A Dissertation on Natural Phonology
David Stampe
ED Garland
Publishing,
Inc. • New York & London 1979
CONTENTS
Preface
Library
of Congress
Stampe, David, A dissertation
Cataloging
1938on natural
in Publication
Data
The
v
Acquisition
o
A Dissertation
on
Phonetic ' c Representation Natural
Vii
Phonology
xxvi
phonology.
(Outstanding dissertations in linguistics ; 22) Originally presented as the author's thesis, University of Chicago, 1973. Bibliography: p. Includes index. 1. Grammar, Comparative and general-Phonology. 2. Psycholinguistics. 3. Language acquisition. I. Title. II. Series. P217.3.57 1979 414 78-66538 ISBN 0-8240-9674-6
Chapter
T:
The
Nature
and
Chapter
II:
The
Organization
Function of
References
Process
Processes
1 43 11
Afterthoughts Additional
of
75 References
84
Q 1979 David Stampe All rights reserved
All volumes
in this series are printed on acid-free, 250-year-life paper. Printed in the United States of America
sss
PREFACE The minor
present
edition
corrections
submitted
to
in December 1972
but
of my dissertation without
the
University
1973.
The
version
distributed
pagination
changes,
Chicago
Department
of
latter
has
was slightly
under
the
title
been
retyped,
from
the
with version
of Linguistics
abridged
from
How I Spent
a September
my Sunrmer
Vacation. The 1969
paper
Regional
reprinted the
readers
The added
Meeting
here
the
exhaustive.
Phonetic
Chicago
minor
Representation,
Linguistic
Society,
corrections
the
from is
as a convenience
discussion
in
the
to
dissertation
paper.
here changes For
o
the
much of
dissertation
some
subsequent
of
againwith
because
presupposes
o
The Acquisition
was originally as
'Afterthoughts, in
details
Natural
Phonology,
"Additional
References"
the
' mostly
theory. the
a 1979 that
without
reader
These
footnotes. to
are
explain
not
by Donegan
furnishes
some
intended
may be directed
paper
I have
and me (see survey
work.
Columbus
1
be
to The Study
a general
July
to
1979
Ohio
of
recent
`~ i
vii
THE ACQUISITION
Since o
natural
1965
phonology,
system
of
ience.
since
this
report
is
to
more
into
that
restrictions tically
fall
the
into
restrictions.
by
voicing,
talks
have
some
by
example
between
the
be
voiceless
contradictions The
contradictor
and are
most
of
not
on a theory
the an
ways
of
Phonological innate
by
been of
system
of
linguistic
exper-
published,
I have
those
speech
talks
rather
which
sets,
oral
other
reflecting
voiceless
than
voiced.
tries
become
these
are
characteris-
voiced
required
in
voiced
overlap,
an ways
their
airflow
processes
three
phonetic of
the
a contradiction: There
the
irrespective
they
Where
opposi-
conflicting
impedes
hand
is
least Processes
constriction
there
Phonological
capacity.
become
the
a potential
opposition
"assimilation." vowels
residue
highlights
merges
human
on
Society
work.
of
their
while
environments
.
certain
contradictory
because
both
in
Obstruents
context,
the
that
revised
process
of
REPBESENTATION*
assumption the
recent
member
to
largely
review
A Phonological tion
the
my Previous
year on
reporting
on
processes, But
chosen
been
based
a language
phonological
to
I have
OF PHONETIC
for
abstruent bY which
cannot such
resolved.
radical
rocesses.
resolution
is
The master
by
suppression
of
one
of voiced
obstruents
of
the
in all
Papers from the Fifth Regiona7 Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, ed. Robert I inni. B ck Alice Davisan Georgia Green and Jerry L. Morgan. Chicago: University of Chicago Department of Linguistics, istics, 1969. Pp. 443-454. (Reprinted with permission of the Chicago Linguistic Society.)
.
f
viii contexts
entails
above,
with
contexts The
the
a
resulting
except
those
second
resolution
Process---limiting contexts and is
applies
strict
obstruents, because
tense may
vowels,
example.
clearly
are
not
For
role.
many
nairfi
of
between
low
voicing
aPP1Y
vowels Just
traditional
such
or
between
phonological which
lax
high
parameter
of
be
imposed,
ones.
but
their
latex
processes,
are
to
nasalized
Latin
and
Greek
(.gen.
o:s
Absorption
Greek
'nose'
from
ment-s
apply
after
original not
assibilation not
limit
there
the
innate
these
all:
the
ordered
application. this
can
obstruents
are
application
suppression manifest
of in
of in
Many
the either
ascribed
voiced the
the
be
voicing order.
to
effect
order process.
then s stem
a full
set
that
of
syllables
are
would
plified,
obstruents
But
merge
For example ,
the
utterances
become The
fullest
o f what
might
lax
could
could
not
not
aPP1Y to
of
Vnts
these
processes underwent
to
the
be assumed Latin
and
be called
the
only
caarticulad become
innate
system
Post-babbling"
reflect a
Process
state
unlimited
did
reflects
absorption
observable
the
was
can
li nguals of
but onlYto
Greek
processes
stops
the
h ima ; s ,Since
a innocent
clusters
effect
mans
the
xpresses thefullsystem of restrictions
usually
deleted,
form -
, whereas
y
are
Latin
himant-os
of
la ngua
phonol ogical
Greek
gen.
, iit
effect
its
and
of
processes
g
stressed
a ,
in
eP
processes
to
these the
extreme
process.
process
ordering-of
ical
speech:
of
anon
order
to yield
either
relationship
limitation--by
honolo
of
o,ss
absorption
it
to
gen .
assibilation
, the
simplified
'blood'
Thus
processes
nepo .ts
to
denasaliz
'thong'
absorption
The most
in certain
voicing
opposite
languages
then
is
and
changes
Both
, which
hr i ; s . As the
however
himant-s
. Latin
shows
s
which
o , •t- os
subsequent
the
before
changed
san9uin-s
from
Greeks
s
spirants
en, (g en.
ment-is
resulted
In
and
"sonority"
but
,
(by
to
absorption
nePo:ss
on in Latin.
which
at
before
Latin
(gen.
assibilati
limited
vowel
nr in-osjto
'mind'
Vns
as
'ear'
s
(gen. .[
sequence
to here
to
o:t-s
san9ui
t
Assibilation
changed
to
hrin-s
changing
neP o:t-is
, and
.
refer
nasal
I assume
application
from
in
process
I will
nePo:s
Vns
vowels
limitations
may
plus
'grandson'
discussion.
the
nrrneesses
suppressed,
limited
obstruents
indistinguishable
tense
be
in
of
altogether
to
may
opposition
pair
limited
which
sanguin-is
the
process
by
this
of
final
is
by
which
and
resolution
contexts
subtle
initial
third
process)
various
or
limitations this
of
contexts,
to
Besides
set
generality
may be
non-high
the
applied
the
suppression
is
of
a
an assibilation
a process vowel
than
to
devoicing
voiced
example
greatest
it
the
limited
are
is
them.
voicing
between
be
or
..
to
Likewise
not
part
all
there
favorable
relevant
a voicing
the
be
a
less
vowels
the
unless
to
process
process
voiceless
nonPhonological
The lack
ones
some
applies
complete
lax are
this
the
devoicing
forth.
may In
plays also
so
from
in
merges
of
each
mentioned
obstruents
process
it
in
process
in
suppression
the
to
voice
segments
to
devoicing
voicing
Implicit
to
limited
it
by
ranging
between but
are
in.
not
and
contexts
for
of
obstruents
be
positions, to
set
example, but
the
is
the of
which
motivated, For
it
in
hierarchies
phonetically
of
opposition
the
it
process.
Or
suppression
ix
and in
.1 innate
of unordered
infancy:unons are
coronal is
simvowels
seen period
in ,
.
.
xi
x which sist
although of
y are
still
well-articulated
nonsemantic,
sequences
of
characteristically
identical
and
stressed
lables a
mamama
that
the
or
stop
the
may
vowel
the
mayY or
may not
be
and
forth.
The
so
antes
in
these
vowel
Each
or
mayor Yor
structure,
with
like
Even
this
early
may
not
be
voiced
may
not
be
fronted
palatalized,
nasals
semantic
indeed words
by
resemble they are
is
some
assimilation
by
a
may or
f
e, first and
ere
cotonal,
may not
this
the plusstandard, ~a-lables composed of lax stop or nasal lowvowel:
but
this
words
post-babbling
often
dust
continuations
And in
of d
imPort.3
new phonetic
opposition
the
child
1earns._.~. ~..~~~pronounce t .o ~~
This
the
the
in
the
German
example
of
hunt
the
the
of
German
is
hund
of
but
all
has
.
as
In
well
obstruents.
morpheme-final
system
face
shows
representations
lack
to
permits
representation
in morpheme-final
example, in
flies
phonetic
children
to conform
because
Phonological
stays
is
As the
opposition
for
process
not,
representation
voicing
claim
pronunciation need
only
which
English-speaking
'dog/dogs'.
governs
no
the
tation.
in-
-
is
languages
altogether,
hunda
P honoloica1 g it
there
their
children
governs
languages
in case
utter-
process the
obstruents. if
German hunt
since
other
be denasalized,
word-final process
devoicing
devoxcxng
the
a coronal
these
of
suppress
freedom, to
acquisition
must
syl-
dadada
a a in
the
con-
consonants
no overt
phonological
manifes-
theories
n-
lvessomerevision the resolve
of
mechanisms
the
innate
of that this
contradictions
phonologrcal
revision
between
are
the
processes:
system. same
as
It those
suppression,
such
which
if
limitation, Pronunciation Thechild..s task
as from
the
standard.
e equivalent In those has
the
view of
the
survive
determine
obstruents is
innate
are what
For
not
then
only
the which
mature the
learned.
there
a limited
in
which
is
usually
of
governing
are
of
manifests
the
order
itself
unmo-
at
child
words all,
me that attested
with are
language
in which tophonetic h'sp
convinced
appears as
in
they
imply
g upon
whereby that
Jakobson
word-final
immediately
foreign
pronounced
mechanisms
which
devoicin
to be supported
seem
to
final
by the
pronunciation,
voiced
obstruents,
characteristically
have
noted
striking
mastered.
regularities
can be
fully
properties
of
the
innate
Ystern is
there
is
no need
1940
proposed,
since
result
entirely
from
to
for
example,
the
spirants
and
spirants
imply
to
to the the
imPlicational sto Ps
by
processes, three inner their hierarchies and
In particular,
"imPlicetional extent
innate
explained
by the
revised.
refer
in
My studies
system-its
interrelations--and
the
Consider,
innate
which,
regularities
are
these
in
devozced.
representations
their
pronunciation
pronounceable
process
of
appears
all
phonetically
devoicing
it
Students k in uiring acq: _adultof
have
must
retains
The processes
a process
version
system
mastery
representations
(presumably
system
but
independently
those: . rules
certainly phonetic
resultant
me
theyY ar are
the
speakers. b
system
example,
general)
ful 1Y,. the
of standard
(But
alternations
obstruen
that
intact.
language.
he succeeds
to
languages,
is and ordering.
onunciation revise :.. _.„~.....~_,.. „r .~. the~_~s ~.~ stem s__......~,.._.~..._P which se ara .. all to
I am proposing,
tivated
the
of I If
to
aspects left
ects
known
appears vo
it s
laws,"
that
these
such
are
valid
system. laws
Jakobson processes
that 51,
affricates 55).
There
affecting to be seem onlytwogeneral, context-free
.
i xiii
xii obstruent ited
articulations:
to
these
affricates
the
are
tion
laws
it
can
and
A similar
thereby
which
be seen
ordering
result
these
account
mechanisms
can
am aware.
be
given
for
spirants.
that
which
in a different
conform
to
all
imPlicational
the
the
is
laws
no
of P
less
general
processes
implicational
b Y the these
contradictions
is P 1 i ca txonal
contradictions
to
laws.
the
, tory
example,
context for
neutralize
if
there
-sensitive phonemic
was
processes
cannot
oppositions
laws
But
in
be ignored,
certain
In
words
there
laws
cannot
even
account
for
the
other
imPlicational
has
but
only
for
the
phonemic
inventory,
which
form
terms
treat palatal affricates
as
the
contradic-stops
for
they
to
abundant
contradictory
Processes
and
to
resolve
to suppression,
the
notion
still
the
the
contra-
limitation,
and or-
then
innate
be no difference
processes,
which
between is
just
what
system
support
speech.
of as
own
child's
Since
this
claim
child's
Phonological
assumed from
that
no evidence There
that
the
is
is
on the
to some
to from
sort
his has
other
closely
essential result
of
whatsoever
representations
productions system
have
distinct
assumption. the
tion
innate
his
that
the
the
phonology
I am aware
this
am proposing-that of
child
the the
con-
theory applica-
of Phonological
Phonetic
is
concerns
appropriate
and
representation.
Most
child
has
internalized
cends
in
detail
his
I
think,
comes
examine major
to digress
briefly
some evidence works
on child
a representation own reduced
of
this
language
of
adult
productions.
from
strictly
Phonological agree
that
speech
which
The most
striking
the transevi-
the when a child
masters
a phonological
opposition
rePresenta-
unaffected
had
merged.
in precisely
the
From
that
moment he pronounces
the
new
by appropriate
morphemes,
without
rehearing
neutralizations. them
im licational
laws
since
theory,
as
if
the
processes
are
taken
is
possible
as
the
the
I am proposing,
resentations
as
well
it as
inventories,
and
the
old
substitute
does
not
reappear
again.b
Unless,
of
primicourse
tives Lives of
of
evidence
to adult
segment contextual
we extended
would
the
So far
representation-it
may
Therefore
phonemic is
and
students
language.
he previously Lion P
laws
a phonemic
advanced
dence, im licational
if
be contradictory,
dering.
hand
in
contexts.
Even
to ignore
imp licational
no contrast.
the
to appeal
been
acquisition
could phonemic representation, of
for
have
standard
or
Ja predicted kobson was able
by interpreting which
of
themselves
Mostmodern
context-sensitive palatal andevenvelar stops, splrantizing
the apply order
allow
we would
of
processes affricatln g stops before high them after vowels for example. As might be expected, if these may arise
to
level.
I am arguing.
laws.
laws
affricating and
there
would
child are
law
every
dictions
the
representa-
5
t there
at
violate order
of
representations
imPlicational
If
there
could
levels
about
may be lim-
become
"intermediate" by
However,
stops
readily or
I
permitted
I
and affricates
limitation,
im licational
which
stops)
innate,
suppression,
acquisition.
become
become
processes
possible
obstruents
his
mastery
is
only
conditional,
so that
the
process
remains
to make preand in
fact rations
about reA
optional.
But
even
in this
case
the variation
between
the
new and
I
.
x iv old segment
will
guage have the
k
arise
at
I have the
first,
result
apparent.
fantile
process
word
so that
change
that
in favor
at
the
whose presence
in question,
makes coronals earlier
first
appearance
of velars
Gag Herr
Goka,
presumably
evidence
against before
son had a process
pronounced
like
now ing, one are
cite
common
at
some
while
the
having but
from
ordered clear
two examples
of
innate
processes a child
which
another
actually
do').
LeoPold
1947:
bat
dered
application
was
after
LeoPold
voiced
Performing
has
suppressed
the
order-
lel
at
not the
pplication
everythin
p
flapping
the unor-
Velten
of
n
b
changing •
nn
k~ni
a
1943)
.8
flap-dele-
of 'candy'
became
g after
ons of both
His pronunciation
'candy'
ob-
But the only
bap , with
End] to
of the
word-final
'spoon').
a
dud u
.
Later
steps,
Pu: bu months
of limitation o bstruents
only between
I will
of
f zcation
a
a
and then a
d ut
nd
was at nn
by ord
to
ering
n b
(a).9
and later
Examples
b
months
compare
n
which met the conditi
m into
20
am
processes
had bee
, had been pronounced
making
at
ore vowel
devozced
bud
I
, beside
and then denasalzzed
acquired
k~i , b y processes before
bef
by unordered
,
owever
of the
+ch0o-choor
bub'broom'
'lamb'
and desyllabi
Examples
assimilated
application
My son John had k~r,~~} ' Channin g+ by
applied
'Joey'
obstr uent voices
bite'
consonant
processes,
upon the
child
'bad
H
LeoPold
do:i
At 22 months Joan Velten
these
first
'dog'
'June'
268 and 1939: 126
ward she had previously
the
b
literature
, Hildegand
But at 19 months
did
support
the
example
du
and
a non-nasal
Guken
in
r choo-chop'bY
in the
phonological
the
d
find
For
'juice',
d ud u
'to
tion,
illustrating
processes.
,
struents
in-
unchan g e d
speech
[du(t)j'}
to
cases.
same processes.
Jakobson's
representation,
few
du
which
Far from being
being
a
becomes
as
then
so that
of
difficult
t1
of
is clearly
actually
Immediately
phonetic
children's
of
stops,
remained of
[9o9].
represented
[9a], the coronal
'doll'
suppression
kaek and
and
Tag, Herr Doktor'.
da
became
tat
Duten Ta Herr Dotta
final
as
occurs
aware
'church'
kt
is the
are
said
that
was not pre-
a velar
them, such cases
acquisition
examples and
f irat,
says
of this
from NadolecznY,
mentally
deleting
'dog'
velars to
limitation, child
'doll'
of velars
Returning
having
he can produce
claim.
to]
ordering
after
repression
culprit
pronounced
said
'Guten
child's
if
quoted
a child for
k
must be , explained
the
velar
and 'dog',
analysis:
of
the cases
process
to this
deleted
But all
'cat'
the
by a hypercorrect
in fact
In the case
lan-
in the use of both phonemes
can be
subject
to the
coronal,
(54).
of such a hYPercorrection,
acquisition
makes linguals
of [k]"
example
oppositions
emergence
caused
literature
which
the
those
of some additional
viously
discussing
, mistakes
especially
in the
in morphemes which in the adult
Jakobson,
appears
t
seen
only
of the process
finally
expected
place
former,
suppression
"when
the
the
take
xv
'paper between
The denasaliza
people, Papu tion
much easier
before voiced
process 25 months
are
Papa
voiced
segments
'guppy'
process
segments Pab a
LeoPold 1947:
Joan Velten purple'
t o find .
and and
first
Pabu
in herspeech
baba bef
31
'pa
pa')
ore she finally
B y exactly
distinguished
bu.bu
Hildegand
Paral-
between
'baby' , and then at 27
'probably' , mentioned
Velte above
n 290). was
t
i
xvli
xvi limited
to
nonf
inal
positions
before
being
suppressed:
sa:bud
PassY believed 'salmon'
becomes
sa•bun
and
finally
sa:mun
291
.
Chao's
tion, daughter,
learning
Mandarin
at
first
palatalized
all
coronals
this
to continuant
coronals
by children,
h -deletion
process,
successively
limited
stress
until
he
dropped
only
before
in
context-free
attained
my son's
the
in
speech
standard
completely
of the speech
explains
its
most
general
from "imperfect
imita-
of adults"
1890:
225.
This
why, except
in borrowing,
phonetic
change does not
to
formal
stressless
syllables
of
with
This
It
also
explains,
as the Neogrammarian
view
change
as due to subconscious
drifts
of pronunciation
does
decreasing
pronunciation
syllables.
in adults.
form
of phonetic was
change arises
Chao 1951: 29f))-0 seem to occur
'~ e
phonetic
and
hypothesis
hen limited
that
grand-
is
not,
how change
might be quite
that
children's
deformations
radical.
And finally,
to the extent
h
of adult
speech
are
regular,
it explains
a condi-
certain Phonetic change why is regular. If thec tional PPed limitation
even
in the
standard
at
least
in
my dialect
and
so
sound, it
may apply
to
increasingly
stressed
syllables
in
increasingly
it will
substituted speech
his
henhouse'is
example
illustrates,
Process
governs
henhouse
'is
hen'ouse>
'is
appear
'en'ouse
.
The
of
a
for
the
way the
inner
hierarchy
ing. not
only
its
form
a
violation
but
its
application; lication•
Phonetic
requiring
of
the
hierarchy
(e.g.
it
to the
sound he regularly
A Phonetic
^in readily change
be expressed
occurs
by the
when the child
theory
fails
I am propos-
to suppress
some
repre-
innate sentations
he has changed
it.-
This account incidentally,
that
Y relaxed FP a
enhouse~
process P
which
does not
aPP1Y in the
standard
language.
Thus if
do not
an American
child
obstruents,
for
fails
to suppress
the process
devoicing
word-final
occur.
As an example
of suppression,
I will
"u
cite
just
will exhibit w111 as pronounced
by a two-year
old boy in successive
his
speech-compared
to the adult
standard-
the word 'kitty'
interviews.
~ pronunciation changed fromki:
a phonetic example, change
corresponding
to the
"addition"
of a
His kii
to k i
to kL i
devoicin.g process to kLr i to
to the phonology.
This change
has in fact
occurred
gical many dialects in of EngT kLtib
suppression
of the processes
of prevocalic
tensrepresentations
ing, ingt
PostsY11ab1c Y successivedesYllabification
, flap-deletion,
and flapping
those boy's
parents
speak
a dialect
with unf1a PPed
t
It
a child
to manifest
all
the
intermediate
steps
0f later
between
his
pronunciations
of a form
My son's
the
pronunciation
of this
direct)
sate
that
Y from
ki:to
he suppressed
[kin].i all
This does not necessarily
the processes
at once
since
indi-
suppression
first
(flap-deletion)
would have resulted
generations
original
if
there
in the same abrupt
representations.
earlier vowels
respectively, of [b€: t].
the
nor would it affect
remained
any phonological
support
In the dialects
in question,
'bet'
bet
and
be•] d b
the
standard
process
making Y
word stressed
went
of course,
first and 'bed'
and last
originators,
is extraordinar extraordinary for
for
of its
the
change.
long are
still
or short
before
distinguished
voiced
or voiceless
by vowel length
segments, as
bet
and
,
I
xix
xviii adjustment these
before
can
be
seen
Velten
at
length
adjustment
first
tinguished ordering
devoicin$
and
before
most of the
gradually
admitted changes
often
have
admitted
more conservative must not
which
imagine
admit it
first
optional
sions
are
sa Y the
essentially
the
exemplified
devoicIng
standard
standard
has "generalized"
itself are
forces
speech. there
are
changes
many others
speech.
We
Even those that
in the isoglosses
at
the child's
progres-
of change-we
might
dialects
in the opposite dialects
it be ordered
order
in
which have
with respect
to
was relaxed in
to the form compatible
it will will
appear that he therefore
con-
change will proceed in from the child's
limita-
are optional
ones, as in the
They are sometimes even reflected dialect.
