, 73–74, 93–94. See also behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) nonpharmacological interventions, 2, 23–24, 50, 55–56, 83, 94; affect of, 10–11; behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and, 80–82, 94; depression and, 195; funding for, 82; implementation barriers of, 11–13; importance of, 2–4; individual tailoring of, 9–10; neuropsychiatric symptoms and, 43–45, 56–64; principles of care, 50–52, 54–56; types of, 4–9. See also psychosocial intervention norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, 138 Nurses-Informant Activities of Daily Living (NI-ADU), 27 nursing interventions, 8–9 occupational therapy, 156 olanzapine, 84, 98, 100, 101 (figure), 103, 104, 105, 209 (table), 216, 219 Omnibus Budget Reconciliation Act (OBRA), 2, 49 orthostatic hypotension, 84 oxcarbazepine, 217 Pain Assessment Checklist for Seniors with Limited Ability to Communicate (PACSLAC), 53 (table)
246
Index
Pain Assessment in Advanced Dementia, 53 (table) pain assessment tools, 53 (table) parkinsonism, 97, 103, 105, 219 Parkinson’s disease, 85, 125–127 patient perspective, 157 Patients with Alzheimer ’s Disease and Other Dementias (American Psychiatric Association), 50 person-centered care, 3, 8, 49–50, 56, 64, 163 pet therapy, 6, 9 phantom boarders, 77 pharmaceuticals, 217–219; acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, 103, 137; Alzheimer ’s disease and, 137–138; antidepressants, 55, 86, 138, 139 (table), 141, 210 (table), 216–217; antiepileptics, 217; antipsychotics, typical, 85, 97–98, 99–101, 103–107, 209 (table), 215–216 (see also atypical antipsychotics); atypical antipsychotics, 83–85, 98–100, 101–102 (table), 103–106, 108, 209 (table), 216; benzodiazepines, 86, 217; cholinesterase inhibitors, 85, 207, 211–212, 213; depression and, 138–140, 194; mood stabilizers, 85; neurogenesis and, 137–141, 144–145; NMDA receptor antagonists, 86, 137–138; psychotropic, 54–55. See also pharmacological treatment; specific psychotropic medication pharmacological treatment: behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) and, 82–86; neurogenesis and, 137–141, 144–145; prescribing practice and, 82–83; side effects, 84–85, 97–98, 100, 105, 108, 216. See also pharmaceuticals
Philadelphia Geriatric Center Affect Rating Scale (PGC-ARS), 48 (table) phobia, 76 phosphodiesterase-IV inhibitor, 138 physical aggression. See aggressive behavior positron emission tomography (PET), 34 possible selves, 122–123. See also ideal self post-mortem studies, 142 prescribing practice, 82–83 presenilin 1, 95 principals of care, 50–52, 54–56 problem-solving, 158 professional caregivers, 162–163 Progressive Lower Stress Threshold (PLST) model, 54 propranol, 218 psychiatric disturbances, 31–32 psychiatrists, 83 psychological symptoms, 75–78. See also behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BSPD) psychosis, 51, 95–96, 103 psychosocial intervention, 15, 155 (table); behavioral symptoms and, 156–159; caregivers and, 156, 159–163; cognitive impairment and, 155–156; in dementia care, 152–154; personalization of, 153, 161, 163–164 psychotherapy, 82 psychotropic medication, 54–55 quality-of-life, 48 (table), 49, 74, 94 Quality of Life in Late Stage Dementia (QUALID), 48 (table), 49 quetiapine, 84, 98, 101–102 (table), 103, 209 (table), 216 QUILIDEM, 48 (table)
Index rage reactions, 75 randomized clinical trials (RCTs), 99–101, 103–104, 106–107 rating scales, 74–75, 78–79 reality orientation, 6, 81 reboxetine, 138 regenerative medicine, 143–144 regenerative process, 134, 141–144 religiosity, 119–120; case studies, 127–128; health and, 120–121 religious cognition models, 119, 121, 128. See also decoupling model religious coping, 120–121, 125, 128. See also decoupling model religious practice, 120–121, 123–124, 125 reminiscence, 81 reminiscence therapy, 11 research studies: light therapy and, 25–27, 28 (figure), 28–30, 37–38 (table); methods, 181; religiosity in dementia and, 127–128; risk factors for dementia and, 182–183 respite videos, 7 risk factors, 181, 185–188 risperidone, 84, 98, 100, 101 (figure), 103, 104, 105, 209 (table), 216 rivastigmine, 85, 103, 208 (table), 212 rolipram, 138 schizophrenia, 100, 108 screening tools. See assessment tools sedation, 84, 85, 86 selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), 86, 138, 140, 141, 216–217 self-concept, 121, 124 self-control. See self-regulation Self-Identity Questionnaire (SIQ), 10 self integration, 125 self-regulation, 121, 122–125 sensory interventions, 5–6. See also aromatherapy; massage/touch therapy; music; snoezelen
247
serotonin (5-HT), 95–96, 138, 140 serotoninergic antogonist, 140 sertraline, 210 (table), 217 sexual behavior, 219 side effects, 84–85; antipsychotics and, 97–98, 100, 105, 108, 216 sleep, 28–30, 34–35, 81–82. See also light therapy sleep latency, 28, 30 snoezelen, 6, 62–63, 81. See also sensory interventions social intervention, 6–7. See also pet therapy Spouse-Caregiver Intervention Study, 160 staff training, 8 stage-specific therapy, 50, 65 stem cell regenerative treatment, 143–144 stimulated presence therapy, 7 stimulation, 7–8 stimulation-oriented therapy, 58–64 Stimulation-Retreat model, 54 stress coping, 152 structured activities, 7–8 subventricular zone (SVZ), 136, 143 sulpiride, 99 sundowning, 24, 25 suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), 24, 34, 36 syndromal patterns, 51 tardive dyskinesia, 97–98, 105 temporal discounting, 125 therapeutic activities, 60 thioridazine, 100, 216 thioxanthene, 216 topiramate, 217 touch therapy, 60 tranylcypromine, 138 trazodone, 210 (table), 217–218 treatment. See nonpharmacological intervention; pharmacological treatment tricyclic antidepressants, 138, 217, 219
248
Index
unmet needs, 52 validation therapy, 11, 81 valproate, 217 vascular change, 189 vascular dementia (VaD): behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and, 77; depression and, 187–188; light therapy and, 34 venlafaxine, 217
venous thromboembolism (VTE), 108 verbal outbursts, 77 vitamin E, 214–215 wandering, 74 weight gain, 84, 85 zeitgebers, 24, 34 ziprasidone, 99 zolpidem, 218