Ideally,
the
have admitted a process in its most general form, and isogloss
enforced,
at the outermost
rarely
of
assume the form of hierarchies
an innovating
each successive
marks an additional isogloss
The classic
limitation
we encounter
the process in any form.
encountered.
Of course
example involves
which has been dialects the ideal
the isoglosses
which is of the
"~~ Rhenish Fan ," on the boundary between High and Low German)13 -3 have dwelt on phonetic change at some length because the account of phonetic
to ordered
regularly
cited above.
surrounding
have not admitted
means thatY optional the oY are
process
in the process; rocess•
if the generalizations
until
anent-s
along these hierarchies
example of [h]-deletion
innermost
mens
Latin ordering
The generalization
implicit
of applicability
are characteris-
from unordered
appears
the process.
is
the dia-
as in
and to the extent he fails
These hierarchies
Beside
has the same length whether it repre-
a Phonological
tion of it.
innovator
unor-
n was absorbed here as well.l2
only
language.
in the child
absorption,
direction
the
in admitting
bund . The conservative
the opposite
by
than obligatory
only in relaxed
progression
required
with the standard
bunt
or
must limit
to the
rather
to t he tendencies
P phonetic change. have
exerts
On the contrary,
opposite
is why wh the typical
corresponding
admitted
speech.
regressions-of
This application
in child
after
The child
Thus it
before thethey fact become that phonetic obligatory
bunt
form to the hierarchies
standard
p
so that
popular Latin so that
by
at first.
devoicing,
that
tically
the
still
289),
Innovations
as optional
obligatory
the
German has gone a step further
underlying
assibilation
process.
•n hxs formal
which
processes,
she attains
conditionally
to suppress lects
to these
of the
sents
with
Thus her phonological
of children.
The conservatism
bat
adjustment.
dered application,
Joan
Velten
of the standard
begin
length
of
then at 24 months dis-
the devoicing
of ten just
as
respectively
regard
innovations
example,
alike
devoicing.
influence
application
For
and 'bad'
Three weeks later
rejecting
pronunciations.
children.
b a, t
by suppressing
phonetic
unordered
devoicing,
identical, 1, with
The conservative
that
of
after
as [bat]
above.
innate,
'back'
adjustment
cited
pronunciation
speech
applying
these
becomes
dialects
the
pronounced
length
system
in
The
acquisition
presented
here appears to explain
currently
known mechanisms of regular phonetic
addition
generalization,
child's
failure
of the innate
and unorderin
respectively,
of processes
to suppress,
system to the extent
change.
limit,
fully
all
the
The apparent arise
in the .
g
or order processes
required by the standard tanguage.
14
I1
f
i
~°
I
xx
xxi t
i
1 i
In
other
in
some
words, or
must
refer
ited
to
the
all
child
simply
contexts.
to
the
This
innate
understand
fails is
system
the
really
and
precise
to master all
the
nature
a phonetic
that
is
mechanisms
and
the
opposition
involved.
by which
regularity
But it
is
NOTES 1 The Greek
we
lim-
of phonetic F
change. The theory tions
outlined
acquisition And
i i. ~,
finally,
it
much
acquisition
can be
change can
observed
in
the
here and
Jakobson but
of
extended
for
the
Phonological
detail
than
change
of
representations
StamPa
regularities
systems
of
Jakobson's
for
the
theory,
the
as 4
languages
which
was
limited
Ei, ~;
i~ ,, ~~ ~~ i
)just
to
a subset
ness
theory
of
of
Phonological
ChomskY and Halle
approximately
the
theories
limited
were
context-free
same
1968:
set
of
processes.
just
to
the
processes.
chapter
9
most
The marked-
was limited
As a result underlying
both
levels
tion,
which
are
relatively,
though
not
entirely,
undisturbed
by
the
re-
,~
maining ,,i
the
processes.
theory
account ~~
such
outlined
for
all
theories
stantive,
in
Phonological gering standing
i
An important here
levels as
the
task of
must
can
present
is
that
succeed
instance It
confront
phonology.
then
of Phonological
these
system.
difference
becomes any
between the
latter
is
insofar
increasingly attempt
and to
Of course
as
by a description
theories
intended
representation. only
serious
those
they of
obvious
are
made sub-
the
innate
that
this
to advance
our
that
the
corre
ct forms would result if the processes applied sequentially and iterativel Y in random order. The notion "unordered" corresponds to the "unmarked ordering" of Kiparsky 1965 except that h I extend it, as noted, to include re-application. More on this below "Ordered" here corresponds t o KiParskY' s ~~marked ordering."
Post-babbling" utterances as defined here, are guished from the unstructured, random vocalizations of which significantly, is essentially alike in deaf and dren. Since the structure of post-babbling utterances for by the innate Phonological system, one might ufurther they are underlain by phonologic al representations, in haps as crude imitations of adult speech, prior to the its distinctions and semanticit y.
to be distintrue babbling, hearing chilcan be accounted speculate that some sense perrecognition of
representaI
'~
in
This crucial issue is discussed in Stampe (forthcoming), where it is contended that the problem of "phonological admissibility" is contained in and therefore inseparable from, the larger problem of loan phonology.
to
these of
unordered
1968).
such
world's
are
2However most of the processes mentioned in this paragraph make limited appearances, at least in some adult languages. For example, Vietnanese has lost Austroasiatic unstressed syllables, Oceanic has lost Austronesian consonant clusters etc. Such radical changes result from the collective effects of numerous specific processes.
representa-
way to account
imPlicational
can
phonetic
in a natural
of Phonological
account
in
finer
and
processes
stag-
under-
Conversely, it turns out that incorrect imPlicational laws like Jakobson's conjecture that spirants imply nasals Velten 1943: 282 fail to correspond to actual processes; in this instance, there is no general process changing non-nasal spirants to nasals. 6 The most notable exceptions to this generalization involve words which the parents have imitated When adults adopted my son' s Pibi for 'T.V.' by desPirantiz anon and labial "harmony"), this word persisted for months after he had suppressed these processes, even though he could pronounce tivi with ease Later after he adopted the standard form he still occasionally used Pibi i n bobYtalking. The most striking cases of "frozen speech" involve elder siblings, who occasionally adopt some stage of the speech of the younger, and continue it as a private language even after the younger sibli ng has attained standard pronunciation. Jakobson 1940 cites some references.) Such exceptions support, rather than refute the claim that the child's representations correspond to the productions of his models. In Yes Virginia... I argue that the child's representations must in fact be at least as deep as sentation" of adult speech, based on examples like
Phonological a "phonemic reprebAdn for adult
xxiii
xxii
bn?n cesses)
r
each of which from b nt n .
derives
by the application
of different
It is possible to avoid this result only by denying' of the two absorptions• but it was to express such KiParskY proposed the notion of change by reordering Place. For further examples of this sort, including plication, see StamPa (forthcoming).
pro-
With b for w for I (compare bibi for biwi for bL 'Bill' , reported to me by Ar lene Zide . The form bap persisted until the 27th month because it was adopted by the adults of the household Velten 282 note). Therefore the final [b] expected upon the ordering of denasali zation after devoicing at 22 months did not appear on schedule in this form. Compare note 6 above.
,.
ii
9The Further discussed
unordered
application
examples of in StamPa
survives
optional unordereng forthcoming.
, in
however, synchronic
in
allegro phonology
speech. are o 'a
10Since
I' i. i
allophonic
i
dude course
en
I i I
i I
~ I
~I
ii
II
i !~
i. i.
~
she
ordered
process
an
her
fronting
and
san
that her phonemic this conclusion
alatalization vowels
as
en
system has is necessary
process
after
the
Mandarin P
after pronouncing adult palatals, and [fun], Chao is forced to conmore vowels than the only if one insists
adult that
one. the
Of
13 It should be emphasized perhaps , that "generalization" as used in this discussion has little relation to its use in generative phonolo g ical theory, y, since t he latter is concerned not with innate processes but rather with rules which are "internalized" by the child to represent "significant generalizations" about his language . That the child supplements the innate h opological system with rules , particularly morphological and expressive ones is not at issue . But so little is known of such rules as yet that most recent speculations about constraints on them-such as the rotational and "markedness" conventions of Chomsky and Halle 1968: chapters 8 and 9-seem quite premature.
14 KiParskY 1968 also proposes change by suppressing processes of the standard language which he explains, along with generalization -:i phonological representations were a phonemic version of her own girl's j ~[ and reordering, as simplification of the grammar. He is thus unable pronunciations. t} s •:r to explain the addition of processes, whereas my proposal can explain addition generalization and ordering but not suppression. But t 11This view was applied by Jakobson 1940 to the phonemic level. t°. there are less than a half dozen clear cases o f change by suppression, The more recent proposal by Halle 1962 that change reflects the '# against innumerable cases of addition. r phonology, is not so limited, and re-addition of a process to the One of the clearest cases involves the suppression 0f the German flects our growing conviction that phonological processes are not mere ~` process devoicing word-final obstruents in certain Yiddish and descriptive devices but rather genuine i components of the mental gramnorthern Swiss dialects KiParskY 1968: ll1 That the process was 1F mar. But Halle's implication that adults might spontaneously add a ie suppressed seems certain but the cause appears not to have been general theory is difficult process to understand. Halle's is based "simplification" but rather a dile mmasoccasioned by the loss, in all :r all Phonologica 1 Processes are rules which are the on assumption that 'e these dialects of most word-final schwas. This introduced thousands constructed by the child to account for his linguistic experience, b cs of voiced obstruents into word-final position and, since the lost and that the phonological system is evaluated according d to the simschwas lacked clear morphological support, flatly contradicted the plicity of these rules so that (other thin things being g equal) the fewer devoicing process. Two resolutions were possible to children con, given this views why a rules thebetter. It,c is notat all clear fronting this situation. They could aPP1Y devoicing to the newly process should be added in the first place. s final obstruents and thus merge bunt and bund < banda : as [bunt]; or suppress it and keep [bunt] and bund distinct. Some dialects 12 On the basis of this Latin example it can be argued, contra ~~ took the former option and others, the ones KiParskY cites the latKiParskY (1968), that a process is not simply reordered but rather ter. What this suggests is that although suppression occurs under in this sort of change, because the absor process had unordered absorption certain circumstances it is not a primary mechanism of change. to PP1Y twice The first, and original, application san9uis
a
i i
the relationship relationships ' that in the first "iterative" ap-
i
i
xxv xxiv
REFERENCES appears d
eeper
plying
this study la ter,
superficially seems in variabl
Y to
to reveal
undoes to other due,factons most the effects typically of
occur that
~
the a distinct standard
in child language, such appearances
process process,
which,
a Although are ap-
Chao
Yuen-Ren. California
1951. The Cantian i.diolect. University o Publications in Semitic Philolo 2.27-44.
Chomsk Noam and Morris Halle New York: HarPer Row. Halle
Morris. 18.54-72.
1962.
Jakobson Roman. Laut9esetz, Universals. KiParskY, Paul. M.I.T. KiParskY, Paul. Universals T. Harms
Phonology
Sound Pattern
in generative
1965.
Phonological
Change.
grammar.
Ph.D,
1939, 1947. Speech Development 1 and 2. Evanston: Northwestern
Word
dissertation
1968. "Yes Virginia..." to the 4th Annual Regional Society.
StamPe,
Forthcoming.
H. V. 1943. infant language.
o
a Bilingual University
C'haragements Phonetiques.
StainPa> David. presented Linguistic
Velten
English.
1968. Linguistic change and linguistic universals. in Linguistic Theory, ed. by Emmon Bach and Robert 170-202. New York: Bolt Rinehart and Winston.
Paul. 1890. Etude cur lee Librairie Firmin-Didot.
David
o
1940. Kinders rache A hasie and all emeine t r. as Child language, Aphasia, and Phonological The Hague: Mouton 1968.
LeoPold, Werner F Child. Vols. Press. Passy,
1968
A Ihsse rcation
Paris:
Unpublished paper Meeting of the Chicago on flatural
The growth of phonemic and lexical Language 19.281-92.
Phonolo9U. patterns
in
A
DISSERTATION
ON NATURAL
PHONOLOGY
I
CHAPTER THE
NATURE
A phonological speech
to
the
process
substitute a
presenting
s
ecific
r
be ca
use
i
mp
voicing voicing
a class
alternative
of
voiced
their
or
to
the
stops
are
charact use eristic
d ePends.
There
is
simply
mouth it isthis diothis diffitis bs ervable o
in
of P avoiding
throu
re h t ghe
nasal it
incre
exam p the
nose;
the
of
many
diff
spirant
the
example
plu
s
spirant,
es
the
requir
precisely 1
with
of
lackin
the
icultY which
size
the
e.g.
is
c osur
the
e of
of
the
of
the
in
which
the lack oPs
which
voice:
offer e
these pressure
through the
the
glottis
cavity. sound
sequences:
closure velum. 1
it
lowering
difficult
oral
and
voiced;
suPraglottal
relieves by
of
processes
relieves
and
for
languages
up
articu-
avoids
stops children
suPraglottal
a class
of
which
giving
it
nose
process
other
which
involvin ;
release the
of
the
vibration
without
1 nation i
to
glottal
Young
are
C hndimplosion, relieves whi
An
capacity
voiceless
There
.
nasalization P
asing
speech
of
a Phonological
s p eech
a and
sequences
difficult
obstruction
b Y substituting the
-Haw pronuncia aiian them for
include
in
applies
sound
ut
relatively
t tion of voiced s speakers by
means
that
identical
e am edes the on air sir which
iculty
operation
sounds
class
PROCESSES
y.
For F example, late late
OF
a mental
common p difficulty
an p
FUNCTION
is
for
ind individ ual
diftl:n'
AND
I
If
a
to
articulate
of
the the
nasal velum
sequence because ns to
closes
coincide
of
t
3 2 before
the
oral
articulation
there
resembling
precedes
the
spirant
release,
velar
closure,
articulation,
processes
stop:
a nasal
lacking
or Ends].
oral
there
to this
nts
or
difficulty
closure. •
speech
If
the
is in the interim nzs
The other
oral
in children's
an oral
resembling
responding
an oral
nts
is in the interim
nss
for
Either
release
the
dance
of some English
The inborn
birth
of
all
seem
to
language
All of ,
genetic be
the they
inborn
capacity
capacity
lacking
voiceless
and within
fronted These
overcome
sequences
But in the
meantime
sounds
stop
in various
all
the
the of
consonant
properties,
are not merely
this
the
a
earliest
have mastered
utterances,
25
most of the
not reduced
Pronounce,
they
to silence.
employ substitutes.
random or occasional,
but are
,
and later
pronunciation a Year.1 r
notes.
voiced
stops
Finally,
the
are
ar s
haend hand),
same word as
The child
J'i•v
who
leave
vowel
and all
(e.g.
sn•
final
stops,
finally
as substitutes
child's
repertoire
change
his
physical
to produce
a sound
baba
substitutions:
[w] for
Er] in speech
dog, 1968:15
Jfor
I .
sounds Joan
bread
may at
the
did not appear
Jakobson
but
example;
papa
as Jakobson observed Joan
bed
for nasals: of
Hildegard's
is an interesting
a growling
as in
However,
it to substitution:
substituted
of
peripheral
who substitute
and Sully
result
by maturation.
children
(compare
or to imitate
Jbut
as a direct
not nerely
submit
bidi
Alexandrov
may
lights
eventually
1939:120-1
Children
in play
devoiced
he
in the
as tong nasalized
overcome
LeoPold
1943)
et
nasal
nasalized for
lion).
initial
from my notes)--pronounces
J'a•t
is not uncommon for
dren who dropped
Con-
zad
months e.g.
all
han
consonants
might be explained
at first
may use
language. are
It
for almost
are not ab-
plus
ants
who
But the
belong) , all
its
salmon
themselves
pretty
o
buzad
for
[j}.
1943)-pronounces
nonnasal
J
limitations,
expected
variety, P
as
substitutions
pr~ti o©
says
at fifteen
regularity
correctly
zab , another
One child
man).
phenomena.
vowel
Eats]
sabud
of vowel
This
over.
(e.g.
speech.
leaf,
following
[t ]'s
mae•
physical
respectively
These limitations
children
some
sequences
and
are
some of them in their
they cannot
initial
any
world
stop and a stressed
vowel.
children
side,
(e.g.
he child's
Velten
zuf
J'ae• -Daniel
whatever peoples of
negative
consists
(etc.)
among
of
limitations
the
of their
a
stops
says
dents, nasal plus ofspirant sequences, e.g.
e.g.
nasals
voiced
languages
zab -Joan
z
and all
pronunciation
typically
normal
and sound
substitutes
similar
syllables:
neutral
a few years
with
the
artic ulatoY r
s as
nasal
is observable
individuals
On
r eveal
unas itated
of the
children
acquiring children.
-vowel
i
as nonnasal
seems to be uniform
normal
speech
any P positive
such as [a],
sounds
to
earliest consonant
nonnasal
solute:
as
seems
children's simple
of
confront
two
lamb says
who says
sequences
speech
heritages:
capable
child
speakers. for
pronounce
One inserts
in the variation dews
and unexceptional
cannot
There are
process
and in the pronunciation
regular
a nasal
of timing.
substitutes Ns V
.
stop
bird
bud remain 15-33
1968: chil-
Velten but used
spoon
bin.
unchanged months
could Y
I
5
4 Pronounce
only
the
e
she substituted
J
for
vowels a
initial
I
he was equally
able
sounds
are
nounce
vowels
that fact
actually
bu • t
bead
versus the
versus
this a bit
retain
their
silently
have
beat
Velten
lengthening
the
len our
causal
thought.
1943.289 After
voicing
physically
phonetic
reveals
There
is
when
laIt]•
therefore
We must
look
are no
elsewhere
and
over
for
example.
of
slide
an
seems
planation,
to
of
have
these
much
tution,
whether
the
chaotically
word
e.g,
J
so forth-to
that
less in
it
children
which
would
1'n one utterance, cite
would
a physical
unpredictable lamb
with
be
mistaken
one, or
for
adults.
the One
pronunciations result
if
to
[w] in another,
a phonetic
regularity has of
the
seek
child
only
of to
substi-
consider
a specific avoided
single I:
En] in another,
only a few of the substitutions
observable
bY
N
of
with
motor
interpretation,
physical the
and
English
in
option
nts
other
of
using
.
According there
a lack
that
or
Vs
of
might
is
where the
articulation
they
difference
regularized occur to
insistent
for
ns
in
variation
distinguish
the
fo sub -
child
seem
not
to
merely
But
a language
a
from
a
typically .
Even
speakers
prince
given
invite
directly
,
to
not .
The examples
ns
to
he needs
is
result
in
cor -
seems
feedback or
,
en -
particular
.
of
that
, which
the
constraints
either
1939
i have
situations
habitual
in
treatments
investigators
substitution
for
the
and
only
LeoPold
understandable
substituted
since
speech
two
the
all,
various
of opportunity
motor
my speech
tempos
at
to habituate
Phonological
peripheral
II
lamb envisioned
, if
the
to
him of
is
in
speech
by adults
deprives
speech
of
are
tendency
is
his
substitute
dialects
year
pronunciation
which
mistiming
example
One is
child's
can
word
occurs
first
with
a
(Chapter
modal
the
are
-
way .
for
variability.
argued
above
or
matters
whether
a reflection
ex-
word-indices)
conversation
of
of van
there
to various
regular
her
deal
dtiv2n2
the
substitution
in
child's
of
variants
Compare ,
the
a great
pronunciation:
respond
a very
The other
determine
explanation
the
I have me
variants in
these
rection
stitutions.
the
variable
the
adult
that
of a word like
unlike
actual
massive
pronounced
for
But
courage
regularity.
It
s in
discussed
In
in
to deny
pronunciations
by Hildegand
interfere
that
dozen
childhood.
s
not
even
contexts
bed and bet
doubt
three
these
under
even s
tolerated
is
so pronunciations
earliest
Joan's
voiceless.
they
or
This
Chaotically
is acquired,
that
is
various
to
e.g.
or A's
segments
even
mental.
for children
they devoice,
ability
English
even when they are whispered,
distinction
are
that
children.
here,
pro-
as in standard
is not unusual
segments
of introspective
substitutions their
It
can also
often
various
dozen
of how or whether
Children
segments
though
period.
are mental
Pronounced.
voiced
this
regardless
Smith 1970:42).
makes
in
using
1but
w
w throughout
substitutions
bet.
light
same words
fact
of
bet
MY son John used
and thereafter
they occur
even before
I a it
though
these
long before
vowels
Jand
that
u .
years,
physically
bc•d bed beside lengthen
four
to say
from the
and [u l, but up to 22 months for
and thereafter
until
The conclusion be drawn
a
one in
have from
Przn
the is
,
N
1 6 for
example,
as
substitutes
are
the
underlying
ing
Soras
India
write
sim
nasal
in
speech.
the
appear
tended
ns
in
clearly
before
are
represent
confine
idioms silent
Halle
finable
in
features.
mental
is
is
that terms The
class
where
slips
the
of
is
character
mechanical
A second
to an outline
of
some of
the
made explicit
refer
undergoing
of
of
stitute
in-
where
bY
takes the
pronouncing
neuro-
sounds
the
definable
in
the
aspirated
for
have
ideas
of
for
terms
raised
by
of
about
resolved
which
the
independently
phanoloical
physical
substitutions there
features
feature
the
them-for in
voiceless
In is
is
some
not
nonetheless
,
between
English
stops
sub-
..lation
.
example
substitution
no nasPcrated
the
dissime
there
con-
Traditionally
changed
but
between
.
context
feature
features
neighboring
, where and
of
the
the
context
context
a feature
contextual
its
of
definable
assimilation the
on
a physically and
of
loses
motivation
dependent
is
include
in
been
been
the
a substitute
aspiration
all
neigh
like.
interdependency and
are
substitutions
to
stressed
in
,
the
far
example
onset
of
syllables. A third
acoustic I will
a
context-dependent
of
from
substitute
a physical
it
on
whose
have
are
relationships
identical
for
major
comes
between
any-in
them
evidence
recognized
properties, substitution
sort
absence
chiefly
to classes
they
such
or
our
what
nection
an
and
and
sounds--if
doubts of
of
omit
speech-its
recognized,
most
Typically
nts
and
but
sound-classes,
operation,
temporal,
widely
an observation
of
this
oral
difficulty,
a mental
Occasional
revisions
stress
physical
sounds
truth
are
of occurs
substitutions
supplied
of
class
Children's
its
for
targets
substitutions,
common articulatory-acoustic of
of
features.
there
the
substitution
these
con-
segments
that
by the
substitution
substitutions of
the
the
motivated
speech.
now becoming here
First
confirmed
lose
awareness
phonetic
clearly
triple
language.
motor
of
classifier
although
voiceless
merely
was
and
borhood
teach-
languages
what
which
substituted
against In
Munda
the
an
the
goes
animal
clearly
these
morphological,
my argument
Morris
require
by
in
this
for
,
and
not
is
Phonological
evidence.
knn
in
distinct
This
This
as
spirants
ns
PhYsiological,
nts
spelling
language.
the
wrote
indicating
they
the
dialects
for
motivated
properties.
English
folk
where
of
occur
indicates
but
one
In
knntsim
evidence
Although
of
of
1971:1$-19
whose
even
of
otherwise
substitution.
speakers
structure
Such
ns
mental
not
nasal
Read
even
pronounced
do
.
written
language,
'chicken',
of
the
be
usually
the
spellings
closure
Prents
language
they
in
clusters
Vs
to
their
that
ns
spontaneous
is
likely
found
underlying
sonant
versus
Phonological to
I
plus
Prigs
1
type
stitutions
types
is
given
bY
are
de-
substitutions
or
recognizable
class
commonness
many
whose to
of
evidence
application In
substitutions
which
often
in
to
do
not
to
their
grammatical .
words
In
so-called depends
relaxed
aPP1Y
conversation
from
typically
articulation.
due
comes
speech occur and
on in
in
formal
phrases or
my speech
any
the
verb
sub -
how
attention
much
language styles
which
semantic
optional
are
status think
there
.
These
readily or
their
is
optionally
8 pronounced
with
parenthetical its
use
phrase
h
use,
I don't
no such
limits
substitutions
assimilative
sonants
adjacent
to vowels
and which
are
more readily Similarly, example,
versus .
i
artic-
and statisti-
degrees
consonants
in the
they potentially
apply.
for nonPalatal
a substitution Palatal
of general-
involved
e. g
e.g
con-
is more likely i
versus e.g,
and it is more likely
not septo affect [f]).
in vowels which occurs
for
of weaker
articulation
lax
s
e
versus
basically
vowels
a context-free PalatalitY versus
(e.g. tense
process, e unstressed
stressed). These
substitutions
though
widely
including
the
recognized fact
that
properties they are
of phonological far
more extensive
of
children
motivated
are
their
characteristics
of
difficulties. formed
and
which the
mental
speech.
Its
speech,
and
process
seems
its
Al-
are
physical
articulator
mental
y
operations in
per-
speech
can
be performed
in the
distinct
is
from
not
rhythm
per-
in volume,
and
the
they
only
topic
may be
their
physical
and
later
becomes
further
here,
phonological
origin,
sorts
, for
physical
silent since seems that
,
thinking of physical of physical
The sublimation young
children
speech
, which
later
no manifestation '
Similarly
there
.substitution
that
shows
but
a complex
who have
of
variety
physical
older.
is
by those
than
impossible with
sub-
In my opinion
faster
eventually
g raw
in
mind
Identical
play
is not
ordinarily
other
essentially or
This
other
a sublimated
strikinglyng1y
is
,
activity.
certain
simply
difficult
as
motivated
physical
which
to Phono l0gIcal '
but also
is
speech
be subject
speech
accompany
first
why rnentai
only
to be
interesting
attributing
are
perceptual
involved
not
outwardly,
speech
speech.
they
its
systems
physically
tempo
reduced
at
should
language
who typically
aloud
of
the
are
are
as
employ
all
maximize
to minimize
is
corresponding
speech
becomes
to
physical
they
occurrence
processes
leaves
these
involving
activities
at
this
if
mastered
is
that
character
in
and
the
physically
stitutions,
which
speech
doubt
production.
One puzzle
topic,
mental
purpose
of
little
physical
Phonological
on behalf
ception
leave
by the
substitutions
teleology:
this
and other
speech
ultimately
of phonological
with the consonant
of PalatalitY
and weaker
to which
extremely
boundary),
to affect
function
to casual
the
in
tolerates
the
difficulty
of palatal
in vowel neutralization
is more likely versus
loss
speech
exhibit
vowels,
Palatalizable the
sounds
contact
by a syllable
establish
the
in
actualized
of physical
to palatal
in close
I dent noses
the application
substitution
are
The common but
by lexical
substitutions
degree
which
I am.
in
mental.
of the various
In the
it?-not
or less
on substitution
remind us that
to the
articulation
arated
placed
Phonological
according
aou or
in adapting
is nonetheless
Fourth,
to
commonplace
isn't
therefore
such examples
substitutions
the
raining,
but uncommon phrase
Although
considerations
next
it's
as in I think
similar
of Phonological
ity
but only in its
know can be reduced
reduction.
ulation,
of [e],
as in I think
as a main verb
phonologically
cal
instead
, reading'
is
I cannot
pursue
it
that
seems
done
mental
to be no difficulty it
shares
with
In
10 physical
speech
Sounds ulate. of ,
and
and
world
the
and
1968.
But
patterns from
in some
extremely equal
world's
sounds
which most
some
retention
South
seems
to
Another
seems
Conversely,
all
in childhood,
equally
demanding been
universal
and
sound-Patterns
vowel
in
of that
sound
which
be of vowel
the
Mon-Khmer
group),
scores
of
northwest but
systems
consonants
with
few
American
few
are
sounds
distinctions
and Africa there
of as
to
originate
it
is
probably
with
some
impoverished
e. g.
limits
to the
beyond
which
homophony
is
Chinese in
the not
the
systematic
and
the
attempt accidental
languages syllable
are
this in
in
simplicity becomes
development
South
seem
Phonological
we find
of
to
the
that
in
quite
of
acquisition
with
the
English.
the
complex
Broadly
to
set tie
for
a
with
which
this
capacity
flexibility
early
different
sound
can
children
be
corn-
speaking
certain
,
moderate
fixed.
But and
not
there
as
their
is
sounds to
deny
much
in
development
of
children.
now
generally
and
sound-Patterns
that
more
achieved
faculty
even
i t
is
is
speech seen
acquire
This
systems
the
investigators,
orders. are
of
flexibility
of
different
children's
certain
orders
diversity
than
in
was
at
first
recognized.
analysis
of
reveals
that
of
them
much the these
a
this
distinct
shared in
of
are by
many
lamb
as
intolerable.
surprising
z
for
i
of exten-
processes,
two
paralleled Both
homophones. also children
in children
responsible has
is
only
a
the
of
small
out
are
shared
children
the
studied
number
of
Consider
Joan
J`"• ae
,
by and
detail
from
of by
and
in
Edwards
proJoan's
three
distinct
all of
This dark
some
Daniel's
and
J
often
Phonological
languages
to
pronunciation
children
applying
Daniel
in
Detailed
respectively.
t o result
1
in
distinct
but
and
turns
apparent.
children
delateralized for
been
of
z:ab
which other
to
manner.
of
be
due
cumulative
of
diversity substitutions
nunciations
no languages
Nicobarese
permitting
example,
systems
same
opinion
recognized
inventories sound-
say,
and
and
great
The
the
processes
of
for
require
diversity
beings.
Despite
Ubykh,
of
to
of
capacity.
testimony
of east
Munda languages,
to disambiguate that
languages
system
of
sounds
However,
languages). tonal
of
is
vowel
system
binations
Indian
combinations
large
consonant
human
relatively
elaborate
the
The
aside
others must
and
huge
by Jakobson
, languages
many sounds
relatively
with
systems
languages
This
are
conclude and
to articlanguages
diversity
we must
e, g,
in
sounds
incredible
languages
easy
documented
almost
are
inventories
compounding,
late
various
that
indication
rare
the
are
with
America,
and
relatively
has
Caucasian
of
indicate
are
languages,
rudimentary
American
Asia,
patterns
early,
There
languages
not
less
sound-combinations
South The
that
few consonants
,
are
acquired
sounds
northwest
languages
which
the
with
many
ones
when we consider
relatively
the
it
combining
origins.
usually
of
difficulty.
e. g,
sive
are
acquired
rare,
languages
but
and
are
physical
demanding
converse
common
its
sound-sequences
The more
the
and
to
11
of the
which world.
process, 4as 1910.
can
which w
in
She
both
is
~
13
12 compares
Hilde
Charles'
Ja
~ ckie ' s
baJun
piano I
from
and's
lie,
J'u
la and Edmond's balloon
*Zanu
in the
J'ai
kJ'e
lower
clef
Albright
and bianco
Zutscht
Grgoire
as
Italian
Charles'
Jakobson
1968:17), Y
ation
of
u
Joan
Pirantized
speech
of Daniel
!
in the though
used the
at
an earlier
you
Leoofd
stage
and is
to [d3],
she
to
z for
I
should
zad
states
that
in its
steps
pronounced
three adult
1943:290.
far
he does occurs
1910 not
also
it
further
in p Jas
in many languages, e.g
change of
was
Jas
for
to plain
-
[z].
J
cabbage t mach and
9arbLd
does not affect the du
shoe
J
I
is
substituted with
for
I .
the depalatalized
I cite
and dePalatalization
adult
systems.
that bY
of
speech
Furthermore, stand the phonetic are scores
stopped
of 0riYa
, t
observed personally
of Orissa,
India).
becomes [z],
be multiplied, sPirantiza-
of child and
or even
.
is the one I am proposing:
The function
no such process
of the individual
I but lack
on the other hand is rather
whose difficulty
last
master
z
for
z
Hockett
responsible
seems fairly clearly
for
clear.
to eliminate
makes laterals
Jakobson 1968:57);
is avoided not only by this process,
to under-
I as
processes
rather
and languages.
of Joan's pronouncing
tongue-configuration sounds children
that
exists--but
common to many children
of delateralization
The
z is due not to a process peculiar
of languages which have
her unusual substitution,
Depalatalization P
I
I as
utility
met
because
3 observed in French and Czech
only on this assumption is it possible
1955 .
teacher is shared process by Daniel, e.g. b ns brush th
1937.345
are matched in a variety
for these facts
pronunciation
The function
and
On the other hand there seems not to be a single
process
This
Grge oire
processes s delateralization,
case except for Joan in which
there
and
Edmond
whose number could easily
the individual
tion
shoes
1970
ber ~'ere
in the Desia dialect
these parallels,
to show that
of
z
for
[s]
dz
su•z
and the change of the palatals
to a sequence of processes
garbage.
as in Joan's
Jakobson 1968:61),
adult
-'
mats
z for [f,
John's Edwards
bezer
to her own speech -apparently
This is confirmed
wash,
s
brush
and
alveolars
dePalatalization
wa
of
my son
bwns
ehambre
only explanation
J
of the substitution I
The final
kaebLd
s irantize in A's
e.g.
for Latin
itsr Edwards
above apply
briefly,
she pronounced
My analysis
in Joan's
abudz
cited
1970
in the GanJ'amand KoraPut districts
does not s
diachronically
alveopalatals
no examples.
Velten
is paralleled
in the
it
s
Smith
Melissa's
sabr
Joan's
Velten
entails: have
and
step
pronunci-
Oiulio.
z
but cites
did apply
as in Jules
Joan daPalatalized
changing
children
is exemplified
an intermediate
as in Italian
Finally,
z
Hildegand It
actual
This process
or the other
in French
JuZ2us
in his
Jto
1939:34.
man
as
[j]-substitute
John
aphasics to
Daniel
soon
substitutions
Jfor p
and so forth. Whereas
du•n
dza•n
the use of
of Russian
in
1937:317
& Albri ght 1956),
from *blancu
Ko1 ma dialect
LeoPold 1947), g
this
which substitutes
a
some of the configuration glides
but
d
14 also
by Processes
~ 1970.
which
SPirantization
substitute
id ]2
.
u• you E1eni' s
substitution
posture
Jahr'.
of
the
These the
Finally,
ultimate
I
Edwards
substi-
of J is null J'uk
as in
Edwards 1970:
dePafatalizationeliminates
Phonological
in
parallels
the
failure
is
due
to
be understood
in
part
to
underlying
serendipitous
of to
terms of
based
on the are
them'
into
substitutions
to carry
the
that
view functional
to sounds
there
t
and
its
unjustified
d as
features not
ogy
this
requires
ization
is
view
analysis are
certain In
the
single are
if
they
analysis
that
of actual
What
sounds.
application
they
the once.
d,
.424
whose
d
Jakob son 'weak'
in
s analysis
Pretonic
appeals
d
to an otherwise
The Posttonic
srespectivelY
phonetic
, pronounced
versus
same processes
voiced
and
pronunciation
, is
from
the
not
due
of
to voicing
that
its
and
example
process
which
would
postvocalic from
be
adequately
processes
Danish
already
regard
the
recognized
could
simpler
context
ffrom o f both
a language-universal
point
changes
phonol -
sPirantizat1on
application
in of
spirant-
.
on our
assumption
that
and
others
impossible
by positing
postvocalic
of
In
view
would
point
the
of
them .
of
are
not
discussions
voices
the
5but
be described
sPirantizes
processes
in
possible
simultaneously voiced,
of the
position
contemporary
to voicing
are
facts
In
depends
these
to
ordering
example,
postvocalic of
t
in Tamil
recognition
Phonological
8in
effects
of
to the
with
Tamil , for
sPzrantized
voicing
whereas
been
to
the
is
as due
unrestricted
are
Danish d
to voicing,
aPP1Y in
sPirantized
In
explained
is
But
g
51962
underlying
has
ordering
In phonology phonemes
both
is prior
such
the
results
Danish
languages
by the
which
'strop'
and
1969:141
constituent
systems.
that
t is
d
Pretonic
feature.) d
phonemes
features
processes
mulative:
of
as
of
distinctive
t of Danish like
c congener
Furthermore
neglect and
d
their
examples
exactly
distinctive t and
of
e,gthe
posttonic
posttoni
Danish
invariance
well-known
positions, and
where
the
'overlap',
Posttonic of
by
are
realizations
the
gross
through
own structure.
of
but
Trubetzkoy
this
comparison
Phonological
distinctive
related
that
their
a careful
in
with
to analyze
such
traditional
favor
itself
of
adult
However,
nineteenth
I think
conviction
the
in
concerned
I
phonology
of
the
for
and
processes
of
processes
z
structuralist
sounds
phonemes
child
in
fails
cases,
previous
the in
mental
view
of
individual
But
phonology
analysis
a rejection
abstractions this
most
not
like
in a variety
The
led
as
to be suggestive
beginnings
processes.
so long
The correct
the
of
sPirantization
.
intended
of
the fronted
in favor of the neutral
etc.
only
unfortunately
substitutions
processes.
z,
explanations
has
understandable
inexplicable
are
some promising
phonetics of
, etc] s,
remarks
phonetic
explanation
except
alveolars
Despite
century,
In
plain
imprecise
question.
are
for
of JbY
uk look later
and raised tongue-Posture of [f,
this
r
and the Scandinavian loss of Germanic *[j], as in Icelandic
ar 'year,
is
or
Its function becomes apparent when we note
that the most commonalternative
23
n
increases the audibility
tuting a sibilant
Jennifer's
15
of
a
stops
fact
such
Danish
be expressed view , this
.
and , an in
that
just analysis
lb is not satisfactory: have to explain voicing
t
it merely adds another
the existence
and sPirantization
is voiced and
of.
but
d were sPirantized
The difference
to the many we
esses
For there are languages in which
d
in American English
is not sPirantized•
PostvocalicallY,
of the Danish situation
same processes
process
occur separately:
intervocalicallY,
The analysis
17
but
as resulting
as are found in American English,
and many other languages, but with an ordering
whereas it
was not voiced.
from precisely Old Irish,
if
than
the
at
Tamil
the
from
imposed in Danish
a diversity
And it eliminates
any [j].
process tinct
which simultaneously phonetic
an interesting
that
this
voicing t
that
t
to
analysis
This follows
5s
and sPirantization.
This prediction
system will
t
two distinct
If this is so, there is no without
is not overturned
d also being
by any language of
which I am aware.2 The same reasoning children's
speech.
to two processes, of
J
to
both of them: remains
reveals
that processes
analysis
may be ordered in
of Joan's Pronunciation
delateralization
of
I
to
Jand
of [I]
appealed
J'u
lutscht.
But for Hildegand
it does not become sPirantized
,
for
speech
capacities,
I
as in Joan's speech. J
mortals
processes
represent, system.
themselves
which
however
have
For vowels,
results
well-attested
languages explanations
of
the have
been
natural are
o f numbers they
children for
may seem
perversely
proposed
.
various There
cross process
Jakobson
which
of
of
limited such
individual
a unified furnish
adult
such
languages
substitutions individually -purposeful which
1968:71-72) plausible
is
processes
involving
to
and of
and J
any sort
to their
to devise
to
I
simple
as these
children
se,
, revealing of
to create
managed
aiming
Jderived
persisted
a system
i s a context-free in
world
result
which
there
has not of
be the
reject
anger
application
treatment
sounds
Per
to
1
attempt
nature
phonetically
example
Jfor
by
J
its
difficulties
and instead
is no str
except
but two distinct
to specific
to
collective
withholding
difficult
The pronunciations
reveal
to
god of language , free
However,
d
Thus
can pronounce
oddity
sPirantization
apply
, Joan does.
soon acquired
her earlier
one would surely
does not
Hildegand
of
of
implies
to adapt
responses
a system.
step
complex process
atomistic
global
, Hildegand
a short
the
all
to say postvocalic
substitution
one were
system
sPirantization
, and, as the examples cited show, Hildegand shares u you,
only
But her
If
from the assumption
as in Tamil , involves
could be voiced and sPirantized
sPirantized.
is
anon
This apparent
application
as our analysis
provides us with
in no Phonological
d becomes
the change of
way that
dis-
it
the
was not a single
langage-universal
prediction:
5unless
processes,
Performs two quite
that
modifications,
Furthermore
become
but selectively
a putative
I
(
inability
withholding
from
systems.
to say
Jat
not in the proc -
Hildegand
of delateraliz
pronounce
attempts
lies
of application
Not surprisingly
tog ether in ~just two processes
pronunciations
to the output
Dane's
It].
their
order
Joan cannot she
which is not imposed in Tamil, brings of Phonological
in their
sPirantization
t
in Old Irish
t
but
between
in addition
, .
den asalizes and
in
phonetic a process,
.
18 also
widespread
vowels
in children's
adjacent
to nasals
and adult's
teleolo
Yggestures
g ies
(nasality)
processes-one
in vowels
in general,
spread
the
and
the
Given the
the other
these
then nasalization.
of sequences explanation, in thisphonetic P
feature
processes American
apply, English
first
is that
time
vowels
before
like
si"•n
nations words
mama
in
with
oral
nasals
are nasalized
seen
vowels
beside
before becomes
It are
is
tempting
as
become nasal
However,
there
known to me. are
nasality words "
nasal
e.g.
only ka~t
b3nami
or the
identify before this can't
these
b3nami
nasals
that
rule
vowels as nonnasal.
nasals
is not perceived
versus
remains kat
to indicate cat.
Vowel
tion
in any language
for
the fact
as such, the
even in
with
underlying
denasalization
.
ad1acent
Trench
is adequately
a surrogate
I
the
think
be
fact
denasalizeng
na
French
cited
above
do not substitute
e.gsYllabis
En],
explained
vowels
n3non
that
explained
for a
by the fact
making such a substitution
process for
which
specify
Speakers
and this
process
conditions
examples
sound
fact
constraints
,
cannot
pronounceable
testimony
for
this
it
, whereas
Thus Joan Velten
ats
for [its]
vowel nasaliz
anon.
both involving like
nman] lion, vowel-*
vowel denasalization
nasalitY.5
t o the individualistic
can be shown that . Prior
by a nasalized
known to me that is
not
which
,
surface
processes-potential
words.
the
and
ants
haend hand.
Joan applied
nasalized
The superficial
in
view
conditions'
do
vowels
only
to
negative
as
is no general
says
to
'redundancy
foreigni'
English
affecting
not
but
in
in
customary
due
such a distinction
vowel
comes from forms
that
been
de nasalize
processes
evidence
in general
to
nasalized,
As further ological
has
like-essentially
English
against
for
nasalization
as
confronted
example,
ha n
Y
nonnasals.3
vowels
us to account
for
representation
representations
is a general
that
before
their
nasality
on nasalit
restriction:
of such a unified
,
are
there
there
which
the
any similar
that
It
rather
derivative)y
nasalized
vowel
representation
of
dictates
underlying
vowels.4
or
they
rnarnan b just
alter-
bon ami,
restrictions
and nonnasal
enables
"
like. theinEnglish mouth
consistent
the assumption
in English
of vowels where
before
Furthermore,
speakers
e.g.
a single
is no evidence
denasalized
English
into
At the same speakers:
,
but even in forei 8n
nasals
to su PPos e that
somehow unified
vowels
si•see
of
certain
speakers
Foreign
not only in native
with
underlying
when
denasalization
ecomes [mama], etc.
on
languages
prohibit
b words with nasalized vowels aretypically denasalized y English French
most
conventions'
that
is an example.
in
a condition
substitutions--but
adjacent
is not too surprising
effect
on
a difficult
permitting
in
realizations
distinct
ssubstitutions. make ions. they What is surprising
both
of
timing
eliminating
in nasality to to a vowel-it
opposite
most languages
1972).
which nasalizes
is capable
involving
Schourup
of these
nasalit
speech,
and which likewise is case
of articulator
19
this
to sort
in which
nan-since
o f nasalization
+
two pieces
processes the
Ian there
o f Phon-
t o vowel denasaliza-
There are
intervening
Ian
purposes
I ~
.
of One
is probably I""an +
nan "
is no evidence
can be caused
by non-
N
1 20 The other Lion process segment
evidence which
in a word:
sabud
nan
stops
is
that
it
can account
for
implied lied
nan
in 'n
to progressive
etc.)
tion
allows
at
applies
it
a
that
t
Ian
am M. We
at which
for a peculiar VeIten
noted
onlyY bef before
u
(286),
9, d> z> w> etc.
not before
room, beside
Joan's
ham ham.
,
other Now
Of
sonorants
before
subcase
a word
2
regressive nasalized
nasalized
denasalization
and
4
segments I
of
denasalization
all
x
wum
samun
hum
ham
Ian
na
1
wum
samun
hum
am
In
na
2
wum
samun
hum
ham
nn
na
3
wub
sabud
hub
ham
nn
na
4
wub
sabud
hub
ham
nan
na
iterative
each two
are
regressive in
Joan's
can part
be
paralleled
of
English
nasalization
speech.
She
in
corrects
other
vowel though the
after
children
is,
phonetically,
vowel h
h
whereas
1972
and
ingly,
in Joan's h
izabilitY
a breathy
in
ham
of
vocalics
an
inverse
Joan's
vowel
nonsyllabic
pronunciation is
a is
It
is
of
nasalization
well of their
established their
process
high
nonsYllabics
such
as
w
or
uh
u .,
that
the
syllabicity
height
, namel namely to
following
of home is presumably
o function
function
copy of the
Chen
seems ah
to • ,
be it
Consequently,
.
Accord-
limited does
, having ~ suppressed
and
progressive
nasal-
Schouru P 1971
291
not while
in
to
like
vowel
extend andnonhigh syllabics nonsYllabics
feres
with
the
and
denasalization
their
inner
un-English
denasalization
The existence vowel
the
contrary
in
and
a reverse
order
order
a year
Phonetic
articulation
complexities
nasalization
in the
independence
and vowel
denasalization the
3
of
contrary
teleologies. which and
of single
Context-free
eliminates acoustics segments.
(
287n
be
2 291). like
sought
ultimately
processes--
a feature of
Velten
N
Processes
must
from
later
of
meantime
nasalization
and
denasalization
.
h
a
vocalics: non
processes
n
segments
of
this
becomes
lion
and
de-
nasalization
ham
languages,
that
a
iterative
home
these
of
progressive
salmon
on
speech
that
room
This assump-
in Joan's
by eonsYllabics
in
na
condition
as
1
progressive
prior
as in
Joan has
I
iterative
nonnasal
Nh N aam
to
wub .b
or nonhigh
include
progressive
vowel denasalization
wub
would 3
process
applies
[hub],
of syllabic N
is nasalized
N home uumandroomwumis not , and it causes
To summarize
am + Earn] M would then con-
naturally
home, like
In replaces
lion
of ham
nasalization:
denasalization
denasalization.
was triggered
hub
Ian
of derivation
which
denasalization
pronunciation
progressive
denasalization
caused
vowel,
after
f
lion.
e.g.
nonsyllabic hN
follows
the vowel nasalization
since
and
for
by vowels that
Tan
the stage
us to account
although
h
b assuming by
-(- naan --
zad
the
a non-nasal
clearly
the progressive
is not triggered
applies,
progressive that
about
denasaliza-
following
This process
i , this
denasalization
denasalization non
thing
this
tam p no nonnasal
segments
we would have
nasalizing
The peculiar
process
all
salmon.
otherwise
when Joan
a context-dependent
denasalizes
I -nasalizanon fact
involves
21 N aham
that
inter-
vowels-respond
Segments
are
to often
22
23
compounded
of properties
which compromise
fore
are
processes
which eliminate
one property
at the expense
there
emphasizing the vowel
a
may Y be viewed
as a compound of Since
alataliY
ness ness,
the vowel
There
is
of its
alata1itY
clearly
sonority:
1
2
Lowering,
3
De a 1 ata 11zation
to into
e
Finally,
}
ee
are
ization might
as
to which be noted
is
that
susceptible), lax
syllables
once
almost
necessarily Miller,
the vowel com-
which splits
4
ultimately
its
are
and nature from these.
especially
of these
Assimilation
to the optimal
latively,
to which
The formulations
of Miller
1972a
+
Diphthongiza-
processes
1972b,
4
vowel are seems to are
and StamPe
ai
the result
followed by a maximally narrow
of a single
in the
In addition
segment
processes
For example,
diPhthongization
the
of contrary
conditions
speech.
Context-sensitive
there
in informal,
Applied
is a process 3 Height aPP1Y cumu-
to a final sequence
Processes
under which they
aPP1Y most generally
is more frequent
-'- [hi].
process,
ee
altogether
[e l.9
teleolagies
dialects
[all
Assimila-
these processes
which eliminates
contrary
affecting
1 Palatal
This is subject
.,
Context-free
most generally
by two processes
If all
is tel.
that
segments which
on behalf of the syllabic:
-} ae .
and
A diphthong like [aLl ,, requires
in fell.
the ponsYllabic II,
There are processes
2 Height Assimilation,
they result
radical
organs which are quite
of the eonsYllabic:
mi , and
The contrary clear
a vowel to a feature
by substituting
the sequence .
MonoPhthongization,
in favor It
here
into
on behalf
[ail
affecting
of the
difficulties
These extremes are mitigated
cumulatively
the
susceptible).
properties
better
one of the
of Raising
which assimilates
in speech of normal tempos.
the syllabic
of sequences of segments,
Sequences of segments often require
respond to these 'blend'
which respond
of segments, there are context-sensitive
movements of the articulatorY
difficult
tion,
processes,
which respond to complexities
context.
one.
fronted.
but retains
complexities
a maximally open tongue aperture
palatalitY: it
sonorize
segment:
Two
and of Lowering g and De
the essential
revisions
of its
but keeps
b~Y the application
the existence
duee too Patricia 1972.
or
the vowel.
each to polarize single
properties.
at the expense
the vowel,
[eel, which develop
especially
understood follow
or
precise
is a process
permitting
of the original
y open-
of these
to these context-free
vowel nasalization
and sonority
the expense
cumulatively
there
e.g, of its
i , narrows
backs
phoneticians, a -ness. P
palatality
the vowel,
e P -' [A],
two halves,
diphthongs glides
at
ae ~ opens
two applied
a
two properties tion
--
The last
pletely vowel
a
sonority
+
processes,
or
a compromise
Raising,g, [e]
For example,
with sonority
its
to the inner
compromises,
Indian
by narrowness
In contrast
and there-
of another.
J' -ness
represents
its
these
the ancient
which increases
increase
heig ht
or
is maximized
one process
Processes
with
each other,
of vowels like and extreme processes, alleero
are
particularly
tend
in formal
to aPP1Y. lento
speech.
[ml in Appalachian
in slow or hYFerarticulated on the other speech.
hand,
For examule,
apply in the
I 24 same Appalachian like
a ,i
dialects
25
the monophthongization
to Em] is more frequent
i n raid
of diphthongs
effects tion,
o r hYPoarticulated
speech.
typical
ordering
systems.
teleologies
earlier
of vowel
sensitive
nasality
syntaginatic
difficulty
to adjacent
nasals).
often
are
only
responsible
for
the
for
our
for
our pronunciation as {skritsi
normally
ui dozens
govern
which merely
govern
stops
of the
of forms like lax
stop in
of foreign .
if
speech
let's
to
of context-sensitive
sko
There
s9in skin
is
is
k
to
all
to contrary
it
may not
of casual
traditionally
which extend
their
productive
constraint
as can be seen in its in foreign
no matter
words applies:
how rapidly
for
this
their
constraint
vowels
or relax -
Additional
evidence mostly
detail,
and
for
d gin. t . fr
In all the
these
process
rnei " "nl i
lost
Jennifer
examples producing
and found
speakers
kme "• n like
matinly
also
faust examples
syllabication blocked by re nasalization
surface
nasality
ou .ne nonnasal
or
observed
like
FromkLn 33
a n underlying
of reprethe One
identify
invariably
can and
moon NN I I
slips
or Pig Latin
level
in StamPa 1968 .
even in forms which like
representa-
I have discussed
English
by Fromki n 1911.40
i Dst n faund
than the
phonemic .
come s from blends
ones is
on underlying
underlying
vowels,
motivated
motivated
in some detail
as non nasal
with nasalized
phonetic
constraint
already:
pronounced
moustII „
of paradigmatically
synt agmaticallY
called
b
speech ~
kaet
be less
has been mentioned
for
, it ~
For example,
to which the process
ordering
applies s9o
style
for an important
that
for
s9rit•si i
process
The study
processes
are
as a voiceless
Italian this
tion,
sort
in English
9'o is reduced .
which
[S I, which
words like
In informal
devoicing
representations.
tautosYllabic
responsible
sentation
in a language
speech
context-free
way.
le vowel nasality
does not become
prior
evidence processes
representations
after
absence
surface
is a fully
representa-
edlYy one sPeaks . 10
processes
though by no means exclu-
in this
in " English
to nonderivab
The characteristic
Phonolog-~
effects
representation
can't
their vowels
underlying
sPeech,context-sensitive
representations: undergoes
vowels of English
its
to superficial
to find a single
denasalization
is no casual
kit
context-sensitive.
underlying
apprehension
to surface
those
to aPP1Y to surface
devoicing
grzzzz
which
there
maximizes
assimilating
extends
vowel
application
but minimizes
speech
predominantly
in casual
permitted
vowels
which
on underlying
to the context-
This order
predominantly
while
are
a process
turns
Processes
context-free,
govern
prior
representation
I have been unable
although
Phonological
of the underlying
in connected
in the
the context-free
is ordered
in individual
representations
normally
in English
distinctness
(eliminating
Indeed
that
themselves
in mature
of vowel nasalization.
paradigmatic
representations
manifest
Processes
denasalization
process
sively)
also
of contrary
I noted
process
ical
but
process
These contrary
the
on underlying
are
mrt
m2nt.
me1ts i i , but without n I Jnd with
as in singing d E.ni.fr for vowel
is blocked.
"N oun
own, etc.
surfaces
when
Note that
we
i 26 cannot
attribute
nasal
vowels
English, the
before
since
vowel
iably
the
ization
process it
the
words from
which
need
need
nonna
sat
would
be
no
to
be
all
be
a
saved
taking
nonnasal
.
nasal
the Of
seems
vowels
be
it
cannot
this
to
be
an
be
derived
example-
vowel nasalization
and which therefore
sounds
nasal
system
translates
representation
m
nasalization
in English
sounds which
the prior
Thus the notion
which gives
general
process more
is allophonic
process
And nasal
vowels
in the 'allophonic'
to sounds
in the
because
context-free
vowels
rise
system .
i t gives
nasalization
language
the
and
nasal
thus
ordering be
of
seen
in
ordering
dena
from m nonn a s al
salization
vowels denas
Joan
is
processes
process
would
alization
paradigmatically
motivated
to
developing ' by
have
'phonemic'
prior
Ve lien's
encouraged
be
would
another
in
to the
corollary are
Page of ordered
in be
our
such
a
To recapitulate,
suppressed
, The
by
a child
21
above
can
that
to
sYnto
relationship
between
contrary
Processes
gives
innocent
g-
a systematic
the nature
by relationships
underlying
learner
.12 motivated maticallY processes likenasalization This
defined
natural
This
theory, prior
rise
to
are
therefore
"allo-
through
language.
nasalization speech
processes
vowels;
Vowel
of vowel denasalization
y are natural
of in
vowel denasalization
of the language.
by a prior,
in
any under -
vowels) in underlying representation
as any process
eliminates-nasal
in English, which
phonology , the nonoccurrence
to a process
,
An allophonic
does not neutralize
I n natural
is attributed
eliminated
superficially
like
Phonemic
representation
process
sounds which do not occur in underlying
process
prior
'allophonic'
Phonetic
creates
a language
The
course
but only in superficial
is any Process,
Phonological
English.
ordering
all
language-Hindi
superficially
by
can
phonemics . An
process
certain
processes in
representation
the language
of
in traditional
is a sound which does not occur i n underlying
lying distinction).
they
nasalization
simply
and
unde rlYing1Y ' in
innate
denasal-
vowels
since
counterinstances
processes,
impossible
which
by
,
invar-
acquisition
nasalized
counterinstances
vowels
can
nasalization vowels
be
of
The innate
The
allophone
'allophone'
due to a context-sensitive
nonnasal
language
.
though
.
underlYing1Y '
in
account of the notion
in
even
analysis
theory.
overcome
lost
representation
as
our
exists
never
speaker's
ot~m
of be
not
process
nasal
""n u
confront
denasalization
in
in
denasa1izing
process
is
surface
English
a process
such
can't
c ounterinstances
themselves
the
the
not
to no
kit in
corollaries
confronts
above
derived
to
as
from
~,
of
for
27
nasality
clearly
a nonnasal
vowels
/oun/comes
of
nasality
explanation
nasal
unless
absence nonnasals•
precedes
The
.
this
phonology: sPeech
that
capacity,
unless
they
is explained processes
confront
Vowel denasalization
lacking
vowels
all
nasal
vowels
and their
nonoccurrence
bY a basic
are expressions
counterinstances
altogether.
assumption of the
can be derived
through
of
by the language he
in a language
, in a language
nasalization
in
language-
in the language
is not overcome Similarly
s.13 : the phonic' 'allophones' of nonnasal vowels in
is systematically
and they are not overcome
is learning. nasal
of 'allophones'
o f processes,
representations
g
in which
processes
, it
29
28 suffices
to
order
denasalization
inability
to
produce
provided
such
vowels
Thus
the
that
if
they
must
in
all
so
It do
guages,
vowels
can
be
not
occur I
possible in
think
derivable
to
which
is fairly
be side
instead
of
aPP1Y,
see
flected the
vek ,
alternants
final
It
had [k]; as
there
is
a process
like
me from underlying
was like
and is k
ma
here occur
in
vowels
nasal
re mains.
sufficient
derivable various
mention
the
unde
that
are
' SaPit
Yiddish we have
process
ceasing
e.g.
representation.
avek
'road' to
out that has
that
bars
nasal
obstruents there
surely
distinction.
For example ,
bun to
'associations'
are neutralized
of final
words
like
ve9a one
are
might
be used
vowels
explain
words
this
the
results
one
view
somewhat
representations
obstruents
obstruents
of ae
k/ . , according Accordingly, it
neutral
-
colorful
in their
with like
unin -
that
contained
obstruents
voiced
for
cannot
conditioned
zeka
which require
for
versus
' contrast' 9
entirely,
the voiced
resembling
.
are
basically
of
from
, because which
to be suppressed.
In natural
paragraph
representation that
to pronounce
any process .uo9a
SaPit phonemic,
Bloomf with
phonology
.
that or
to
, from the acquisition
of the words in question underlying
representa-
would have been introduced
representations
as in this
for pre-
be derived voicing
9of
suppressed
e.g.ve 9
speakers
contextually
notion
of
underlying
obstruent
'bags'
Yiddish
ve9'roadt
ve9a , indicates
to account
k
after
e .gin
the voiced
of the pronunciation The
voiced
would have been impossible
there
in German there
voiceless In fact
is
in English
bars
term:
Traditionally,
invar-
a~ but it
which generally whereas
analyzed
avek
of these
a voiceless
as
as
or to our own .
to use the
of final
it
value
traditional
Plural
Otherwise
has to be analyzed
definition
'
even those
as [bunt].
vek
plurals
'away',
avok
bunda
devoicing
tions
systems
voiceless
i n German is not allophonic
traditional
forms
vious
1915
v9 e
process
The surfacing
vek
~"
since
in German can be taken at face
an underlying
the
English
'mor p ho p honemi
flected
effect
in Phonological
children. k
and
would have this
universal
to the
is
ones'
opposite
devoiced:
in the fact
generally
final
lan-
discussion
can invariably
representation;
but
izes
known from recent
Why wasn't
lies
youngest
features
representa-Y
to point
kin
the
either
in
for this
denasalization
from underlying
to
obstruents
The difference
no process
no
are
which
The devoicing
4Bu t in forms which did not have in-
in English,
analyzed
vowels
in
obstruents
reason
requires
nonnasal
of
t o aPP1Y, so that
segment
is not
examples
ceased On the
nonnasal.
it
In Lithuanian
with voiced
voiceless
from
no process
obstruents
overcome
e acquisition
roads'
StamPe 1969:453
as /ave/.? iably
process
as
we find
well
'road'
'
be
English.
do
in Germans in which final
devoicing
derived
suffice
which
ve9a
not
representations
occurs
the
be
multiply
will
features
tions. tions.A case
languag
underlying
it
need
exists
The
represented
in
in
nasalization
such
Consequently,
representations
but
case:
energy
derived
would
as
to
underlYing1Y
of
nonnasal be
vowels
are
conservation
underlying
which
nasal
prior
emerges le1d: '
is underlying
'morPhoPhonemes'
like
30 the
German
final
morphemes normal
voiced
whose
alternants
situation
form
to be
is
two
the
phonetic
all
forms
in
a system,
that
level that
owing
level.
The other
a form
from
the
has
' ac
This
be
which
's
of
are the
that
taken
level
is
underlying
traditional
the
specific
to
, its
a11
cannot
under-
'phonemic'
particular
pronunciation
forms: be derived
representation
representation
as
nature
of
emphasis
of
both
struc
of for process),
k voice
in
assumed
must
defined, as This
the
be
and
its
is
pointed that
out by Halle
substitutes
contexts trast
an 'archisegmeet'
Phonological
example,
in other
between
be captured process
in which
not
to
linguistic have
probably
predicted
embracing
by the k,
9
there
Russian
if
but
.)
stop obstruent However,
might
have required
source
of
J
argued
while that
substitutes,
substitutes
acquisition
possible
identity
process of
d
to suppress
and
(since,
con-
could
assimilation in traditional
however,
was taken it is a
We can assume process
J
in
i s barred
more general processes issue
whatever
from process
this
happens
whereas
assimilation
in nonderivable it).
t
substitutions
is not at
due to voice
voiced
distinction
assimilation
b y an earlier,
its
assimilation
d
the
of processes.
of several
has been
of substitutions
phonology,
just one voice
but
avek .
in certain
of the voice
phonemic status
one of its
speakers
t
and morphophonemic
by any earlier
be,~ the
for
interrelations
know which
not barred
d
Halle
representations
of the
and
In natural
that
representation
e .g.
This would have been true
the
lacking
underlying
has a voicing
the integrity
is indeed
to be in Russian another
theory a velar
where
of the
I don't
language-Particular
recent
being
but
that
sole
were denied.
underlying
structuralist
accidental,
and
Chomsk Y
'redundant' the
the
allophonic only
their
Russian c
positions.
these
corollary to
by
is
for
, these
to the view I am proposing
1958
J
this
process
phonemic representation
impediment
of such
from abstractions
Rather,
to be a primitive.
of representation
a&ay we would
voicing
rather
recent
similarity
descriptive
avek
in
for
one
phoneme
turalxst
differs
level
defined
unspecified.
basically
devoicing
barred
as
thebasic
phonemic',
the
unspecified
Processes
reflexes
the devoicing
do not derive
to their
An apparent
voiceless
suppressed
segments
is identical
of contrary from
invariably
any voice-specification.
affecting
as
deep
neutralized
than
of making
generally
in
instead
constraint
are
a
phonemics,
inherent
Thus
deeper
by the
sounds when Yiddish
, there
result
whose
underlying
has
This
features view
of
stematis
1964.
ts
of
general
representation
from
theory,
general
is
the
cordingly.
view
strikingly
least
as is indicated
that
representation
coexistence
the
constraint
alternan
Assuming
representations
the
individual
representation
properties has
in
them.
surface
the
at
phonemic
'deepened
to
This
representation
its drive
'allophonic'
lying
included
underlying
One is
representation.
if
the
with
constraints
nts
require
for
identical
are
obstrue
31
d
is
such a process contexts
would
u
32 Parallel a process
situations
that
a following tn.bed
occur
assimilates
stop. or
n
It
in bed
The substitution
of
m for
being
no general
process
other
hand
f o] occurs process This
only where
barring
can't
spellings
cisely
to English
markedness
Notice
.
Previous general
. derived
is capable
distinction
these
representation
o
theory
for
which
been
the spelling.)
rules
that
govern
called and
treats
there
and allophonic
rules,
and
that
this
distinction
is
arbitrarily
determined
dual
roles, if
its
process
general
16 /n! as is a
Pin
may govern
the
Rather in
und erlying
from the lexicon o utPut.
bY
Bey ond this,
but morPhe e n -sPecifis: unles s its
a specific
alternants
are
in the syst o m only from a deeper
of this
Parallel
progress
in all
the
Germanic
applying
at
before
less
sonorant
first
nut
eonsYllabic
consid er
h
hnutu
before
conclusion
deletes
hlaxan
ones
(e.g.
more
susceptible
hwe) govern
between
or , in the case of Proc-
that
phonemically
Russian
ocesses
barred
which eliminates
can be derived
In support
.
them
is not
that
imposed one.
any Process
output
is no
one for
The distinction
any subProcess
is not
they
to believe
~n
system:
There
representation.
treats
_
ides'
with
pre-
Phonotactic which
esses
the distinction
men-
underlying
those
Phonological
such that
is reason
by natural
given
languages
has tvo such processes,
processes,
morpheme is represented
for point-of-articulation, 9
English
in certain
in English
is not an extrinsicallY
is determined
an earlier
> since
phonemic'
that
of processes
which
En]-assimilation
any more than there
representation
expect
correspond
have assumed
(morphophonemic
_
words with
link
1968•~ Chapter
theories
redundancy
-'- En].
by the process
of accounting
variously
and
lint/
unspecified
have
it
In forms where no alternation
('Systematic
processes
roles since
~
are processes
has two voice-assimilation
N~' ~uue n, by children's
representations
between
these structure,
nuYen
representation
ChomskY & Halle
Phonological
representation morpheme
that
is
like
to believe
each role
The general
in foreign
e.g.
reason
there
is allophonic,
But there
have both roles
On the
representation
U mP
as underlYing1Y
. Neither
n
of
one
Iirlk , we would therefore
spelling.
theory
rlfor
each language.
one case,
m in English.
and so forth.
from underlying
above.
wn.keis
by assimilation.
representations
tioned
nasals
out
we have
boundaries:
is a morphophonemic
n
Tint
syllable or
substitution
from
IimP >
~, is barred
these
n
of
by this
point
across
underlying
be derived
underlying
of articulation
arises
ifrom l
e.g.
to the
ruling
it
Read 1971),
occurs
For example,
w,n.keis
substitution
is evidenced
which
the
the
in English.
is optional
Lm.bed
33
hwa ..i
wh~ than
whale retains
is sometimes lexical These hierarchic dialects.
segments
laugh,
hrin g
ones:
ring '
so that
(e.g.
older
and then before
hJ'
h but at onic
it has appeared
se gmeets
there
the process
ar
hew e dialects
In Fig. 1 are represented
four dialects
English
words are
in which
hwai why has lost
are ref 1ected
in,
more sonorant
Atonic
even restres sed why lacks
conditions
that
This proces s has had a dial ects
and eventually tonic
further
it.
This
Eh].
in modern English I have observed
~
34 personally;
the
application
obligatory
+ , optional
of [h]-deletion +
is indicated
or inapplicable
atonic /w
n s l br
35 as
In a theory
(- ).
representation
between
of lesser +
+
-
-
of greater
B
+
+
+
-
would be incapable
C
+
+
+
+
which lack
D
+
+
+
-
I-Fig. l.--[h]-deletion
in four
English
dialects
before
of generalization
of [h]-deletion
to the
least
susceptible
contexts
no doubt
that
we are
dealing
with
have
o f course
no trace
o f the
merged
hn, hl,
in these
of the
process
governs
h
a single
process.
hr] with
in alternations
lexical
n,
A deletion
alternants
like
representation.
is optional
before
hwai wai
why require
In dialect
B deletion
only
optional
in tonic
words;
in all
this
hw in their
process
in the case
of atonic
words
in these
governs
since
with
[h].
In dialect
C this
words
so that
whale
and wail,
general
with form,
1w!.
merging
In dialect the
lexical
there
ete
generalizations from C to D as represent
o f the process
(since , if
h
would be no reason superficial
coincidental
governin
does not occur
to posit
ation
developments.
g
before
an {h ]-deletion
representation).
But these
It is a single
Its
are not
process
that
distinct
roles
in lexical
representation
result
simply
from the fact
remain
when it
is obligatorily
and derived
are
reaches
that
n o Phonological
words of
h
• aration
of p rocesses
with
of the
theory
deleted
dual roles .
.
There is
11
of underlying
representation
Presente
is extended is
that
underlying
segments
are
ontologically
of
the
same
are
mental
identically its
as
any
segment
in
surface
representation
sounds
which
are
in
Particutar
the
most
of hue and you
representations •,. nounceable.
with
del
s
diachronic
underlying
here.
lexical
role
for example,
representations
loss
they exhibit
lexical
D the process
the
simultaneous
governing
simultaneous
governing
status
represented
dialect
but only optionally
is generalized
here
to tonic
no English
it would present
of the process
A corollary no alternants
Such a theory
words;
no s representation
nonsYllabics
underlying
is obligatory it
are
much
dialects
w in atonic
therefore
.
and the development
leaving
traces but
noesY1labics
before
altogether
example
two _processes,
representation
separate In dialect
deletion
why there
for
before
, the
All
I, r],
therefore
representation
/j/,
representation
dialects
nonsYllabics, original
optional
underlying
from the most
superficial dialects
of /h/
from A to C as coincidental
and a coincidental leaves
nonoccurrence
Furthermore,
a generalization susceptible
derived
of explaining
1w!.
governing
governing
and its
h/ before
of each of the The pattern
processes
sonority y would be missed
development
A-D
the
sonority
A
h
separates
from those
connection
tonic wJ
which
of They
are
not,
, at
/j/. the
'archisegments'
of
structural
and
least ,
in
principle,
pro-
semi-abstractions
generative
phonology
like .
In
36
31
is not identified voiceless
p/ of pin or the voiced
voicelessness dictable: spin
is
is not distinctive obstruents
are
considered
b
of bin
there
being
no shin
after
Es].
Rather
voiceless
a labial
Archisegments
came into
view of underlying
CourtenaY the
(1895),
oomfield
phonemes
1933
and others
view of Saussure
1933
and others
tures.
As Twaddell
phoneme as a set sion
that
voiceless
are
P3 b
b
of bin,
evidence
identical, after orthographic
spontaneous
spellings
orthography
Ze, mash/smash,
in Wright
stops:
bash
the mental-
In the
these
school
to its
sought
the redundantly
s
that
as opposed are
the p' s of spin to the b of bin,
phonologically
tradition
voiceless.
in English
--WISPRT whispered, TID started
with little
s.
segments,
knowledge
SCICHTA Read 1971.
of
As the
empirical
of
pin
it is remarkable
th
1 view of the phone
e t al .
as a basic
the Phonologfists
theory
8)
t
our rather
leaves
cited e:
the
archise
(1954)-and of the
Halle
in
the
archise
previous
(Stanley .
of
ver
gmental
1967
the
ies
'sy
stematic
proble
ms
, ChomskY & Ha11e
ofarchi -
segments
indeterminate.
indicates
of
,
ultimately
formal
paragraph
ental
e
1962 , Chomsky 1964).
certain
identity
id not
were never
property does noteliminate thecritical
since evidence
d
Trubetz koY (1936)
Halle
phonology
features
at Twaddellts
, it was explicitly
1935
to eliminate
18
who followed
for or against
1951
voiced
me treats
facts
characteristic
of orgenerative
.
a s nonidentical
o r another
unch
to words with etc
evidence
with unspecified
not In
nor
scr
of Englashh doublets
but not scrouc h
segments
Twaddell
of this
indeterminat
simply
added
But the psychological
Jakobson
Chapter
pe , NOSTR English There are monster SKEEIG skiing, STAR
identical
In one version
theory
associated
not
a rchisegmenta
F romkin
in crunch
a long list
by many Phonologfists-Firth
phonemic'
There is
areoflanguages) writing p t k
the
It was incorporated
1968:
There
that
1951
and that
and comparable
than b d 9 after
of children
and pin
grouch
sed in slips
for whisper s
is never
of such evidence
representation.
hwibsr
etc .
face
neither
i s transpo
intensive
1905:242-44)
distinguish attempted to find
Harris
P of pin and the
when /s
not
that
abandonment.
out:
accepted
to the conclu-
fact
phonologically
lead
fea-
hwips r
but not spash,
observation
view of the
is also
s tr
The revision
rather the
there
is the
occurs
'distinctive'
voiceless
There
un-
which distin characterized solely entities by those
leads
1971.
p, b
'functional' Bl
this
features'
distinctively
is abundant
stops sto
persistent
recognized,
e.g.
trample
by
which appear
tongue,
pof
the Prague
their
stops
the
of spin.
phonologically
in general eneral the
the
voiceless
But there
first
of 'distinctive
besides
distinctively
1934
but pre-
was supplanted
phonemes are
phonemes in a language,
of the
after
properties them from other
its
of Baudouin de
1959,
that
the voiceless
stop assPecified to voice.
theory
segments
SaPit
an timers talist
Phonological
thetheories those the Pofspin
because
an 'archisegment'
specified
ist
with
it
is
representation
are
correct. theory than
the that
identification of
bin
tape follows
of
the
from
stop the
spin fact
with that
that there
is
,.
38 no
process
that
sibilant
context
sibilant
voiced
earlier
in
depends,
eliminates
voiceless
example
skou
let's
go).
words
correct
principle
system of
the
subtlety
of
and
from
generality
taken
ants
orb
the
that
face
value
Y a
g eneral
representation. fully
specified
as
This conclusion language
assumption
is clear)
to
devised
identif he
seed
they
But
there
underlying find -
some -
-
is
'archise
their
under)
the
are
no
--
undoubtedly
it
is
obvious
all
prevented them
underlying in
true
segments ea
Sapit
to an understanding
of and e r 1g in
inventories:
to
P,
b
of
'archise
g ment'
alphabets
which rather
the
segment _ _1 -
P
of
spin, m
pin to
n
furnish than
and m
of
b
of
limb
lint,
and
graphic segments.
inventories
laws
alter-
structure
of
which
is
laws
high
of
Jakobson's
so
the Y Y n
of
Zin-t
of
t actual
(-count:
the
the
inventory
implic
vowels
these
the
loss
ational
laws
processes front
unrounding V palatal !lower
.
from
the
and
o f phoneme the P of
a
vowel.
se e ks
and
unr ound ones
derives
-'- -round
imPlica-
e governed
the
vowels:
the
These
id
For example,
front
corresponding
th e loss
systems
nature
the
im ' lies
and
1 Processes,
he called
implies
the m corresponding
that a r phoneme
phonolagica
of
change
vowel
in-
stratified
vowels
and
in
to Jakobson
o f a mid vowel high
of
hat
so forth
acquisition
acquisition
presuppose
process
and
indicate 1972a9Asthese s
their
in the
round
vowels
assumes
study
governing
occurrence
o f front
Miller
of phonemes
According the
corresponding the
by Patricia
inventories those
([l939]l968)
Jakobs on discovered w
implies
also
implies
of
sPonding
for
necessary
rt1
govern
stud
of vowel tudies
implication'
unround
context-free
~relations
round
forth.
sounds.
front
Miller's innate
of
occurrence
thus
vowel
are unable
and
of
acquisition
out,
bin
the
occurrence
further 1n her
independent
phoneme
to the
and carried'
'unilateral
the
processes
by TrubetzkoY
the
of mid vowels
inventories;
This
are
of
context-free
d'ustification
governing
surface
vowels
tional
each
indirectly
underlie
nFr~4 rrht-cnrt.r
gP
196$)
principles
universal
Y
the
systems
segments
high
b
inaugurated
study
of
equivalents It
1941
occurrence
out
irical
and Jakobson
are
ruled
question.
inventories
ventories
of how alphabets
representation
es-~m
all
that
has pointed
is-r-~st
which
ures.toas
1925).
in
languages
are
assumption
language
one,
of
not
honological
i
underlying
honetic
the
there
but
by their
from
As McCawley 1968:89
'
is
the
from
paragraph it
consciousness
critical
of
is
the traditional
speakers'
archisegmeans account
This
last
in the
To summarize,
bars
of
L2 f segments
was cited
phonemes which is at least
ent'
add
is
that that
theories
these
this
this
processes
learned.
the
post-
of
in the
not
post-
identification
identification
means
and used.
mental
add
it
has an inventor to its
archise
the
rescues
accessible
are
of
unless
to
This
since
cited
process
This
ones
of English.
evidence
this
eliminating
interrelation
identification
stops) at
on the
affecting
a process
of
Phonological
are
is
in favor
natural
nonsibilant
there
stops
stops
in other
the
voiceless
whereas
the
39
to
possible
law that from
bY
derive in terfront a corre-
41
40 ,The process historical dP>
exemplified
is
change, dePdee
lower
vowels
round
p.
are
There
vowels
Mxddle
As is
indicated
more susceptible k sen oxen is
law does
e.g.
English
>
process Process,
the
mastery
front
of
predict
what
it
need
not
1947
fir
notation
front
for
mastery
untoand
of front dueConsequently,
vowels.
happen
law can a primitive
and its
to front
It
imPlicational
Shift,
can
be extended
to other
from does process andsince t
of phonological
as Miller or
chromatic
it
is
also
to
a process
true
palatal f only or that
low
raising
round
vowels
chromatic
vowels,
and
for
such
imply
mid vowels.
This
vowels
one degree
of height:
dence
that
also
exemplified
in children
laws.
the
the extent
vowels
law that that
raising)
thongization,
[dip)
deep, [w1 eptibility susc bone. The grea
illustrated
by
a k] > [week weak os > 9
of lower vowels to raising
is ter
heweooptso the aliyof loth century Englishk
mouse u
Chehalis,
exceptions
to this
processes.
> mi i s
mice
Vowel left
a
In StamPe 1972 I give evi-system with , the raising
American Indian and in Pashto
high vowels
of
to mid.
imply high vowels
is not overridden
by conflicting
e
languages,
and other
are lowered
of course, processes
that
underlie
leap
the
explanation
underlie
a e.g.
languages In other
is true
process
just
to
in this
processes
diPh-
from systems
systems
referred
to systems
of the
the system,
to phonetic
causalities.
causalities
involved
attempted
causalities.
In Miller's
of Phonological
must
to the phonetic
Jakobson
to phonetic
many exceptions.
in turn
of phoneme systems
the processes.
confront
amples
the basis
as in the English
Phonological
that
are
are
preceded
mid vowels
are
of other
mis
he proposed
these goose,
vowels,
s,
case the
lowering.
go beyond the
Leopo
there
the corresponding
Ultimately,
corresponds
!lower
interference
In several
lax
In this
law.
that
diPhthongization
(presumably
shows, lawthat The midvowels imply high vowels is case true,
1943) and in hi storicalcal us
but no
Yokuts and Southern long
however,
> m s
o
this
make a direct
i
imPlicational
of high tense mus
causalities
This process
of applicability
by over a century.)
The
implicational
hierarchy
law due to the
where
words reasoning
wake obligatorily.)
must be noted
e
round P
be derived
> [wk]
the corresponding
The
theory. Similar
[wk]
DiPhthongization
vowels.
by a child ifthey vowels the areor not adult.
be considered
process
•ire, in
than higher ones, ~r ire remained f YOid e.g. English ~
the
will
week while
and
[!lowed,
rounding
unround~.n$
he implicational process,
>
to unto unding ' eksenwhile
pronounceable
the
fYr
the
not
Leopold
by the
no context-free
implies
im licational
children
e.g.
to round
in
work, processes,
I have sketched above on pages
22-24.
to
The laws phoneme and some exAs we
have seen, processes respond to phonetic difficulties in ways that > wik t t ti n are appropriate but which may conflict with other processes. The
_
~i l
42 systems
enables of processes inclusion 1n the theory of phoneme
CHAPTER Miller
to
account
straightforwardly
formed
errant,
vowevowelsystems as
which
Jakobson's
and
remain
which
for to
discover
hidden
does
not
many
many
from
allow
vowel
the
such
There
single,
returns
us
coherent
speech partially
to
my remark
but
f or rather
contradictory
purposefulness
of
characteristics
dealing
these
with
a set
a biological
standpoint
The
animal mal
is
capacity ,
is
is
the of
not
specific the
imperfectly
itself,
adapted
expectations that
often
cross-
which
from
speech.
man
is
aspects
of
to
discover,
for
ultimately
it
Limitations
cope
i
Arabic
a u
A11 of
of
these
vowel
trnY
with
the
existence
A A a are
systems. optimal
Abkhax
readily
, and
3-vowel i
accommodated
Phonetic distinctions
of
e a
of
by
as
of
n
studies
o f perceptuall
between
sounds
must
diverse LeoPold
Miller's
process Yand be
that
t
h onemess govern
such
Yterns systems.
themselves
but
rather
to
the
language processes
hedge
that of
occur
speech
.
is well-justified,
be satisfying.
which
processes
of
remain.
for
in
the
In
certain or perception production (p
linguistic
'as-if'
type
calls
of of grammar
but
ExPlanations
descriptions
language,
view
petence'
competence/performance
of
basis
such
of
limitations
the
when less
performance
are
explanations. required
by
a divide-and-conquer
limited
the
presuppose data
data
are
will
not
expanded.
as . theory
that be This
theories
devised s y
fundamentally presupposition
could
processes
than
in
the
case
of
the
competence/ be hardly
es increasin
1
clear
that
of
Performance
optimal systems L
general
are
the
conditions
responsible
means
for
theory
I
the
nature
of
striking
'1'he
of
the
a peculiarity
Particularly
articula-
referred
plausible
dichotomy, use of
phonology,
language
language.
This
is
but
phonology
assumes
that
systems
and
superficial
not
Presents
by
any
a
case. am presenting
of
phonological
not o .y processes
to
actual
'as-if'
the
But
inadequate
1972 perceptuallycan vowels the
Hildegand
the
to
his adapted evenfor evolving sound-systems
systems
systems
cannot
complexity
on the
in hardly
the
this
bas ed on
trate
imperfectly
indblom
of
linguistics
hard) Recent Re imperfections. attempts
of
in linguistics
'erformance')
the
Further-
minimize
not
OF PROCESSES
speech.
for
for
descriptions
and
phenomena conflicting
1 oonly al y secondarily
after
our
and
of
a tendency
a
of
a reflex
speech
furnish
limitations
highly
merely
for
have more, we Jakobsonian
did
Undoubtedly
processes
the
nature
with
Processes.
of
speaking
that
been
as speakers,
system
capacity,
has
competition
processes . 20 among This•
THE ORGANIZATION
of formerly seemed aberrant,
viewpoint
natural
II
which
regularities
a static
for
systems
of
are
utterances
related
by
real really
the
actual
that
the
exist agency
underlying and
that
they
o f these 43
are
processes.
representations
constrained It
and assumes
inter-
that
., .
45
44 when processes tions
perform
occurring
Without
be a
these
in the performance
of utterances. merely
substitutions,
these
are actual
mental
as well
assumptions,
model ' : furnishing
the
not literal
substitu-
as physical)
theory
evidence occur
cited
in Chapter
in the mind.
striking
fact:
but only analogical
of the
or
ste ipfor
khat
of the
stops
with
pronounced
tat
for
spa9r E t
with
aspiration
here,
but
various
memory processes in
tongue
the
tape
or
spaghetti,
do not
.
the
or
of
studies. which
utterances
apply
after
processing
of
which
not
slips
occur
occur,
constraints
ar
phonetic
behavior.
examples
occasionally
and therefore
etc.,
be given E
1971
scramblings
of
in the
cites
features,
short-term
Phonological must
demonstrably
also
aPP1Y
d
other
but those
slips'
of speech,
e.g.
by Pronouncing
which they
Now, the
significant
they
these
create
and alternations,
one h ears
after
PalatalitY
k itty
and
electricity
Fromkin
41),
is
behavioral
constraints
which are
d
The distinction
a puzzle
readily
perceived
between
is
by speakers.
not
fail
and
simply
r. n~n n ~.~o r a n l'r~ n h tinr. n.•. .-1..- n.... n .+Y. ...+..
our
like
..,
,...
this kar
9,E.n Lk l. sa' by
1.._
is
of
an
z. zPs occur
Thus
innate Phonological the behavior.
constraints
constraint s
this
if
Phonetic
acquired
An innate
n 9i)
spoonerism
governed
govern
and
place,
sometimes
e would expect
as
which
innate
s
first
alternations
but
do
Children
in the
fro m reading
sl'
learned
[si,
to Romance derivatives.
spooner is readjusted after process, ism occurs. become
k
vowels in
and 9e t with
ive to sli ps: the
9],
of
adult s say pedago[g]y,
•iz
of
which
learned.
just
abo ut learned
insensit
beside
learly
s
or learn
rule
applied
with
learn
d3intkl
the
is
sub stitutions
thing
are utterly
k
words like
guys is not
Note that
electric
pedagogy c [d3],
merely
govern
constraints
[d3] bef ore reflex es of frontt
limit
uho± hontheories ological The t P
the alternation
For example,
utterances. when learned,
t
tha
ng if these
Phonologi cal constr aints
to show the alternation
w learned
however,
from the outset
do
learned.
9beside
and then
cynical
innate.
speed h.
in adult
Some words they
that
clearly,
shows and
than
It would b e surprisi
are
do not begin
could
one, • Fromkin
,
Phonological
in many ways from th o se involv ing processes,
pedagogue
s 9ep~:i
~
in speech -production.
constraints, after
are
in
9aspEr i,
language
child
most
rather
learned
words of Romance origin,
we have ,
y readjusted;
a novel
occur
i,
are
with Es] and [g] with
slips
Similarly
skapEc
Slips
individual
g ~.ca1
sk,e ..,p i
we do not find
Many further is
to the result that
of
of
assumption
the
constraints
different
by a
are adjusted
sthep i
the
accept
There are
t he ip , the aspiration
and Palatalit
observation
storage
kat
occur.
sequences
apply
of the [ki
that
substitutions
like
skat
skebEr i,
, voicing,
'
processes
we
did not persist
given the
is corroborated
new contexts:
sk ei
How fhonolo P constraints
reality
Thus in slips
scotch
their
doubtful
Phonolo g ical
Phonological
corroborative
segments,
I that
and the PalatalitY
accordance
g,'aspri
may appear
But their
that
of 'slips
and
of reality
if
evidence
would
explanations. The assumption
only
represents
is a
,
47
46 for
a monolingual
English
speaker
to
suspend
the
innate
processes.
process
it governing
aspiration
syllables;
this
k hatste
of i s
„~Pfor
of tonic
voiceless
reflected
in
skat
his
thoJP
to aspirate
is perceptible before
certainly
etreatment of the in
scotch tape,
J
as in
pat+t
his
petite,
aspiration
and in his
be suspended
beside [gatje]
but it requires
etious In the following
to
discussion
innate
honolo ical processes
it is essential
that the distinction
for
these
two
classes
of
have
a
failure
work
Recent
characteristics. to
make b Ymarred
in
phonology
has
between
di the stinction
are
by
governed
properties
f processes
conversely,
certain
have properties
acquired b een o of
optional,
may be optional;
in fact
to
have
been
fact
it
one,
istinction
brings to
and
the
supposed
in s P eech of the
infant
Old
English
shortened:
before
from
applying
distinct
might processes cases
in sequence. as
rules
nts
it,
which which
are
is
the
the
language
reflected
in
P roduction.
failure
to distinguish willingness which
origins. there
between
to
attribute
seem quite
For example, were
to
out
of
KiParskY
two contexts
1968
in which
clusters
, and before
processes
e.g.
two-consonant
vowels
bremblas clusters 'brambles' ble"tsi'an
certain As.a
from on the mistaken assumption that all
bletsi'an
shortening English
and,
attributed
The
I understand
constraints
three-consonant
braernb las
been
wind
contrary
origins
characteristics
their in
seemingly
rules,
rules
processes
and canstrai
an uncritical
cognitive with
language
certain
individual
between
roles
been
that
that
and
whose
result
has
that
in mouse/mice,
speaker,
ct
processes
a to
the
rules
those
processes
an absolute
brings
like
and that
processes
speaker
because
substitutions, several
acquired
hand
appears
between
were
quite
in
of an optional
other
been
distinction
notes
andrules and
result
rules b
attributed rules
are
processes
constraints
substitutions
sometimes
involve
'ambles' different
also
case
On the
do so;
keeping
'rules',
be kept
constraints
has
Another
between
and acquired Phonological
it
their
eZectrickit in thefac apparent y.
overningthe g 1 k s alternation'n z English
~n mind ,
as is
and how tthey are organized into systems.
In this
f irml
substitutions
pages I will discuss so me characteristics
of P honolo gi ca t pprocesses
ike that p
difficult presents electricity Y whatsoever,
make radical
actually
got you, can
some conscious effort
pronunciation
many Processes
rules
clear
rule.
make minimal
apical
k in electric! obligatory substitution ofacquired s for no articuiatory
because
that
know a single
likewise
the other hand, to suspend thesodoin actual discourse. On
resents
supposed
phonological
wound
processes,
The process palatalizing 9atJ 'a
been
I do not
spoonerism
Even in the case of optional
difficulty.
has
example,
the
stops in words like Gurkha where the stress
does not fallow the stop.
obstruents
at
treatm
stops in French wards like
failure
there
stops
For
to
.
The latter
ChomskY and Halle speakers
unreasonable
had
to assume
is nowadays 1968
to learn
these
further
that
,
referred
On the
assumption
constraints since
to as
both
it
is
involve
'bless' (roughly) followed by twounaccented syllables (e.g.
trisYllabic ~ that
Old
not shortening
49
I V
of
vowels
speakers
Generative
would
have
phonological
binations
of
rules:
V
}
generalized
theory
their
furnishes
common
a device
for
properties
such
And for
.
V -} -long
com-
This reanalYsis
-long
cc
CC
VCV To show of
that
this
combining
the
Middle
English
by one
consonant:
E .g.
argues,
. it
is
were the
cognitive
analysis
makes a single
have
not
way
English
in
early
matron. these
tightened
. represents
two-consonant As for
by the failure
three-consonant
e.g.
hiehsta
typically
clusters
involve tendency
As in Modern English,
of shortening clusters
'highest'
cases
bletsi
a weakly accented of this
English,
Middle
.CC, e.g.
like
medial
ader
ad,e)res
re sponden
~'an
divinity,
vowel,
vowel to be syncopated
e.g
to
which were
or before
syllabicated
the trisY llabic
VCV
the
changed
of
of
this
versa,
implied
of
imposed should
by the
i t turns
been
incorrectly
analyzed syllables
otherwise
it
this on
. and
a consonant
KiParsk Y
zeroing
as is
both in
'father/father's'.
to
the
examples
which
at
to its
When this
oddity
in fact
The correct in trisYllabic
-)- -tong a syllable
left:
There
is only
brackets
analysis
simple
contexts
change
singly-closed CC. boundary.)
,
we are ological
di.vin
i
eliminating reality
the had
only
the syllable
syllabication,
of brmmblas and husband.
responding
to closed
under the proposed for shortening
In eliminating
been
divi-
by the same closed-syllable
conditions
syllables.
into
Given ' this
from Old to Middle English
because
of the
divinity
d o v, a, nE i. n
consonant
the shortening
the one process,
is needed
da,vrc.a.ti '"
contrast
the preceding
for
the change
,
zCr.a.wi ~
vowel in the Old and Middle English
~'an
responsible
to explain
the
blets.i.
[n],
words were shortened
condition is
in manY dialects)
forced
of supporting
compare Modern English
of syllable-final
likewise
IrisYllabic of
syllable;
The weak medial
odd
sort
a weak vowel is incapable
z i ro
flapping
seems
by its
in its
beside
nation).
changes
language
that
with
unconnected
same way.
sort
be subject
out
divinity
nonetheless
the
brackets.
thoroughly,
in closed
bracket,
in the
argument,
changes were
the
why these
simultaneously
beauty
vice
reality
'stirrups',
process: V
C.CC
Old and Middle
more
shortening
syllabicated real that
were
Old English
C
to explain
inception
and
investigated
simultaneously
s t2rope ~ s > st ti ropes
motivated
their
out
is motivated
C
possible
speakers,
changes
constraints
points
C.
aPP1Y before
shown by the
phonetically in
a psychologically '
Kiparsky
By recognizing
Granting
least
shortenings,
> husband
constraints
that
represents
-~ -long
husband
> divinity
device
both
V
Middle English:
example evidenced
in for
no recourse analysis
brackets
historical in
to
it is a
from doubly-
the brackets which
syllables;
in this and a sound
to case psychchange.
51
50 Another the
cognitive
formulation
extended
would them
not
+
processes
principles
governing
in
any
language.
In
by
the
nasality
a process process would
example),
superficial
nasality
it
all
impossible
nasality
tautosYllabic and
clearly
indicates. denasalization
this
Phonological
against
the
speech,
vowels
does not occur,
is
as
before
impossible
analysis: thereby
the
Perceived evidence
determined
of
analysis
cited
in
there
is
when segments
deleted
are
nasalized,
of the
My third
processes
involved just
this
ordering
argued
I
process
in casual e.g.
Sons'. cussion,
1-1- e'ma
nt'i r~a1--
a avnnea h1fI n li Z1? 'f:"''?
of the English
196$
e.g.t
I have attempted
he rule
here is in the
proposed
the formation
is no reason
in
phonologic al
analysis
of the use of cognitive of processes
bY
of the imperfect
to expect
logical
reflecting
is the trisYllabic didn't
exist,
devices
concerns
of contradictory
innate
a recent
Processes. shortening
S Anderson
appropriateate proposal
One of the process,
(1969:
which i i
137-43
CV C V of Middle English e. g , baleen
were lowered:
Anderson's
which lengthened
stressed
OE bacan
The vowels
V '
formulation
simplified
-I-long 1-high
etc .
wik wekks `week/weeks ' , Sun SaI28S ' Son
is: +stress
.3lcl -3- I h1 I n l -+ r hi 1 f11. }»iman
device
capacity.
in open syllables
so lengthened evidence
of the variable
is
process
and another vowels
rules
it It
may seem as a strategy
there
speech
in the analysis the
scat
to be imposed on constraints
example
regarding
-}
What I wish to emphasize
to describe
V + -long
explains
are
limitations
in
context-
unequivocal
356-7
sort
cites
a
Chapter
between
nasalization
to nasalization
before
of acquired
devices
to rules
variable
the device
languages,
of this
9- stE "a
by ChomskY and Halle
plausible
formulation
denasa lization
has been employed in recent
of in Stampe 1912.
however
I have
invariant
the merits
in West Semitic
is
as nonnasal
distinguishing
Besides,
fact
here
proposed
ChomskY and Halle
in
senate
Vowel Shift to dispose
reciprocal
s"iat
The controversial
apparently,
English
occurring
to discuss
mental
application
with
e.g.
analysis
by a context-sensitive
invariably
and context-sensitive perception.
to
to the hypothesized
analysis.
that
nonnasals.
segment
due
make
this
be reciprocally
why vowels
the
also
The reciprocal
phonologically
The analysis
exposed
is
there
vowels
each case where it
of
that
to
nasals.
vowels
languages,
reciprocal
elegance
and,
followed
to explain
are
similar
free
before
would
in English
vowels seems
vowels
i.e.
nasal
English
been
which
to be an incorrect
appearance
vowels
but
of a following
this
denasalizing
make
has
nasal
For example,
nasals
I argued
denasalizing
vowel
nasalizing
seems
I,
or nonnasalitY
an apparent
which
in
as reciprocal
7) .
nasalization
Chapter
process where
a role
variable
The cognitive it
the
languages
before
nonnasals.
of
In
but
alpha
Chapter
nasal
notwithstanding,
no Phonological
the
play
nnasal
before
formulation
is
1968:
make vowels
nonnasal
rules
to be expressed
anasal
only
indeed
Phonological
ChomskY and Halle V
might
to processes
assimilation
changes
which
of acquired
uncritically permits
device
CV
slightly
for
this
dis-
,
52 He notes
that
single
if
form,
in which eveZ
they
they
give
incorrect
evil
do not
form
result
from
sumer-e
shortening
x
x
lengthening
*eveles
notes
that
would
not
either
order
s
had somor
had
explainable
sumere
ivel-es
sumer-es
lengthening
eveles
sameres -
* eveles "
*someres "
shortening
lengthening
properly
includes
the
context
of
the
shortening
Citing
a parallel
proposes
that
process
and
the
principle
when
two processes
applies
and
examples
cited
correct
forms
the
general
the
2veZes
one
each
does
lengthening and
not .
process
sumeres
would
,
.
or
a grammarian
would
be
totally
here
makes
no
like point
of
rules
proposed In any processes,
which are
b y its
disjunctive event
the by
would
positing
the
are
about
formulate
a specific
b Y a more
These
language
as a hypothesis Panini
in
eradicated
language. the
sense
general
imposed
rules; rule
rule.
on learners
Phonolo$ical
how a language
changes
if But
the
correct it
by the
1 earners.
And it
is no t at
principle
should
be a pPlicable
principle way that
seems
to be fals ified
allophonic
prope rties
were
This
s
all
of
imposed clear to
at
of
speaking
why processes.
fact
on is
e .g.
iv.e.les,
these
e.g.
ivies,
syllabicated
on
by the
the
surnres. hand
clusters with
the
nonoccurrence the
short
the
preceding thus
least
for
left,
sounds
are
The same
consonant closing
syllabications
the
vowel and
of
these
It
apparently it
so that words
out
the
and
special turn
for
due
ated
the
bY
len
gthening
'son /sons'.
clusters
before were
is
also
confirmed
like
c2
man
weak
syllable
lengthening persist
,
that
in Middle
was too
into
C
lengthened
these
.
pointed
so . nes
were
in words
forced
syllable
when
syllable
lengthening I
vowels
patron
following of
final
context
such
. Again
corrobor
The correct
in ' which
, e.g.
is
in sun
that able.
was in
variants
as
to which
English?
their
syncopated
was
b y words
this
e
would
fact
was of
g th
nasals
inexplic
which
syllable;
have
before
make the
because
sum .e.res,
medial
forms
other
two-consonant
where
forms
confirmed
the
the
irisYllabic
up to
to any vowel
process
I
denasalizln g
proposes
I have already
to
of
fully
of Middle
shortening
that
the
for
is
as nonnasal
the
weakness
English,
effects not
context
This
vowels
not apply
.
Chapter
Anderson
two processes faulty
in
process
condition
Perceived
the
shown
representations
apply , and would
see m; to be
syllabication,
rule,
nature
, processe
learner
about
general
would
later
was
nasalizing
disjunctive
phonologically
applied
the
the
process
As
nonnas al .
governs
den asalization
what
analysis
there
we are
are
that
specific
would
the
obviously its
that
nasalization
the
assume
the
.
underlYing1Y
o f context
But
predict
that
are
we
the
applies.
to
result. This
if
But
contradict
vowels
that
vowels
of Panini's
representation
regardless
point
form
underlying
English
vowels
of application:
base
*so metes
three-syllable
apply
strm©r , it
in
all
the
specific
the
for
English
ignored
order
of
Anderson
in
2 el , Middle
the
context
CV C V .
Thus
of
the
process
the
regardless
formulated
CV
other,
aPP1Y to a
as
process
grammar,
, a s formulated,
results
and
ivel-es
Anderson
53
processes
For Old English
'evil/of
forms
base
these
aPP1Y
iveles
These
both
did
et
c.,
to carry to the not
a
pply.
in Modern English
55
54 P„ei.tran
note
2
sir.
[me. i
syllable-final zation of
of
a vowel
C,
principle
sumeres
of
disjunctive like
do
real
seem
the
sequence.
all
of
that
have
be.
The most
the
been
seems
process
on
that
for
this
to
ized
sequential
an optional
optionally segment
nasalized:
kLi as
hii r~
in
the
beyond which
e.g.
process
vowel
of
as
that
flap '
involves
could
the
dependence
of
the
the
first
optional
process,
in
It
seems
rapid. is like
in
beside
s~i
deletes
flaps
, e.g
k Lri
applies
before
, the
preceding
vowel if
the
a nasal-
in
won
In
the
output
button
not
bra?n " )
wodn each
process
is
wooden
of
these
strictly
of a separate
obvious
explanation
anon
applies
to
apply
to the
output
simultaneous
the
operations
and
for
output
of
this
the
other
are
package'
due
I propose
an example,
1
and
of
i n (2-1)
to linear
explaining neural
an alternative
unfortunately
rather
be much more
one after
8)
it
In
generative in fact
been
The assumption
and
b99' o lz
has
who argues 2
labial
hamn'-um
sequence
the
processing another
has
1969
syncope
bo99ul2
could
line .
in some words , e.g.
} ho"m rum
possibility as
of
processes?
of speech
ordered.
by S. Anderson
sequence
the
out
Chapter
linearly
processes
the
strung
1968•
of other
neurology
on an assembly
however,
1-2
the
are
Halle
processes
application
that
processes
'to
of processes
give
that
-} hamrum
on
and
conjecture
the
Before is
that
challenged,
Icelandic
doubt
nasals:
ation
o f the
of an optional
before
not nasaliz
output
or novaPPlication
nasaliz
ChomskY and
a99ul-i
is flap
and
that
hammers'
And of the
consonants
processes
consecutive
been
[bra?]
true
sequence.
obvious
linear,
nasalizes seen
that
We might
applied
spin
nonnasal
unrelated
The same is
glottals.
of the
application
is
occur,
w"n o n
on the
a doubt . Consider vowels
voiced
dependent
assumed
.
b nn ).
application
phonology
process
deletion
remains
not
not
optionally
Why should or
if
they
of
the
process,
The arguments
an optional
, and
discussed
hSriqhztt2n9' This
of
operation
If
.
cases,
, ffor
most
sequence
wodn
not
deleting
nasalizing
not
does
process b nn
dependence
processes
that
or nonaPPlication
obligatorily,
kit
problematic
Consider
as conclusive
output
application
segments is
not
commonplace
I have
There
the
aPP1Y simultaneously
I am aware
the
o f English
see.
apply
can be under-
accepted
optionally
are not
h"ci ~ q
an optional
process
did
of
functions.
aPP1Y in
are
the
nasal
nasali-
The contexts
to
these
processes
evidence
fairly the
many of
to be generally
their
before
.
deleted;
of
of
corrected
appealing
phonetic
establishes Process
nasal}
of why lengthening
in a system
are
typical
obligatory
, as
without
their
conclusive
this
the
to some characteristics
of a process
like
and
lengthening
of whether
given
n
not
syllable-final
flapping
a tautosYllabic
to show
of
processes
application
second
and
It
and
solved
to turn
question
in
of
application.
reflections
example,
Cases
is
instead
obligatory
The problem
or
I would
as
the t
and
exclusive,
iveles
stood
note
preceding
shortening
that
t
iratervocalic
mutually to
syllable-Initial
umlaut 'to
in others
This
sequential
that
the , - e.g.
conclusion
casts
application
structure,22 explanation complex,
, I would that
throws
like
to
somewhat
r
56 clearer
light
processes
on the nature
than do the
57
of the nonlinear
examples
is chosen
from American English
readily
confirmed
by the reader.
three
flapping,
require
but if
attaches
the syllable
to its
left
attached
to the
syllable
responsible
which ical
for
it can be several
are
,
syllabication
ego,
,
filter.
Bothe
veto.
t€ r• e. b !
is subject
above in Old and
to various
a, dP a t
Left-attachment:
syllabication
from phonologisis
are
to mention existence not
relevant spectrograh formants
vowel
structure
The
application
is
has not received
are
many
to
the
example
include
Palatalization
palatal
vowel,
ig,.]
eagle
o r phoneticians,
stops
only
syllable,
e.g.
dis.t
the fact
that
morphologically table
some of the phonetic and location
difficult cues.
to 'hear' The chief
P Ys are undergo
digress
and Phonological
of syllable
boundaries
.
it deserves briefly
evidence Syllable
once one has become conscious phonetic
the modifications
in ta utosyllabic
of the
cues , which are verifiable ('transitions') contact
with various
that
boundaries
tion
vowel
stages
.
* ri{
,
be one
would Prior
arise
be .
Others
contact okey
,
a
and
to
in a stre
ssed
sense
the stable.
, as is shown b y
syllable
in casual
divisions speech
ns of distaste
the
and this
, syllabication them
syllable .
as it attaches
For example, sno~
Otherwise
words
• . ,
rr . I
real
,t
! •4kt .rc k
and de sYllabifica intermediate
containing
or n _l
optionally
with simultaneous
exactly
optionally
chapge .
to de syll abification
with
ma, st e i k mistake,
The attachment
n-syllable
.
and thz ,stab Ze, with t accordingly
sYllable snowy,
to
alluded
which applies
basic
, attaching
I4kt.rLk
with
leading
versus
sY1labicatio
vowel whose
will
segment
For example
syllabics
simultaneous
which
onset
boundaries
ouk•t •
the first
More strikingly
optionally must
versus
the
syllable
sYllabic
in the full
them .
sno
processes
the
thistable versus 5a •Stelbl
to an adjacent
r ~l
electric
in
eonsYllabics
optionally
from
is
to establishing
to an adjacent
of
ego; o• aspiration,
to dz.s taste
to unstressed
of
tauto
distaste
constrained
unasPirated.
eonsYllabics
in
' iokay
eis t
alter
desYllabification
for
velars
is a process
may be relaxed
applies
, some
at
offset
of
discussion
i f they are
in addition
f CIc • r Prab•a, ~ b {i
this
' •9ou
the
placement
o S ' •theIbl parallel
Syllabication
.
the
~oue,kh
voiceless
echo
I should perhaps
of
versus s
at
occur
comes
English
which
e.g.
VC .V
evidence
in
to
modifications
in
on
examples
it can optionally
the attention
right
dependent
There in
the
phonological
despair,
animate,
V . CV these
to
left.
remaining
Since
the
the
to
probably.
here
of
da] sPE' r
e•k o
aen,a mat
syllabication
morpholog-
syllable
adopt ,
the
constraints
involving
on permissible
terrible
and the
This is the process
s discussed
went.
to its
is instead
to the example at hand
right-attach v• to
is unstressed
left.
and constraints
with
to the syllable
the eonsYllabic
to its
The process
boundaries
right
is stressed
not relevant
Examples
to its
In
the
processes
discussion:
a no nsYllabic
the syllabication
English.)
.9o
It involves
some preliminary
syllable
Middle
so that
The
and flap-deletion.
Syllabication right;
of
of Anderson and others.
example
of which
application
syllabics
n+1
syllabics ri •I
)
-
,".0
58 prior
to attachment.
universally
These intermediate
unpronounceable
such representations reason
fore
given
phonetic
it
that
that
since the
the attachments
in Phonologzeal
, that
optional
out by the same process
that
derivations
nor
they could
basic
Now we are
that
of processes
establishes
is to
occur.
There-
is carried sYllabicatzons
segments
as the attachments
are
evidence
desyllabification
of de syllabified
same principles
is neither
the function
difficulties
seems clear
representations
, and there
ever occur
to believe
eliminate
59
obey precisely
of under)YingIY
In casual
three
released
apical
vowels final
stops
including position.
atorily
saddest,
raer. i $N ~r. r
syllabics,
boar. i sae; ast
at
ratty.
derivation
i s taken
reduction
has
ha rr
elides batting, $i c . r
r
i
thinner,
ooptionally
end of the previous
paragraph.
already
To abbreviate
applied .
because
there
Unstarred
0,
other
1.
syllabication:
2.
flapping:
with
stage
anon;
, the
, t o which vowel
* davC r nat
fnd3
* da ,v'~n.a.ti
f nd 3
da.v'r.a.t~
fnd3
which
. options.
ry
4.
flap-deletion:
har .a s
5.
syllabication:
saer , ast
6,
vowel-nasa
1,
a-harmony:
da.vT~.tI
8.
shortening:
de.vi.
9.
syllabication:
i
da.vt.e.t
i
*da.via.ti
lization:
of
each other
s ubstitutions
da.vrr.e.t
,
i .vi" i. fdn
mark forms which are
obligatory
N
fudge.
e-application
forms are pronounceable
processes
da
the discussion
The asterisks are
like
and r
interwoven
vowel-nasaliz
welded,
in most American idioms .
-initial
the doctor,
bae. ir;
above
3.
is oblig-
,
hearty , 1 w£fr , ad
i s obligatory
in syllable-initial
r
application
divinity
fnd3 fndj fnd3
da.vt.t
i
fnd3
.
in syllable-final
optionally
segment
baer . irbatting
syllable
after
in syllable-
with h-deletion
to be discussed
flaps
flaps
to something
up at an intermediate
have not applied.
, which changes
corresponding
voiced
hothouse
~
is flapping
V > etc)
Examples:
3a.rakt]
oPP1Y to flaps the
haws
process
1•
a following
have optional
The final
to the
no naPical
The process
e.g.
optionally
ci, n
thinner,
mental.
Some speakers
[t,
r
hat
N
mef.1'
to be discussed
Before
voiced: rat
not
process
example , the phrase
the
discussed
processes.
unpronounceable
noesY1lablc
involves
processes
and with other
for the
it can be reduced
derivation
. 23 segments The second
speech
and its the
ready
applications
before
vet o .24
vi.ro
10.
flapping:
is flap-deletion
, which
11.
flap-nasa
position
For example,
12.
flap-deletion:
optionally
13.
syllabication:
14,
vowel-na
saer, ast $i" .r i position
.
saddest,
Flap- deletion , e.gthose
does cited
This the
phrase,
hzation.
salizatzonl '
it
the
not most
,
fnd3 tnd3
da.vrr.
i
f nd3
da.vC7.
i
fnd3 fnd3
da.vi.i
does is
t i
*da .vrt.i
derivation nor
fnd3
*da.vi
i
fnd3
da.vi
i
f nd 3
exh aust extreme
th e pons reducti D
able pronunciations a possible,
but it
of
{t I
60 should
suffice
to illustrate
each other.
They do not apply
and re-apply
whenever
At 1 syllabication at
2 Produces
at 4.
puts
of the
stressed
syllabication this
at any time
t
at 10.
].eke
do not occur ., and it
until
the
resyllabication
tonic
position.
the
the
ante
below to cases
nasality,
this but
flap
post-
removes an obstacle
to
sequential
way that
processes
aPP1Y.
i s imposed on
nonrandom application: of flapping
* da .vC.a.t Flap- deletion performed
o r rapid
example establishes Icelandic
process
re-applies
.
over .
the vowel
This completely of
sPeech.25
that
processes
aPP1Y
example two proces in other
ses apply
forms
b ut
The d vtinz t~ fudge derivation
is possible
applications
it
characteristic
in some forms and in the other
.
There are no fewer
of syllabication
and flap-deletion.
It is this
us to characterize
the application
To understand , shows
to the output
the
re-application
by many casual-speech processes
gesture
that
, as in casual
s not
y occur.
t o vowel-nasalization
is particularly
In Anderson's
in one order
segment
in which the
of vowel- nasalization
of application
iterative)y.
languages
prior
application
Furthermore
deleting
behind which would be extended
substitutions
neither
in other
applied
nasality
processes
shows J
than four
and two each of fl
freedom t o re -apply
that
cf these
processes
should apply
like
apping
enables
as a random
sequence.
In the example there
is only apparent. only the
in immediately
but also
subsequent
distinct
and other
would seem to be the nonoccurring However,
flap-
these
of application.)
were to aPP1Y immediately
it becomes syllable-final
where an ordering
seems to indicate
optionally
more at 13.
which is paralleled
random nonlinear
return
that
at 12 again
involving
of
and other
flap-deletion
optional
application
have flapped
t
Thus if
.
random order
a repre-
with syllable-initial
at 5 Put
only in English
by the
flap
application
of flap-deletion
would leave
can apply ,
The third
to another
until
flapping
[o] , produces
could
arise
could not become syllable-final
a
Flap-deletion
in English
sequence
thing
of
which occurs
derivations
I will
t
"" da.vr~.a.i
deletion,
This example,
Its
is true
aPP1 Y
obstruent
at 5 .
at 9 : leads
but it is not deletable
at 9 (pronunciations
clearly
the relatively
re syllabication
time to
resYllabication
context .
to which flap-deletion
process,
with
they would eliminate
vowel and the unstressed to
interact
order , but rather
into a flapping
by eliminating
vulnerable
flapping, g,
n
a representation
the
sentation
in a linear
the configurations
Flap-deletion,
between
61
the way the processes
classical and
if flap- deletion
before
at 2
between
the result nasality.
does not eliminate tongue
I think
Syllabication
is only one
i , lacking
by the
sufficient
this
term
the phrasing
it
stands
to
processes
the
depend
0n
. and
principle
these,
reason
occurs
stress-assignment but
their
and 'con-sonants+
syllabication
resolving of
to understand
determines
therefore
here
why processes
of
that are
accent
above
suggested , in
turn
derivation
s
functions
in part should
too
it i
.
(to use th
some mutual
syllabication
priority
the
of 'sonants' It depends
(There
syllabication In
respective
this
an stress , be est
averY , for
ablished
dependencies
complex
here
e
to e
seems
xplore
capable
large example,
number every
of
.
63
62 Process is
is
at
least
obligatory
dependent
syllable-finally
nasalization
is
e.
partially
sa r
obligatory
s2
NNN
within
sa 7. r
.
Flap-deletion
and
syllabication:
optional
and
sa~•,nai
occurs only syllable-finally, within syllables,
syllables
thus it occurs at 8 but not after
and shortening
In Chapter
of each of
[31w and
this may be left
for the reader
sufficient
the application
of syllabication
of segmental processes like
that sonority
(syllabic)
condition:
of
segments eliminates
is
impossible
cannot rN ,
be a
flap-deletion
flap-deletion. completely
attached
can
be
in
sequence given
to
it
is
flanked
either.
But
resYllabified.
depends The
natural,
because
the
of the
first
functions
when
Thus
depends and
of
the
less
in
turn
on is
with
processes
to
had applied
to pronounce
two-thirds Instead,
lam lamb
J'ard yard
-
dePalatalization:
zaem
zard
zak
other
jam
zab
zad
zak
processes:
us to understand
of segments. segments
to
of the sounds
is a tYPical
due to resYllabication interplay
example.
of the
of the conditions sYllabications
and inadmissible
which nasalizes
if they are
sYllabications
results
pronunciations:
)
why some processes
Nasalization,
obligatorily
alternative
the alternative
admissible
enables
otherwise,
the
J
ak Jacques
ard
latter
z ,
they applied
m
to nasal
ones.
simultaneously,
-
from [a],
on
changed
to eliminate.
form:
which has the the
This is true
-
optionally
sonorant
the very
sequence:
aPP1Y to strings prior
flap-deletion
re-applications
processes.
for
desYllab-
syllabication,
place,
applications
by
supposed
The same logic
the syllable
* airzn . In da.v&.a.ti
If these
sPirantization:
illustrate
in sequence. simultaneous
creating
are eliminating.
Joan Velten
delateralization:
This
to be pronounceable;
J.
are
adult
by the
are transposed
and must be resYllabified
a
and
airnz] irons '
to
in the natural
depend in turn on flap-deletion.
I
apply
as well as context-conditioned
I, we saw that
the processes
from the margins to the peak
Words like
* ainrz
The point
processes
neural
processes,
in some processes
processes
about
naturally
of individual
she would have ended up having
before they do.
is restricted
if any two nonsyllabics
example, * arinz ication
must increase
of a syllable.
is impossible,
which
applies
from the fact that syllabication
condition
this
himself with.
and this dependency seems
to explain why syllabication
But the re-applications
follows
to divert
However,
function
other
to speculations
why Processes
would result
of context-free
It would be
they depend as they do on syllabication.
specific
configurations
occurs only in interior 14).
have to appeal
to explain
application
•
to show why, given the phonetic functions
they do depend on syllabication,
Given the
optional
sa~N,nai Sinai).
these processes,
is that
organization
vowel-
_a-harmony is near) Y
obligatory
possible
and
otherwise
or
We do not
flapping
otherwise
syllables
signer,
{INN
on
toutos
1labic
In a word like bar.ou.irlor
glide
of
sonorants and
borrowing,
bar.o.wioou , derived
of the nasalization
process
in a complex pattern
of
h
i 64 ,;b' ar,ou. bar.ou.V I
65
i ,
*b ar.o.wiO
irl
* bar.o.wIO
N
bar.ou.N
I
i
I NN bar.ou .~w I N
N N
bar.ou,
N
the
bar.o, N w r~ r l
But this
I
fudge derivation,
applications
are
by the lack
quite
sentation since it
i.e,
to reason
that
the
unaFPlied
The correct
unrestricted prior
of sounds it will
these
forms
application to a nasalized the process
even when the
sequence
due to the application
in derived
this
and of their
on which its
To
application
depends.
to justify
it
doesn't
segment.
despite
And
apical its
could
sequential explain
why
I
simplicity,
this
The constituent
sequence
by their
separate
and they
occur
of the selfsame
I
to
z
in sequence,
the separate
sequenced
iterative
is itself
evidence
given
distinctness
lies
in the assumption
governed
b
which the language
the sound patterns possible
as a simultaneously
that
The explanation
is
should be the case. substitutions
are
learner-the
child-formulates
of the language
he is learning.
to formulate applied
rule,
any sequence the rule
the
predetermined the
reason
is almost
of these
should naturally
But
as is shown
speech as it matures fact
that
the change of
Indeed
the fact
along
simultaneous
formulation
processes,
limitations
z
is incorrect.
can be explained,
the generalized
are
making voicedd into
to [z].
the simultaneous
by distinct they
Jand
processes
rule
and distinct,
from a child's
it
above from Joan
formulation
as is shown by the
I, whereas that
cited
are separate
a change of
Besides
simultaneously
simultaneous
best
of processes
as a single z
disappearance
rrandomly-ordered,
been proposed.
J,
substitutions
entails
formulations
be formulated
they are
substitutions.
of substitutions
continuants
to alter,
representations-
simultaneous
The sequence
speech
should be such that
from sequential
that
nonlinear,
the
substitutions
true
in Chapter
I think
is in principle
always
proposed
one may wonder why this
it
application.
may have to include
the problem:
of languages
application
that
Since
describes
of the sequential
so straightforward
for
merely
explanation
has not previously
to account
isn't
that
To my knowledge
rules
rules
rule
which is commonly cited
as resulting
Velten's
. 26 process
reason
sequential
applicable
other
observation
described
oral
aPP1Y not only to that
but also
application,
of all
sound patterns
to any repre-
exists
representation,
of processes,
conditions
more complex.
of the nasaliza-
in underlying
arises
in which
is pronounceable;
from the iteration
a sonorant
is the sequence
stands
simultaneously
application,
i r~
each representation
of an asterisk.
from its
containing
this
remain
straightforwardly
Lion process,
the
of explaining
N N
ba"r.o. wi
no obligatory
result
N
incapable
I
ba"ri oth
indicated
N
^bar.a.w
I
As in the divinity
be sure, NN
bar.o.wir~
~
~ r~
is inherently
rule
is wrong
with distinct
of the innate
the lines cannot, And
functions
speech
capacity,
aPP1Y in a random iterative
sequence
self-evident.
In sequential
applications
of
A creates
input
hypothesis
is called
a feeding
for
another order
such that
process
the output
B A is said
IiiParskY 1968 .
of one process
to 'feed'
The opposite
B and AB order,
BA
66 however,
is the only case
since
as the derivation
order'
is an iterative
order,
no order
processes which
of divinity sequential
The derivation
of the sort
to A's output.
Therefore
sequential
constraint
shows , so-called
given
It
A introduces the ordering
which prevents
that
applicable
grammatical
does not matter
B is allowed
constraint
or,
B from applying
to apply
better,
aanti-
to A's output
For example, midwest
the vowel
sequences meant
r
,
em of
s r i• n
esses
generally
But the
undergo the other
, r e d nr of
further
raising
:-
applies i
mean
by rule,
or
rules,
sri?eti
to certain
e.g.
mint
change i s It ------i
of
for
s
with the
as evidenced
b"ir ~
constrained
i s not n
difficult -L19'4--
> not
to to
__1...
or aPP1Y establish .
to
shortened
that
various
through
other
output
howaver .-1_.. rl~nlnnfiaefinn
9',
,
do
not
words, of
these
that
the
are
bed
altheagh
fact
learned
Such cases
'slurred
alects
and over':
u
etc.,
[days]
trous(ers),
voiced
stops
e.g. e.g.
bud
spoon , b2n
maz
tomorrow , m
she de voiced gto ttalized high-chair.
?ai~
hei ss
[jai
lie
devo iced
obstruents-
final
shoes -except
as did
z
bottle,
derived buz
vowels ,
final ?as
evidence
voiced
from
Napoleon.
, had been pronounced
But at first
clear
when
remained
b
ba P ;
Similarly
Zee
ezns
she had applied
LeoPold 1947:84-5 , and 1939,
are particularly
, cited constraints
the derivative
initial
a jt a
lamb
pr onounced
from nasals
baz
in this
antisequential
z us
derived
peakers.
The examples
Jbut
you beside
Even generalized
s
constrain
own speech .
who deleted s
constraints
from other
often
For example , Joan Velten
Leopold
in sequence:
bett2n
of these di
antisequential
to their
LeoPold
typical.
at first
[h]:
being
that children
word with de nasalized
apparently
speakers
impede their
which are peculiar
position,
Her first
child
that
from the
liquids
deleted
In the
proc-
in final
b£?n
in words whose semantic
are not necessarily
were derivative:
bEriqbett2n Nr en
they
in children. bead
for
Zett2n'' . be noted
I,
b ~n
speech adult
and Alex androv of children
in Chapter
but
bet him
does not
J , and Hildegand
Hildegand
they
r~" nr
as
when they arise
processes,
6rrn ~
mint
of the tongue.
[Or],Nnr
consonant-deletion to
fact
by slips
N/Y be i q
become
7eti
should
Sully
these
for
r
I:n
is evident
occur
segments, , e.g. ~ se
Thus when words like
redden
various
process
processes.
This process
which arise
processes.
threaten,
a
nasal
does not aPP1Y to the same sequences
other
application
of the southern
Lbefore
This is in accordance
aPP1Y after
process
to
tin.
m"in
serene
from various 8 re ? n
ten
from
met with in language.
common to many speakers
Plus nasal
shortened
is occasionally
is raised
t rn a
from
the
such a constraint
,
way processes can
brim
in casual
character
acquired,
This
I
in the dialect
h m hem
are
for
use such pronunciations
Ian It
be viewed a s unnatural. However,
r
le?n,
AB is
of eliminating
{brim]
Furthermore
occasionally
first.
B's function
etc.
in random
when all
have applied.
one may hear
'feeding
of processes
as A and B applies
It only matters
representations
is justified,
terminates
ones
of processes
to BAB.
fudge
'order'
application
or minus optional
of such a pair
equivalent
in which the term
at all.
Plus
bl
and these
glossary).
for the process
theory
of
phonology. The relaxation
of phonetic
change,
of antisequential discovered
constraints
by KiparskY
1965
is a major source 1968)
.
I have sug-
,
,
68 gested
that
impose
constraints
1969.
such
It
changes found
should
be
context-conditioned only
in
heard of
speech in
of of
comparison
n
(e.g.
f"i
and nasalize they
-'- k~enni
style.
27 There
are
however,
such
speech.
Individual
and as
after
n
-'- ka~ci
of course
quite
identical
whichever
applies
some or all
to which
the other
characterized Kisseberth
tendency
B, A is said
1970
there
might aPP1Y.
by KiParskY
may be natural. whether
argue
to 'bleed'
1968 that
is any Preferred
and it
ordering.
the
degree
does.
fact
these
flap
words
so that
ka~ndi
of ~individual .
between involves
fact
is
provided
striking as
the
in
Chapter
the
the
the
require
that
languages
1971
are
has
in these
two processes
vowel
they
of
in such
than
phonetic
emphasized
irregularities
organization
their
various
the in mor-
are
Similarly,
in
high
vowels
voiced),
of
and
but
it
is
and
between
make the
all
vowels
round were
thus
vowels
in
Sanskrit
e.
nonhigh
with and
labial o•
• and
vowels and
were
palatal
so forth.
nonlabial glides
sequential
would
It
is
this
ordering
many other
syllable principle
from are
vowels
which
be pointless.
nonPalatal
umlauted
the
dif-
distinct
between
and nonPalatal
within
as
respective
some obstruents
were
such
discussed
maximally
ordering
palatalized
changes,
in Fox and
transition
vowels
particularly
processes
their
that
which
processes
minimizing
The opposite
Germanic
context-free
Otherwise,
Thus
and thus
round
nasalization
voiceless
difficult).
early
and vowel
given.
be realized.
voiced less
as
are
opposite
of context-conditioned
distinctiveness
obstruents
is
introduce
functions
functions
of processes
teleologies
which
denasalization
be ordered
not
which
front
natural of
sYntogmatic
voiced
nation
both
as one
order
of view I am doubtful
processes
for
as long
by other
may introduce
The paradigmatic
all
is generally
process
(1968,
of processes
functions--maximizing
superficially
and
study
of
I.
against
the
case
example
potential
of another
into
by in
A eliminates
but Kenstowicz
matter
or natural
may be motivated
KiParskY
should this,
a preferred
relationships
ficulties-would
so that
accomplishes
indeed it
it
s . P28 phologicalgmaradi
sonorants
to see why, if
is
In
configuration
process
I suspect
that
of the configurations
point
there
The insight
ha~nn hand).
order
If
which
in
or Finnish
Bleeding
phonetic
purposes
considerations.
I have found no consistent
regarding
a certain
relationships,
often
speakers
of the context
Phonological
substitutions is difficult
B.
them
detailed
configurations
as unnatural,
bleeding
any
One of these
Where Process
From a strictly
in children's
relationships,
eliminates
of
even
relationships
which aPP1Y to partially
to process
I have discussed.
phonetic
in
largely
processes
first
in
sequence,
than
StamPe
are
marked
(e.g.
potential
eliminate
manifest
they
in Italian
is a matter
many other
for
midwestern
speech
be
variation
processes
input
sort
in casual
case
to
apparent
in unconstrained
kaeni
the
nn
the
apt
is
All
degeminate
in
to
children
learning
adults,
phrases becomes
are
are
casual words
of
that
changes
pronunciations.
-
they
and
aPP1Y these
can
failure
children
funny), d
language
out,
dialects,
casual
the
older
various
similar of
Whether
of
in
the
pointed
fluent
reduction
speakers
in
processes
the
only
originate
69
but •, in
before
many
become
velar all languages nasals areanterior, butbefo ve
but
combi-
in
they that
became is
I
r
71
70 responsible
for
'allophones'
,
There of
the
reason
asking answers
riddled
with
better-formulated
speculation
.
that in
to
whereby
systems.
understand
earliest
to
no
What
of
believe
I have
nature
why
such
to
such
error.
can
we know
to be
a basic
questions,
In
exist
if
fact of
in
question they they
are here
explained
organization
systems
But
show
lead cannot
the
or
all
processes
processes. and
and
REFERENCES Albright, Robert W. , and Joy Buck Albright. 1956. of a two-Year-old child. Word 12.382-90.
that
tried
'phonemes'
between
I.
Fhonological
those
the
exists
Chapter
principles
functions
without
and
discussed
better-evidenced
natural can
is
distinction
as
principles
coherent the
the
is
will to be
of
Anderson Stephen R. and the Ordering M.I.T.
we
Baudouin de CourtenaY, Jan. 1895. Versuch einer Theorie phone tischer A l terna tionen. StrassburE-Cracow. An abridged translation now appears in Edward Stankiewicz, ed., A Baudouin de Courtenag Anthology , Blooming ton: Indiana University Press 1972.
some of
dismissed
the
whether
systems
place.
better
1969. A study in diachronic morPhoPhonemics: Prefixes. Language 45. 807-30.
that
phonological
surely
Andersen Henning. the Ukrainian
into
doubt
first
most,
organized
terms
in I
even
The phonology
be
Our
Bloomfield
confused
answers, as
or
even
Chen
idle
Leonard.
1969. The West Scandinavian Vowel System o Phonological Rules PhD dissertation,
1933.
Language.
New York:
Holt.
Matthew. 1972. Metarules and universal constraints in Phonological theory. Read to the Eleventh International Congress of Linguists, Bologna.
ChomskY, Noam. The Hague:
1964. Current Mouton.
Issues
in Linguistic
Theory.
ChomskY, Noam and Morris Halle. 1968. The Sound Pattern English. New York: HarFer and Row.
o
Darden Bill. 1971. Diachronic evidence for phonemics. Papers from the Sixth Regional Meeting o the Chicago Linguistic Socie 323-31. Edwards Mary Louis. 1970. The acquisition thesis, Ohio State University.
of liquids.
M.A.
Edwardss Mary Louise. 1971. One child's acquisition of English liquids. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 3, Stanford University Committee on Linguistics. Firth
J.R. 1935. The use and description of certain English sounds. Papers in Linguistics 1934-1961. London: Oxford University Press 1957.
Fromkin Victoria utterances. Grammont Maurice. Delagrave.
A. 1971. The non-anomalous Language 46.27-52. 1950.
Traite
de Phonetique.
nature
of anomalous
Paris:
Librairie
,
73
72 Grge zoire , Antoine. 1937. L'ApPrent2ssag thae ue de la Faculto ' de Ph2'losohie p de Li9'e e 73 Halle
Morris.
1954.
The strategy
Hallef
Morris.
1959.
Sound Pattern
Halle
Morris.
1962•
Phonology
Harris,
Ze11ig. University
e du Lana9e. 9 Bibl 'oet Lettres de l'Universito
of phonemics. o
Russian.
in generative
1951. Methods in Structural of Chicago Press.
The Hague: Mouton. grammar.
Word 18.54-72.
Linguistics.
Chicago: Preliminaries
Jakobson Roman and Morris The Hague: Mouton.
o
Jakobson
Roman.
1962.
Selected
1956. Writings
Fundamentals I.
Language.
Mouton: The Hague.
Jakobson Roman. 1968 Child Language, Aphasia, and Phonolo9ical Universals. Tr. A. Keiler. The Hague: Mouton. The 1941 German text is reprinted in Jakobson 1962. Hockett Charles F. 1955. A Manual o Phonolo9y. Memoir 11 of the International Journal o American Linguistics. JesPersen,
Otto.,
1964.
Language.
New York: Norton.
Kenstowicz Michael and Charles Kisseberth. 1970. Unmarked bleeding orders. Papers from the Sixth Regional Meeting o the Chicago Linguistic Society, 504-19 KiParskY, Paul. M.I.T.
1965.
Phonolog2 'cal
Change.
Ph.D. dissertation
KiParskY, Paul. 1968. Linguistic universals and linguistic change, Emmon Bach and Robert T. Harms edd., Universals in Linguistic Theory, New York: Halt Rinehart and Winston 170-202. KiParskY: Paul. wall, ed. versity of Kuroda
1911. A Survey Maryland
Sige-Yuki.
1961.
Historical linguistics. o Linguistic Science Press 577-649. Yawelmani
Phonology.
William College
0. DingPark: Uni-
F. 4 vols. 1 1939;
and the description
Cambridge:
1939-49. Speech Development o a Bilingual Evanston: Northwestern University Press. vol. 2 1947.
Writings Press.
of language.
o Edard
Component o
Sa ir. a Grammar
Miller
Patricia Donegan, 1972a. Some context-free processes affecting vowels. Working Papers in Linguistics 11, Ohio State University Department of Linguistics 136-67
Miller
Patricia Donegan. 1972b. Vowel neutralization vowel reduction. Papers from the Eighth Regional o the Chicago Linguistic Socie 482-9.
Ohso
Mieko. 1971. A Phonological study of some English loan words in Japanese. M.A. thesis, Ohio State University.
PassY, Paul. 1890. Paris: Librairie Read
Charles. phonology.
and Meetin C
tude sur les Changements Phonetiaues. Firmin-Didot.
1971. Pre-school children's knowledge of English Harvard Educational Review 41.1-34.
Rhodes Richard. 1972. Natural phonology and MS conditions. Papers from the Eighth Regional Meeting~' of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 544-57. SaPit,
Edward.
1921.
Language.
view York: Harcourt
Brace.
SaPit,
Edward. 1925. Sound patterns in language. 51. Reprinted in Mandelbaum 1949.
SaPit,
Edward. 1933. The psychological reality of phonemes. First Published in English in Mandelbaum 1949.
SaPtr,
Edward Quarterly
Language 1.37-
1915. Notes on Judeo-German phonology. Jewish Review n s VI, 231-6, Reprinted in Mandelbaum 1949.
Saussure Ferdinand. 1959 A Course in General York: Philosophical Library.
Linguistics.
New
C. 1972. A cross-language study of vowel M.A, thesis Ohio State University.
M.I.T.
Semiloff- Zelasko Holly. 1972. Modern Hebrew fast speech. Werner
Child. Vol.
Phonetics
McCawleY: James D. 1968. The PhonoloCical o Japanese. Mouton: The Hague.
SchouruP, Lawrence nasalization.
Press. LeoPold,
1972.
Mandelbaum D. G, ed. 1949. Selected Berkeley: University of California
Word 10.
Jakobson Roman Gunnar Fant and Morris Halle. 1951. to Speech Analysis. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press. Ha11e.
Lindblom, BJ orn. Unpublished.
Vowel reduction and loss in M.A. thesis Ohio State University.
74 Smith,
Neil V. 1970. Unpublished.
The acquisition
Stampe, David. 1968. Yes to the Fourth Regional Society.
of phonology:
a case study.
Virginia,... Unpublished paper read Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic
Stampe, David 1972 On the natural history of diphthongs. Papers from the Eighth Regional Meeting o the Chicago Linguistic Society, 578-90. David.
To CHAPTERI: 1
Stampe, David. 1969. The acquisition of phonetic representation. Papers from the Fifth Regional Meeting o the Chicago Linguistic Society, 443-54.
StainPa
AFTERTHOUGHTS
Forthcoming.
Natural
phonology.
_
P age 3)
Perhaps
Hildegard's
"whispered"
pronunciation
of ,pretty
was not exceptional after all. It may simply have been the regular reflex of an early process devoicing high vowels in syllables with voiceless consonant rather as in Portuguese, Japanese, etc. When she later learned to voice the vowels, the consonants would have assimilated to them in voicing, regularly, and if the r LeoPold transcribed in the whispered pronunciation was, as I suspect, a natural misperception on his part of the stop release it would have seemed to disappear in the voiced pronunciation. This example has received much attention perhaps undeserved in recent literature. 2
Stanley, Richard. 43.393-436. Trubetzko satze.
1961.
Redundancy rules
in phonology.
Language
N. S. 1936. Die Aufhebun der honolo ischen Ce enTravaux du Cercie Linguistique de Prague 6.29-45.
Page 16 This is incomplete. Since t might become c via either d or 8 the entailment is rather that either d or 8 or both would also become . D0ne8an (1978) has discussed this sateSorial property of. Fhonological derivations in more detail and with far richer illustrations. 3P
TrubetzkoY, N. S. 1969. Berkeley: University
Principles o of California
Phonology . Press.
Tr. C. Baltaxe.
V
Twaddel 1, W. F. 1934. On defining the phoneme. Language M nographs. Reprinted in Martin Joosed., Re a1ings in Linguistics, Chicago : University of Chicago Press, 1957, 55-79. Twaddell, W. F. 1938. A note on Old High German umlaut Monateheto fur deutsehen Unterricht 30.177-81. Reprinted in Martin Joos, ad., Readings in Linguistics, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1957, 85-7. Velten
Harry in infant
V. 1943. language.
age
The growth of phonemic and lexical Language 19.281-92.
patterns
18 --
I am alluding anasal
to the
1905. Press.
English
Dialect
Grammar.
Oxford:
Oxford
formulation
anasal
Usually, opposite changes reflect distinct Processes with distinct teleolo8ies. As such, the application of one in a language never entails the application of the other. There are languages with context-free denasalization but no contextual nasalization (Japanese, in some descriptions) , and languages with contextual nasalization but no context free denasalization Hindi Changes due to a single process, on the other hand are normally in a relation of unilateral entailment. As noted on page 8, if i is deAalatalized to I.e if it occurs will also be dePalatalized to n under like circumstances. See Done an 1918 for numerous illustrations. 4
Wr ight, Joseph. University
popular
Page 19 Thus languages which aAP1Y vowel denasalization lack nasal vowel phonemes, and also nasal vowel allophones (as in Japanese, in some descriptions) unless as in English) assimilative vowel nasalization also applies. On the other hand, languages which suppress vowel denasalization have nasal vowel phonemes; such languages may also suppress assimilative vowel nasalization as French does, compare bon bonne , or they may aPP1Y it as Hindi does and have some nonnasal as well as nasal vowel phonemes realized as nasal vowel allophones. For every Pair of contrary processes of this sort there are just four possible Phonological typologies (assuming that if the context-free process applies it must aPP1Y before the contextsensitive process.) 75
76
77
5(
page 19) Indeed it can be doubted whether nonadJ' acent ever play a role in p honoloical bfeatures b pro cesses. Since it is y now well established that nondistinctive features often condition changes in distinctive features the way is op en to interpret ~ actions at a distance as involving auditoralY unobtrusive overlaPPings off harmonized vowel features on intervening consonants h , and likewise of armonized consonant features (e .gretroflexion in Sanskrit) on intervening vowels. This would explain why actions at a distance entail the corresponding actions in adjacent segments , as e. Old Hi gh German palatal umlaut as in manna > menu(i) 'men' entailed stain > stein 'stone'; we would posit an 1-colored in manna . This is a recurrent hypothesis in historical phonology , it has g be en given up too readily in the face of surprising examples . I see no reason why even consonant harmonies like Pibi t .v. or Qa9i ] do ie in child speech could not be explained in this way; it is the timbre not the stoPPedness , of the consonants which would be posited on the intervening vowels, and I see no aAriori reason why the adjustments of lip and tongue musculature accompanying obstruents might not co-occur, uunobtrusively to the ear , with the rather different adjustments accompanying vowels . 6 Page 21) A simpler hypothesis regarding hub home beside ham ham would be that Joan pronounced hu . as ~u as e. g. in J apanese since ~] would not be nasalized and would therefore cause progressive denasalization. Ve1ten' s phonemic analysis does not give ' phonetic detail , and the notes on which it was based seem n o longer to exist.
case ee
they differ phonetically (which is dubious in the or ee or occur in distinct prosodic constituents
case
..
geminate
consonants
on either
side of a syllable
of e.g.
division}.
Donegan 1978 shows clearly that monoPhthongization is not a separate process but simply the end product of the mutual assimilation of the features of a diphthong. This is in accordance with the natural segmentation hypothesis sketched in note 7. 10
Page 25) The aPPlicational Precedence of context-free processes before contrary context-sensitive processes seems to be a special case of a much more general constraint Donegan and I 1979 have presented as "Fortition first lenition last". 11 { Page 25 Some further discussion and evidence has now been published in section 4 of Donegan and StamPe 1919. 12 P age 26) The re-analysis of the place of denasalization in Joan's speech proposed above in note 7 is consistent with an absolute interpretation of this constraint. Done8an and I 1919: section 3) have argued for such an interpretation, extended from pairs of contrary Processes to all paradigmatic and sYntagmatic processes °'Fortitions first lenitions last") . For example, a fortition like the vowel aPenthesis in boated batted could not succeed a lenition like the voice assimilation in knst kissed and thus * bntat could not be derived (through these processes) . This constraint is extremely restrictive, barring analyses of a wide variety, including
1
Pa ge 21 The application of context-free vowel nasalization as 6 after context-sensitive nasahzation 1 is a counterexample to the precedence constraint proposed between these types of processes on page 23f. I suspect that Joan's actual pronunciations were as in 5 rather than as in 6 • v.elten would simply have ignored vowel nasalzzation where it was obviously contextually conditioned as in 5 Joan did denasalize vowels , as in na_ for French non (specifically remarked) and a is for nets ants , but there is no reason this could not have b een prior to vowel nasalization at 1 This eliminates the counterexample. Page 22 Left out of this account are processes of PaZatalization the opposite of DePalatalization , Tensing, and its opposite, Laxing. Diphthongiza tion can be eliminated being simply an application of one of the other processes to half of a vowel . The vowel research of Donegan (formerly Miller is brought together in Donegan 1978. The asegmen tai hypothesis of Donegan and StamPa 1918 and StamPe 1979, according to which processes have only prosodic domains e .g. syllables, or syllable constituents; measures or measure constituents etc. , and never segmental ones carries with it a natural segmentation hypothesis that adjacent phonetic segments are distinct only in
? those KiParskY sought to elim in
paper of 1968, and explaining why alternations of the divine divinity type where historically, the sYntagmatic shortening of the stressed vowel in triplet words preceded the paradigmatic vowel shift and diPhthongization cannot, despite ChomskY and Halle 1968,be part of the synchronic natural phonology of a language. For further discussion see the notes on 'bleeding order' under Chapter II. 13
Page 21 The characterization of the difference between phonemes and allophones in structural phonology in terms of criteria like 'contrast' 'comPlementa ~' distribution' , 'phonetic similarity', and so forth has been effectively criticized by ChomskY 1964, Postal 1968, and earlier by Bazell 1954. No such criteria figure in the present characterization. In natural phonology, a sound is a phoneme if and only if not all its occurrences can be derived naturally i.e. through natural interactions of active natural processes of the language) from other sounds. Thus a language learner will take the nasal vowel of a form mi to derive from a nonnasal vowel by Progressive nasalization from /m/, or regressive nasalization from a deleted following nasal rather than learn to produce the vowel deliberate)Y , as nasal. But if the language presents him with forms like [pa], which entails the suppression of progressive nasalization; or en which entails the suppression of regressive nasalization; o r even en which entails the suppression of a process deleting a
7s
79
final nasal--so that there remains no natural derivation of the vowel of [ml] from any source but /1/, then the learner can only Perceive this vowel as deliberately (phonemically) nasal and must learn to pronounce it as such. This criterion of natural derivability obviates reference not only to the structuralist criteria but also to the increasingly ad hoc and complex criteria of generative phonology ZwickY 1972 lists ~ about thirty of these). It furnishes an irreducible account of the acquisition of phonology, in which often a single form will suffice to show the child that a certain sound is phonemic in the language he is learning, • compare the zeka example on page aye 29 (However, if it makes matters simple for the child it does not do so for the linguist, since to aPP1Y the criterion of natural derivability requires the linguist to understand fully the processes and derivations in question. At the same time the criterion seems to me to capture the sort of phonemic representation that the structuralists aimed at but which their criteria failed to characterize adequately. It allows for example, for desirable non-linear mappings like kit with ikmnt can't non-invariant mappings like the flap of 8rithree with In / and the flap . of Eci rEdd.~. with Id!, and so forth (compare ChomskY 1964 . And it quite naturally rules outmaPPingswhichthe structuralists could rule out only with vague or ad hoc criteria like the identification o f the complementary h and ~ of English) since there is no natural process which makes initial /n! into h or final /h into rI (compare Bazell 1954). 14
P age 28)
See footnote
14 of the preceding
paper.
15P
age 31 I am assuming that, in the absence of auxiliary hypotheses, obstruents represented as unspecified as to voicing would surface, once the process that previously governed their surface voice specification was suppressed, as randomly voiced or voiceless. This assumption seems to surprise adherents of the archisegmental theory, I suspect because they did not consider it to have any empirical consequences. 16 Page 32 This example may be a bad one because it is difficult to show that all qs are naturally derivable from /n! Plus velar in current English, in the face of examples like singer versus linger, ._ and because tests designed to get at the psychological ~ situation give conflicting results in this case. But it is a clear example, and the facts clearly held true in earlier English, and they are commonplace in many languages. Or one can substitute the dental of plinth for throughout the discussion. Some writers on natural phonology have presented it as having all neutralizing processes aPP1Y prior to all non-neutralizing Processes. But the reverse situation is quite commonplace. For example, the nonneutralizing devoicing .~ of final vowels in Yana women's speech fed a neutralization of preceding voiced stops with voiceless ones SaPit
1929)•
the
optional,
non-neutralizing
monoPhthong'zation ~.
of Kabardian
/9w/to o feeds an obligatory neutralization of preceding nonlabialized stops with labialized ones KuiP ers 1960); ~ and in English non-neutralizing vowel deletion as in poatsrl panting feeds nonneutralizing neutralizing flapping Paecl' and in turn a neutralizing progressive ~ nasalization so that panting = .pBf' ~ = Pannign . Examples like this show that the phonemic representation of an utterance does not necessarily occur in its derivation from its lexical representation. Thus in /Pant iq/ ' /PatT / -' pHei i .poari , the phonemic representation of panting which like that of panning, would be !aen i / ) does not occur. But it may be noted that panting and annin have identical "rhyme fields" (planning , scanning, chanting, etc.), are often spelled alike by children who are unfamiliar with the morphophonemic conventions of English spelling, etc. , and in fact would be represented alike in the memory of any speaker who did not happen to know from ant, ants etc.) that panting has a t in it". It may be helpful to think of phonemic representation not as a level but as the most "concrete" (superficial) representation of a form which is accessible to the phonetically unsophisticated speaker's awareness. This of course follows SaPir's characterization of the phoneme as a PercePt,) Then it must follow that allophones can find no place in lexical representation: what is not perceived cannot be recorded in memory. 17 Page 35) As this shows, the line of argument that Ha11e followed with his Russian example above demonstrating that the phonemic level does not correspond to a natural break in a linearordered set of "rules" applies equally to the morphophonemic level. What is refuted is not the phoneme, but Halle's conception of the structure of a phonology. 18
Page 37 HooPer 1975 has attempted to refute this argument by citing the tradition in some Celtic languages of writing sP etc. with sb etc. and noting that Gaelic beach 'bee' is augmented to sbeach 'wasp'. I.do not understand the difference between English and most other languages and Celtic in this respect but the fact remains that in all languages there is a specific identification of the stop afters with a particular value of the (neutralized) feature. Whether every language makes the same identification is not at issue. 19 20
Page 39
. Updated in her dissertation
Donegan 1978).
Page 42 It should be pointed out that Joseph Greenberg, in his outstanding work on universals of language, has increasingly turned to processes to explain synchronic systems, and likewise Alan Bell Larry HYman, and others in Greenberg's tradition. But Greenberg's exclusively diachronic conception of processes seems to ignore the existence of the teleologies underlying change in the synchronic system of a language. It is typical in language change that, before the old gives ~ way to the new, they co-exist as regional,
80
81
social, or stylistic variants. In variants of a single sPBaker , such as those cited in Chapter II for the phrase divinity fudge _~ , we find a continuum ranged from the "clearest" to the "most pronoun ceable" forms. This is typical of variants over synt agmatic processes; variants over paradigmatic processes, such as the following forms of head observed by Donegan and me in the speech of natives of the Great Smoky Mountains are ranged in the opposite order: hed -i head head head -• he aad [haled] . Here the „ later forms are the "clearest limited in most older speakers just to emphatic speech, though children often use the least conservative forms and this is true both of .paradigmatic and sYntagmatcc derivatives ) in their everyday speech. 21 ( page 47. Further differences between rules and processes may be illustrated by the rule adjusting the pronunciation of the latina to prefix inim- It:-, etc) versus the process assimilating alveolar stops including nasals to the point of articulation of a following stop. I The rule is limited to a specific morpheme. The process applies regardless of morphological identities . 2) The rule is obligatory (*polite , * ~.nrelevant ,the process i s obligatory only wit hen a stress-measure *'un *u' n cle and n wider domain s is optional and deP endentt on style careful i nl ut versus casual i m ut . 3 The rule applies before any Processes and before the point in speech processing when tongue-slips occur. The process applies afterward. Cf.. Page 65. 4) The rule refers only to phonemes, never to allophones . It does not change _in- before velars (incompetent) as it does before labials (impossible) because ~ is or was in Latin an allophone while m/ is (and was in Latin a Phoneme . The In in incompetent optionally becomes q but it is the process that brings this about ,
as it optionally changes n to rm in in Paris. The process to features, and operates on completely novel combinations of e.g. in borrowed words or phonetic exercises like at' obala aaIa ) as well as native phonemes. 5) The rule is insensitive to prosodic structure e.8to stress placement im ~.ous, im ious}. The process operates on prosodic rather than segmental domains, as in ka~nt ,9o kaa k.9o (*ka~nk.9o) can't o •iDd.rnt .9o u9. k.9o f Od.nk9 . o ud) k9. o) should n g o,, where strings of alveo a r stops within a stress-measure are assimilated entirely or not at all, as if the strings constituted a single long alveolar stop segment. See Done8an and Stampe 1978. 6) Historically, the rule was borrowed with the morphemes it applies to, and did not extend to new morphemes. The process, a native constraint, affects all borrowed morphemes. 7 Systematic phonemic representations Chomsky 1964), i.e. representations whose derivations require the agency of rules as well as processes, fail every test I know that might establish their psychological reality. This is in striking contrast to phonemic and morphophonemic rep resentations whose derivations require only processes. For example, two words rhyme if they match in phonemic representation
from their stressed syllabics to the end of the word e .g. lens . bends readin : meeting, mix: sixths if pronounced /s~ks/ step _ ), etc. -Morphophonemic identity is not leapt if pronounced / I eP sufficient without phonemic identity: banned ,hand rhyme only if the latter is not pronounced /hmn etc . But morphophonemic identity is preferred: rhymes like readin :needin are referred over reading meeting, and are more frequent. Systematic phonemic identity , however, is totally irrelevant: rhymes like line , • signn (cf. signal) , revision : division (cf. revise divide), cram damn cf. damnation) are not perceived as differing from those with matched systematic phonemic representations. This suggests strongly that rules do not operate on representations in the way that processes do and casts doubt on the appropriateness of a Process model" for rules. The rhyme test from my unpublished paper on the phoneme, 'Yes Virginia ...' 1968 ,also shows that phonetic identity is immaterial. Words with phonemically Identical rhymes rhyme perfectly even if they are pronounced differently , e.g.. if mat and cat are pronounced may and kae' . But words without phonemically identical rhymes do not rhyme even if they are pronounced exactly alike, e. g. if mat and pad are both pronounced as aec] in the verse. Upon a mat Upon a Pad A Yellow cat Serenely sat.
This would follow, of course if allophonic properties of sounds not perceived.) For further discussion of the rule/process distinction see Donegan and Stampe 1979 sections 2, 4).
are
22
exactly refers these akp
,
P'age 55 The unordered sequential iterative hypothesis of process application presented in this dissertation is basically identical to the one I proposed, on weaker evidence, in notes 1 12, and 14 of the precedingpaper. If Anderson's dissertation of the same year is intended to allow iterative ABA application, which is not clear to me then his hypothesis of 'local ordering' was identical to mine except in its preference for counterbleeding over bleeding order after KiParskY 1968. For a review of some of the subsequent developments in ordering ~' theory, together with a revised hypothesis of ordering in natural phonology, see Donegan ~ and Stampe 1979 section 3). 23
Aage 57 For what I hope understanding of the syllable, see and I have stuck by the notation boundaries .) rather than, as has them as trees because we know of cannot universally be deduced from of the constituent segments.
represents some progress in m Y Donegan and Stampe 1978. Donegan of syllables simply by marking their become fashionable, representing no evidence that their structure the relative order and sonority
V
82
83 i
One idea in the present work that still seems to me worth pursuing is the conception of syllable-division, re-syllabication, and desyllabication as a single integrated process.
derivative representations (excepting, as noted rule-derived ones), or they apply to all of them. Numerous additional examples are given in Donegan and StamPe 1979: section 3. Further consequences of the simultaneous iterative interpretation are presented in note 28 after the discussion of feeding and counterfeeding application.
24
page 57 It is the prosodic domain of flapping that accounts for the main differences between speakers. Those who flap in the offset of syllables flapa only the first t of entity (ent.i.ty): those who flap in the offset of stress-measures a group of syllables beginning ~ with a stressed syllable and ending before another stressed syllable) flap both _t's: and so on up to t hose who flap apical stops at least if they are voiced) even in the onset off stressed syllables as in the doctor, or today.
28
,
25
pa a 60) The relative independence of laryngeal, nasal position and color features has not been given sufficient attention. But although I do not think this explanation of the "counterbleeding" relation of vowel nasalization and (nasal) flap deletion is wrong,~ another more general explanation follows from the proposal that processes apply simultaneously Donegan and StamPe 1979: see note 27 below). In this case da.v'r.a~ ti would change in one step to de.vl.e.ti so that deletion could not bleed nasalization. 26
_
,
Page 64 On an asegmental or prosodic view of Phonological processing Donegan h and StamPa 1978, StamPe 1979), a process nasalizing sonorants--insofar as it does not distinguish among kinds of sonorants--would read the long string of sonorants in borrowing as a single sonorant and would nasalize the whole string in one step. The length of the string nasalized would depend on the length of the string input and the rosodic domain of the nasalization process. Of the pronunciations cited, the first admissible one in each column corresponds to a syllable, and the last to a stress-measure. The intermediate pronunciations correspond to two syllables, or all the posttonic syllables, or some such domain which I doubt is a natural constituent of the prosodic structure of this word. I suspect that mY previous inability to rule out the intermediate pronunciation led me to consider it admissible it seems obvious in either event that my judgment is not to be trusted here. Compare also note 21 part 5.) 27 Page 68 The entire discussion of sequential application of processes here is subject to a quite different interpretation. Feeding would result not only from sequential iterative application, but also from simultaneous iterative application, where all processes apply at once, and then all re-apply, and so forth. On this view, a counterfeeding constraint would he not a constraint on the ordering of a pair of rocesses that one process may not follow the other), but as a no-iteration constraint that one may not reapply) . This has the interesting consequence that no process could be both fed and counterfed. Note that this is precisely the situation= hitherto unexplained, described in the text with regard to the process raising E before nasals P. 66f and the process devoicing final obstruents in Joan Velten`s speech P. 67 : these processes either aPP1Y to no
o
Page 69 Since this was written most of the diachronic examples that seemed to involve a change from bleeding to counterbleeding application have been discredited (by Koutsoudas et al., Vennemannt and others). The inescapable conclusion seems to be that bleeding and counterbleeding application are not language-specific options. But the theory I Presented in the text provides no way of predicting when one or the other will occur, and the available hypotheses seem in one way wa or another incompatible with natural phonology. For example, 's "opacity" Yhypothesis 1971 KiParsk Y Presupposes that all h processes are learned. The simultaneous iterative hypothesis presented in note 27 Predicts that, in the absence of universal priority constraints, no process could bleed another. As pointed out in note 25, this is often the desired prediction. But Kisseherth and Kenstowicz's numerous examples of bleeding order require some restrictions of priority. We have in natural phonology two universal priority constraints which are well-evidenced: rules before processes, and fortitions before lenidons note 12 Applied to the simultaneous hypothesis, the latter constraint predicts that a fortition will bleed any lenition it can see the example of batted in note 12) . This most frequently occurs when the forte~.'t'on separates an assimilating and an assimilated seg"bleedinQ the context" of the assimilation , as Kisseberth thus"bleeding thement, context" of theassimilation as Kisseb and Kenstowicz put it P. 68 above).
I
i 84
ADDITIONAL
Bazell:
C. E. A. Martinet 10 PP.
1954. and 6-15.
Donegan, Patricia Ohio State Donegan>
The choice of criteria in structural linguistics. U. Weinreich3 edd. ! Li uistics Today = Word New York: Linguistic Circle of New York.
Jane. University
Patricia
Jane
Phonological HooPer edd. North-Holland.
REFERENCES
1978. On the Natural Working Papers in and
David
and prosodic Sy llables
StamPe.
1978.
structure. and Segments
,
Donegan Patricia Jane and David StamPe. natural phonology. Daniel A. Dinnsen to Phonological Theory! PP. 126-173. University Press. Hooper,
Joan
B.
1975.
phonology.
The
Language
archisegment
Phonology Linguistics, The
Alan Bell pp. 25-34.
syllable
in
and Joan BYbee Amsterdam:
1979. The ed., Current Bloomington:
in
o Vowels. No. 23.
natural
study of Approaches Indiana
generative
51:536-560.
KiParskY! Paul. 1968. How abstract is phonology? Published 1973 in 0. Fu'imura ed. Three Dimensions o Linguistic Theory. Tokyo: TEC. Kou.tsoudasa
Andreas,
Phonological
et rules.
KuiPers,
Aert. 1960. Hague: Mouton.
Postal
Paul HarFer
SaPit, In StainPa,
M. and
1968. Row.
al.
1974.
and
Aspects
o
Morpheme
Theo.
ZwickY! Arnold W. Dr essler 151-168.
1974. grammar. Linguistics, University M.
Phonological R
Shu pp. Press.
in
Phonolo9ical
Edward. 1929. Male and female Mandelbaum ed. 1949:206-212.
generative Tomorrow's Georgetown
application
of
50:1-28.
Phoneme
David. 1979. Domains. Texas Austin March 1979.
Venneman
On the
Language
Talk
of
at
J.
New York:
speech
the
concreteness and C. 202-219.
The
Theorij.
forms
given
Kabardian.
Yana.
University
in
BaileY, Washington,
1912. The strategy of generative and F. V. Mares edd.! Phonolo 9 ica Munich: Wilhelm Fink Verlag, 1975.
in
of
natural edd.,
Toward D.C.:
phonology. 1972, , pp.